• No results found

Classification of traffic safety campaigns : A taxonomy

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "Classification of traffic safety campaigns : A taxonomy"

Copied!
52
0
0

Loading.... (view fulltext now)

Full text

(1)

l rapport

Nr 394A - 1994

Classification of Traffic Safety

Campaigns - a taxonomy

Stina Jarmark

SwedishRoadand

ITransportResearchInstitute

(2)
(3)

VTI rapport

Nr 394A 0 1994

Classification of Traffic Safety

Campaigns - a taxonomy

Stina Jéirmark

Swedish Road and

' Transport Research Institute

(4)
(5)

Publisher: Publication:

VTI Report 394A

Published: Project code:

Swedish Roadand 1995 20047

ITransportResearch Institute

S-581 95 Linkoping Sweden Project:

A taxonomy on information campaigns Printed in English 1995

Author: Sponsor:

Stina Jarmark Swedish National Road Administration

National Society for Road Safety

Title:

Classi cation of Traf c Safety Campaigns - A taxonomy

Abstract (background, aims, methods, results) max 200 words:

The report consists of two parts: a literature survey and a theoretical model. By studying various campaigns, 'I hoped to find some common features for their implementation, but it was hard to nd a pattern. It was also difficult to comment on the effects of information. It is, however, possible to distinguish a common feature. Several studies are also poor in many respects and above all, they were carried out in such a way that no conclusions can be drawn.

The successful campaigns were extremely well planned. The campaigns where the'message was well in accordance with the road users own experience also had an effect. By studying the literature on typologies, it would be possible to classify road safety campaigns in a more systematic way.

Typologies are the basis of science. Taxonomies can be regarded as more advanced typologies. A typology can be described as a way of classifying data. The main aim is to classify and create homo geneous groups. There are three demands. A typology must be:

1 . distinct

2. comprehensive 3. fruitful.

My taxonomy concerning road safety campaigns consists of several levels. At the first level, the main results are presented with aim and target group. The next level shows the medium used and the possi-bility of combining it with some other type of in uence (reinforcement). The next level offersinforma-tion on the length of the campaigns and whether they were presented in a neutral or in uential/biased way.

The taxonomy is the first step towards a useful tool. The next step will be to fill in thelocations in the taxonomy with empirical data. This is a process of interaction in order to better understand the effects to be expected from road safety campaigns.

Keywords: (All of these terms are from the IRRD Thesaurus except those marked with an *.)

Traffic Safety Campaigns, Effects, Typology, Taxonomy, Classificaton

ISSN: Language: No. of pages:

(6)
(7)

PREFACE

This project was financed by the Swedish National Road Administration (SNRA) and the National Society for Road Safety (NTF). This is the first report of a long-term project aiming at a better understanding of the effects of road safety

information.

The primary aim of the project was to produce a classification system or a taxo-nomy for road safety information. Roger Johansson, my colleague at that time and probably an even greater visionary than I am, was convinced of the realism in

producing such a taxonomy. I was a bit sceptical, more especially as it was my

task to produce this taxonomy.

Inger Linderholm of the Department of Media and Communication Science in Lund, came to my rescue. Thanks to her, I learnt about the course in typologies and taxonomies in Lund. And thanks to inspiring discussions with Professor Karl Erik Rosengren and a number of students, I managed to grasp the subject I had embarked upon. This report is the result of my efforts and my many long journeys to Lund.

The report draft was discussed at two seminars at the institution mentioned above. Thanks to Christina Ruthger s admirable work this report was translated to English.

(8)
(9)

CONTENTS 1.1 1.2 1.3 1.3.1 1.3.2 1.3.3 1.3.4 1.3.5 1.3.6 1.3.7 2.1 2.2 2.3 3.1 3.2 3.2.1 3.2.1.1 3.2.2 3.2.2.1 3.2.2.2 3.2.3 3.2.4 3.3 3.3.1 SUMMARY BACKGROUND Introduction Issues

A brief speci cation of the literature Alcohol, drugs and driving

Children and traffic

Unprotected and exposed/vulnerable road users Seat belts and restraint systems

Road safety - Miscellaneous Practical-theoretical approaches Summary and aim

TYPES, TYPOLOGIES AND TAXONOMIES What is a typology?

Typological functions and attribute space

Monothetic and polythetic typologies, respectively A TAXONOMY OF ROAD SAFETY CAMPAIGNS Introduction

Input variables of the taxonomy Source and Message

Reinforcement Medium

Method of presentation Durability

Trget group

Aim and Results

Description of the taxonomy

Practical application of the taxonomy REFERENCES Page \ D Q L J I -P A U J N N h t r t p -d

(10)
(11)

Classi cation of Traf c Safety Campaigns - A taxonomy by Stina J armark

Swedish Road and Transport Research Institute, VTI

8 581 91 LINKOPING Sweden

SUMMARY

The report consists of two parts: a literature survey of information campaigns and their effects on road safety and a theoretical model based on the results from the literature survey.

By studying various campaigns, I expected to be able to find some common fea tures, e. g. how campaigns on children and traffic had been carried out. It is, how-ever, dif cult to find any patterns in my empirical study, a fact also confirmed by other studies. Another observation, also in accordance with other attempts to summarise road safety information, is the difficulty of commenting on the effects

of information in road safety work. A joint feature can, however, be distinguished.

Several studies show that the campaigns described in literature are often poor in several respects. They have been carried out in such a way that it is hardly possible to draw any conclusions. The descriptions of campaigns are also de cient. The aim of the campaigns and the methods used in the realisation are sometimes missing or are often very scantily described.

To a great extent, the literature on road safety campaigns differs in quality and comprises several different subject fields. As the aim of my study was to find a pattern for the realisation of campaigns, I started by classifying them according to

subject field. The most common were campaigns on safety belts, children in

tra ic, alcohol and tra lc and unprotected road users. A group with road safety miscellaneous, contained factors such as speed and night-time traffic. Another group of studies had a more general approach. These studies were brought together under the heading practical-theoretical approach, as they did not treat any specific campaign but had more theoretical assumptions.

The successful campaigns were extremely well-planned as regards the target group and its habits. Another in uential factor was whether the personnel had any knowledge about campaign activities and research. Campaigns where the message was well in accordance with the road users own experience, i.e. having to pay a

(12)

fine for driving too fast, also had an effect. However, when police supervision

ceases, road users also cease to follow the rules.

The aim of the second part of the report was to classify road safety campaigns in a more systematic and distinct way through literature on typologies, thus nding a pattern of making them more effective.

Typologies are the basis of science. In the 20th century, scientific research is usually based on typologies and taxonomies. The terms typology and taxonomy are often used synonymously, but taxonomies are sometimes described as more 1 advanced typologies. A typology can be described as a way of classifying data, often in several dimensions. The main aim of a typology is to classify and create as homogeneous groups as possible. There are three demands. The classification must be:

1 . distinct

2. comprehensive 3. fruitful.

The risk of having too detailed a typology withtoo many groups must be balanced against one with too few groups where the variables differ too much. A possible solution is to make a classification in several steps and start from just a few

comprehensive categories, which are then broken down into more detailed

cate-gories at sub levels.

My taxonomy concerning road safety campaigns contains several levels. The first level presents the main results with aim and target group as variables in a specific type of road safety campaign, i.e. alcohol and traffic. The locations in the taxo nomy are then filled in with the empirical results, i.e. 17% of the campaigns had a significant effect on road users knowledge. The next level shows the medium used in these campaigns and whether it was combined with some other type of in uence (reinforcement), i.e. police supervision. The next level presents further information on the length of the campaigns and whether they were presented in a neutral or in uential/biased way.

The development of the taxonomy is only the first step towards a useful tool for road safety information. The next step will thus be to fill in the locations in the taxonomy with data from empirical studies. This process also includes theoretical

(13)

III

parts in a process of interaction for a better understanding of the effects to be expected from road safety campaigns.

(14)
(15)

1 BACKGROUND

1.1 Introduction

This report is a summary of two studies. The first is a literature survey of information campaigns and their effects on road safety. The second is the development of a theoretical model based on the results from the literature survey. The aim of the literature study was to make a survey of how road safety campaigns had been carried out and to find a pattern for an effective campaign. By studying how successful the campaigns had been, i.e. whether the effects were considered positive, I thought I might be able to find this pattern. The result of the literature survey was approximately 800 references to road safety campaigns covering a period of 10 years (1980-1990). Approximately 10% of these could be eliminated as they had a fairly adequate evaluation. The original idea, i.e. only including studies with both test and control groups, had left even fewer references to study. The penetrated studies conveyed a very heterogeneous impression. They

vary for instance in quality and extent.

1.2 Issues

In an attempt to classify all the impressions gained during this work, the following

issues have been listed:

Is there a system or pattern for the execution of road safety campaigns? - How do we measure the effects of campaigns?

- What is measured?

- What are the various aims of the campaigns? What different media do we choose?

- What measuring instruments are used in evaluation?

- What relevance does the measurement method have to that which is measured? - After how long do we measure?

- Which are the target groups?

- Which design was used for the evaluation? - What to study: short-term or long-term effects? - How are road safety campaigns defined?

(16)

In order to find some clues to the answers to these questions and to be able to classify campaigns in groups, I started by classifying them according to road safety subject field. The most common subjects were quite easy to classify, such as: safety belts and child restraint systems in cars, children in traffic, alcohol and drugs in combination with driving, unprotected and vulnerable/exposed road users. The subject fields with few studies, i.e. speed, night-time visibility, were combined into one group: road safety - miscellaneous. Besides these evaluations of previous campaigns, I also included a group of studies which I have called practical-theoretical approaches, as they do not refer to any specific campaign but rather aim at more general approaches.

In these groups the media, the target groups, the aims of the campaigns and the way in which they were evaluated with respect to methods and results were studied. I expected to be able to find some common features of the various campaigns, i.e. whether campaigns focusing on children in traffic had been carried out in any specific way and with specific techniques compared with those dealing with alcohol and drugs.

1.3 A brief specification of the literature

The denominations overall aim and primary aim are used in the text. Overall aim means an aim which is often implicit and not pronounced in the project description, but is still visible between the lines in the rest of the description of the project. The primary aim is that which is expressed in the project description.

1.3.1 Alcohol, drugs and driving

The subject field Alcohol, drugs and driving is based on the following references:

Downing (1987), Cousins (1980), Armour (1985), Rydgren (1982c), Rydgren (1980), Lacey (1987), Perkins, Norstrom (1980), Froyland (1983), Elliot (1983),

Harrison (1988). Generally speaking, the overall aim of these campaigns is to try to change road user behaviour, i.e. preventing road users from using alcohol or drugs when driving. The in uence of campaigns is above all concentrated on knowledge and attitudes. The primary aim of these campaigns seems to be increased awareness of supervision and legislation, observed in the campaigns, and increased knowledge of the risks of driving under the in uence of drink. Most campaigns concerning alcohol and driving are not directed at any specific group of VTI REPORT 394A

(17)

road users but to all holders of a driver s licence. Some have, however, been

specially directed at young men as drivers.

The media and methods used to convey the message about alcohol and traf c vary from TV spots and advertisements in the press to radio messages, posters and cinema advertising, in combination with police supervision and alcohol tests to varying degrees. The campaigns were also evaluated in many different ways: by means of questionnaires and telephone interviews, breath tests and accident

statistics.

The results varied; some campaigns measured a signi cant increase in attention to the campaign, while others had the opposite result. In some campaigns, signi cant increases in knowledge and significant reductions of accidents have been observed, while in others no significant changes in behaviour or accidents were noticed. Self-reported behavioural changes and increased detection risks were significant results of some campaigns.

1.3.2 Children and traffic

The campaigns concerning children and traf c are based on the following

references: Rydgren (1982a), Rydgren (1982b), Eriksson (1983), Rydgren (1981), Rothman (1985), Perry (1980), TSV (1977), Elofson (1978). Their aim was to

reduce the number of accidents involving children by increasing knowledge about children and traffic. The primary aim of these campaigns is to teach parents to train their children in traffic and to increase knowledge of children s limited abilities, the risks to which they are exposed in traffic and children s safety in cars. The target group is often parents, as well as other adults and sometimes young

men as drivers.

In order to spread these messages, several methods were used. It is common to send out brochures or advertise in papers, and also to arrange press conferences

and discussions, sometimes in combination with local activities, e.g. combined

publicity walks and quiz competitions as well as other kinds of competitions. Some campaigns even used films, TV spots and radio features.

In order to measure the results of the campaigns, road users have been interviewed and questionnaires distributed. In some cases, observations of the behaviour of

(18)

acci-dent and injury rates in connection with campaigns have been studied. The results vary from the campaign being noticed to signi cant differences in knowledge, use of safety equipment (e.g. child restraints) and a reduction of injuries. In those cases where other behaviour was observed, no significant changes occurred.

1.3.3 Unprotected and exposed/vulnerable road users

Unprotected and exposed/vulnerable road users are elderly or disabled people as well as the road user groups: pedestrians, cyclists, moped riders and motorcyclists.

My study is based on the following references: Huebner (1980), Berchem (1987),

Samdahl (1986), Glad (1979), Stene (1988), Solvi (1988), Linklater (1980).

The overall aim of these campaigns is to reduce the number of accidents involving unprotected road users. The primary, stated aim is to spread and increase know-ledge of unprotected road users, e.g. pedestrians or to increase the visibility of motorcyclists.

The campaigns are directed at many different target groups: parents, young men, elderly people, pedestrians, disabled persons, drivers etc. The media used are advertisements in the scientific or daily press, posters in buses, radio and TV fea-tures and brochures. Sometimes, information is combined with supervision and increased fines, as well as with a discount on the price of for example bicycle

helmets.

In order to evaluate the campaigns, road users have been interviewed and observed

and speed checks have been carried out. Because of the results, attention has been

drawn to the campaigns to a varying degree: between 50 and 90% have noticed the campaigns. Significant increases in knowledge and in the use and sale of safety equipment, e.g. bicycle helmets, have also been observed. Campaigns have, however, not had any significant effect on accidents.

1.3.4 Seat belts and restraint systems

Many campaigns deal with seat belts and restraint systems: Gundy (1988),

Australian Road Traf c Board (1983), Jonah (1982), Jonah (1985), Lane (1984), Nelson (1988), Tuottey (1982), Boughton (1979), McGregor (1988), Lane (1983), Gundy (1987), Hunter (1986), Hunter (1985), Campbell (1983), Levens (1973).

(19)

The overall aim is probably to reduce injuries among all car passengers. Primarily, aims are expressed such as an increase in the use of seat belts through a change of attitudes and an increase in the subjective risk of being detected without a seat belt.

The principal target group of these campaigns consists of car passengers, but certain campaigns are directed especially at parents and young drivers. Combina-tions of information and police supervision are common. Local media,

news-papers, TV, radio, crash demonstrations, membership clubs, films, press con-ferences, brochures, feedback concerning the number of injured and convicted,

prizes to belted road users etc. are examples of ways of reaching road users with the message of protecting oneself in a car.

In order to determine whether the campaigns had the intended effect, e.g. the number of persons using seat belts has been observed. Drivers have been inter-viewed, questionnaires distributed and accident rates in connection with campaigns studied. Increased use of protective systems, changed attitudes and reduced numbers of accidents are all significant differences, .resulting from the campaigns. Approximately 50 to 90% of the persons consulted had observed the campaigns and there was also a significant long-term effect of using protective

systems. However, certain campaigns did not cause any changes in the use of the

different protective systems.

1.3.5 Road safety - Miscellaneous

Under the heading Road safety - Miscellaneous, we find campaigns concerning aspects such as speed and night time visibility. The ultimate aim of these campaigns is probably to reduce accidents, while the primary aim is to increase knowledge, in uence attitudes or change behaviour, e.g. making drivers drive more safely or increasing the observance of regulations. The methods used to

in uence road users varied from so-called Road Shows , where the dangers of

driving were shown, as well as local risk information with maps pinpointing the greatest dangers in traffic. TV programmes, press conferences and articles in the scientific press were other methods. Billposting and elements of competition were also used together with other information.

(20)

The references treating Road safety Miscellaneous are as follows: Jiggins (1984),

Mander (1983), Cairney (1985), Sheppard (1982), Rydgren (1978a), Tingvall (1979), Rydgren (1978b), Rydgren (1982), Moe (1987).

Measuring instruments comprised interviews, questionnaires and observations. Accident frequency was also studied and speed measurements were carried out. Of the persons interviewed, 50 to 90% had observed these campaigns and in specific campaigns attitudes were measured over a long period which showed that 75% had changed their attitude after the campaign. In other campaigns, no changes either in attitudes, speed or accidents were observed, while some campaigns had long term effects in the form of increased knowledge and the use of devices such

as retroreflectors.

1 .3.6 Practical theoretical approaches

The references applying a theoretical approach rather than studying the effect of a

specific campaign are Haskins (1985), Johansson (1990), Sheppard (1980), Mackie (1975), TRRL (1985), Bérard Andersen (1984), Rein (1986), Asp (1983),

OECD (1986), OECD (1971), SWOV (1976).

The content of these studies varies considerably and a survey of every study would be too comprehensive and would deviate from the aim of this report. Con sequently, I have chosen to present a selection of some more general approaches that might be of interest to this study.

Haskins (1985) supports my opinion about poor studies. He studied campaigns on alcohol and traffic and found that only 36 of 500 studies were performed in such a way that it was possible to draw any conclusions from the effects. His comments on the survey are as follows:

The reports were frequently difficult to read, had mixed conclusions or were unduly optimistic about effects (p. 185).

He creates hopes that it is not only by chance a campaign has the intended effect. The campaigns where previous studies have been surveyed, target groups, their media habits, knowledge, attitudes and lifestyle identified, and this information utilised in the planning of the campaign, various campaign material pre-tested, chosen the best, the messages placed in the correct medium and at the correct VTI REPORT 394A

(21)

point in time, had the intended effect. Yet another factor which may be decisive is the knowledge of the personnel working with the campaigns. It is an advantage if they have knowledge of both campaign activities and research, according to

Haskins.

Furthermore, Johansson (in OECD, 1990) confirms my observations. He goes even further and declares that the only common factor of road safety campaigns is the fact that they use mass media to spread something . In all other variables, e.g. target group and contents of the campaign they differ, according to Johansson. He also seeks a taxonomy for road safety campaigns.

Also in OECD (1971), the same observations of the limited effects of road safety

campaigns were made. A systematic survey of road safety campaigns with refer-ence to aspects such as aim and the role of information in road safety work is sought

In SWOV (1976), the effects of information campaigns on the road users use of safety systems, e.g. seat belts, were discussed. It seems possible to expect a small improvement in seat belt usage if small groups are addressed. No comment is

made on other information effects.

In Asp (1983) there are many interesting articles on information and campaigns. Ottander discusses the effects that can be expected by providing information. According to him, people cannot be changed with mass distributed information, whether this concerns knowledge, attitudes or behaviour. Information alone cannot produce permanent effects such as changed behaviour in traffic. Mass communication often only produces short-term effects, lasting only as long as the information continues, and care should therefore be taken when interpreting possible long term effects of campaigns.

Ottander also speaks of latent and manifest effects of information. Latent effects are shown by what people say and do, while the manifest effects of information are noticed in knowledge or behaviour. The latent effects of information are observed after a long time and are thus difficult to measure, but are important as they can explain the effects of information.

Rein (1986) carried out a very interesting theory and literature survey with refer ence to behaviour and actual consequences of information campaigns. The survey

(22)

shows that most road safety campaigns did not have any effect. The few campaigns of effect contained information on the consequences of behaviour in accordance with road users own experience. When the consequences described in information campaigns do not occur, people violate traffic regulations. That is why campaigns trying to make people follow regulations often only have a short effect. Road user behaviour is thus formed by the consequences to which road users have been exposed in traffic. It is, however, more difficult to reward desired traf c behaviour than to punish undesired traffic behaviour, according to Rein. Cairney (1985) investigated the extent to which road safety information was observed with respect to the media used and the content of the campaigns. Nearly 90% of the interviewed had observed a road safety campaign. Stylishly designed brochures and posters are cost-effective media as they reach a large public at a

very low cost. However, TV is the most effective medium with reference to

short-term effects, although newspapers are to be preferred for long-short-term effects of information.

Sheppard and Harryson (1980) studied the readability of traffic information, e.g. the length of sentences and words. There was no uniform pattern: some were easy to read and some quite difficult. Other studies showed that many persons learning to drive a car have difficulties in fully profiting from the textbooks and thus lose all desire to continue, according to the author. The following recommendations are given:

utilise short words and short sentences

avoid negative expressions, abbreviations and subordinate clauses short words are not sufficient - they must also be easy to understand the content must be brief: give priority according to degree of importance! Mackie and Valentine (1975) surveyed different approaches or techniques used

in road safety information, e.g. horror, humour, satire, celebrity as model, official

information, family responsibility or sex. These different techniques were then compared with factual information having no affective features. They concluded that factual information seems to have just as good an effect on behaviour as affective information. In both cases, serious approaches are preferred - humour is not trustworthy. A ranking of the most effective approaches is as follows:

(23)

Factual information Celebrity as a model Authority information Family responsibility Horror Humour N P P P PN ? Satire

The most effective were the combinations factual information with horror features and information on family responsibility presented by a known person performed

with new methods in order to increase memorisation.

1.3.7 Summary and aim

As mentioned above, I hoped to find some joint features for the various campaigns, i.e. whether campaigns directed at children in traffic had been carried out in a special way and with special techniques compared, for instance, with those dealing with alcohol and traffic. This was not the case. It was even less encouraging when I looked at the results, which varied just asmuch in children and traffic campaigns as in the subject field of alcohol and traffic. As mentioned above, under the heading Issues, it was very difficult to nd out whether the

measurement method was always the correct method or whether another method

would have given a different result. From my literature study, it was very difficult to find a system for classifying road safety campaigns. One possibility was to increase the number of studies in each subject field, thereby increasing the possi-bility of finding a pattern.

At the same time, it was difficult to know whether my approach was correct. Were there other, more complete ways of classifying road safety campaigns? The inclu-sion of an item Road safety - Miscellaneous was not satisfactory. As mentioned, an increase in the number of studies could possibly result in new so-called subject fields, which would give a more uniform pattern of how road safety campaigns had been and should be carried out. A one-sided basis of empirical studies to find a pattern for the optimal execution of road safety campaigns is, however, a very time-consuming procedure. A better and probably faster result is obtained if the empirical results are connected with theory. It is then possible to return to empiri and test whether the new theory is correct - consequently entailing an interaction process. In order to find the answers to these questions, I turned to the literature on

(24)

10

classification, typologies and taxonomies. The aim was to be able to classify road safety campaigns in a more systematic and distinct way, and finally to arrive at a pattern for making them more effective.

(25)

11

2 TYPES, TYPOLOGIES AND TAXONOMIES 2.1 What is a typology?

Typologies are the basis of all science (Rosengren, 1993).

In the 20th century, it has been common to start from classification schemata, e.g.

typologies or taxonomies, as a prerequisite for scientific studies, according to Suppe(1981)

The name typology originates from the Greek word typos, meaning type or sort. A type or a concept is defined by Bailey (1992) as a cell in a typology. A type consists of a combination of values of several variables. The variables forming a type must have something in common compared with other types. Suppe (1981) found that these common features are nothing other than statistical correlations. AcCording to him, the best way of creating typologies is to study empirical

Connections.

Gardenfors (1990) refers to classical theory and Aristotle when he talks about

types or concepts. There are two conditions when we want to describe the meaning of a concept: one necessary and one sufficient. As an example, the con-cept of a human being is used, together with the distinction between man and animals. First, we must describe the number of characteristics all people have and then the characteristics of man alone. Man is an animal, according to Aristotle,

but in contrast to animals, man is a rational creature.

According to Lazarsfeld (1937) and Lazarfeld and Barton (1951) a type is a special composition of attributes. There are three types of attributes:

1 . Characteristicum 2. Serial

3. Variable

A Characteristicum is an attribute, which we either have or do not have. It consists of an attribute or data on the nominal scale level, i.e. only specifying the

class to which a certain Characteristicum belongs. Another term for a

characteristi-cum would be a dichotomy, according to Lazarsfeld and Barton (1951). An example of a Characteristicum or a dichotomy is gender.

(26)

12

A serial consists of attributes which can only exist in comparison with each other. It is a ranking of attributes but does not specify the size of the distance between them, so these data cannot be used in mathematical calculations. We can compare the serial kinds of attributes with data on the ordinal scale level. An example of a serial is the different places in a competition.

A variable permits the gradation of attributes in a specific order with the same distance between them. In specific cases, the scale also has an absolute zero. Variable data thus correspond to the scale levels of interval and quota. An example of a variable is weight, which can for example be specified in kilograms and hectograms (equal steps on the scale) and with an absolute zero.

There is a connection between these three different attributes. It is always possible

to transform a variable into a serial and a serial into a characteristicum, but not in the inverse direction.

Baily (1992) defines a typology as a multi-dimensional classification, where groups are based on similarity. According to Bailey, a taxonomy would then correspond to the theoretical part, i.e. the bases, rules or principles of classi ca-tion. The terms typology and taxonomy are often used synonymously and some-times we speak of taxonomies as more advanced typologies (Rosengren, 1993). Naturally, the primary aim of the classification is to classify. There are three

demands (Rosengren, 1993). The classification should be:

1 . Distinct

2. Comprehensive 3. Fruitful.

Tirykian (1968) also adds the demand on economy when choosing main groups or main types, as long as this does not hazard the validity, i.e. the risk of reducing the number of groups so the factors in the group differ too widely. This means a weighing between a too detailed typology, which will reduce clarity and a typology where the elements in the groups differ too widely. Lazarsfeld and Barton (1951) have a solution to the dilemma. They suggest a classification

divided into several steps or at several levels, based on a few overall categories,

which are then decomposed into many more and detailed categories. Looking for an answer to a more general level, the first level could be sufficient. If more

(27)

13

details are required, we continue to the next level, etc. Consequently, we must not

choose.

In order to make categories more distinct, Lazarsfeld and Barton (1951) point to the danger of mixing categories at different concept levels. If we wish to classify

different information sources, would it not be possible to use mass media as a

category together with daily papers, which are a sub-group of mass media? By adding more sub-groups or categories to the classification, we can also develop a more comprehensive typology.

Tiryakian (1968) states that the function of the typology is more than a simple description of reality. A typology creates order in a chaos of heterogeneous observations by simplifying and eliminating all the constituents of the population. A typology makes it possible to search for and anticipate connections that

other-wise would be dif cult to detect. Baily (1992) confirms this specific function and

states that there is no other means of assistance which emphasises all dimensions and at the same time shows their mutual dependence.

Finally, Bailey (1992) discusses the criticism of Webers so-called ideal type , i.e. a theoretical construction which has its highest value in all characteristics. Critics claim that this is a mental construction which cannot be found in reality, a utopia as a result of intellectual acrobatics where we compare actual individuals with ideal typical individuals to see how much they deviate. Bailey states that the ideal type can be found in real life, even if it is rare. It is then a good basis for comparisons, such as the highest value of a variable or a terminal point on a scale.

2.2 Typological functions and attribute space

Both Lazarsfeld (1937) and Barton (1955) talk about attributes and attribute space.

An example of attribute space can be seen in the scores in a test of mathematical and linguistic ability. When these are placed in a coordinate system, a room in two dimensions is formed, describing a person s linguistic and mathematical

(28)

14 Mathematics 5 __ 4__ O l l I l l | | l l l Languajge

Figure 1 The attribute space Language-Mathematics

An attribute space can have as many dimensions as the attributes of which it consists, making the picture quitecomplex. For practical and theoretical reasons

we may then reduce the number of dimensions. Lazarsfeld (1937), Lazarsfeld and

Barton (1951) and Barton (1955) speak of different types of reduction: Functional

Arbitrary numerical Pragmatic.

To be called functional, there must be an actual relationship between the attri

butes of the reduction. The result is that specific combinations will be very rare or equal to zero. A well-known example of a functional reduction is the so-called Guttman scale, where it is possible to continue to the next level etc. if the

state-ment at the lowest level is considered correct.

An arbitrary numerical reduction is carried out by giving each attribute a specific value or weight. The attributes with the same value are combined to one category. The example given by Lazarfeld and Barton (1951) is an evaluation of various household equipment. Do we, for instance, value central heating as much

as a dishwasher? If this is the case, both will have the same value and are placed in the same category.

(29)

15

In a pragmatic reduction, individuals with specific combinations of attributes are placed in categories with respect to the aim of the study. This means that other specific attributes, of no interest to the study, areexcluded. If we wish to study how married women s attitudes to their husbands careers in uence their hus-bands success in professional life, women can be sorted according to how positive their attitudes are towards their husbands, e.g. very positive, fairly positive, etc. to very negative. The same goes for men s success in professional life, from very successful to completely unsuccessful. The result may be ten cate-gories in all, which can then be compared.

Barton (1955) also mentions a fourth type of reduction, called reduction through simplification of the dimensions. As an example, he mentions the classification of a continuous variable, e.g. division into different age groups.

According to Barton (1955), the most frequent and most important type of reduc-tion in empirical research is the pragmatic reducreduc-tion.

The contrast to reduction is substruction. According to Lazarsfeld (1937) and

Lazarsfeld and Barton (1951), substruction is the procedure where we try to find the underlying attribute space and its implicit reduction. The substruction expands the typology and adds new dimensions. Thus the typology will be more "fine-meshed and it will be possible to catch new attributes which are of interest in our studies. The practical value of the substruction is that it is possible to check whether any characteristics have been overlooked and whether they are over-lapping. The result is a better adaptation to the aim of research and improvements in the typology. The substruction which has the greatest practical importance takes place when a pragmatic reduction has been carried out, i.e. when different groups have beenbrought together with regard to the aim of the study. In the example above, concerning the attitudes of married women, specific characteristics had been eliminated which were of no interest to the study. A substruction where a new attribute, for instance social group affiliation is introduced, may explain possible differences in the attitudes of these women.

Transformation is another function of the typology. Transformation involves trying to find another basic attribute space than was previously used. Since the transformation is the basis of our interpretation of the statistical results in a study, it has a very importantfunction. This interpretation often consists of a

(30)

trans-16

formation of the studied attributes to a new attribute space. In the new attribute space, new connections and new reductions of attributes can be found.

2.3 Monothetic and polythetic typologies, respectively

Baily (1973 and 1983) speaks about monothetic and polythetic typologies. A typology is monothetic if a specific set of properties is both necessary and sufficient for the individuals1 in the typology. These demands can be compared with those mentioned by Gardenfors (1990) as demands on a concept there is a necessary and a sufficient condition to describe the signification of a concept. Firstly, we describe the properties applicable to all individuals and then the speci c properties of the individuals in whom we are interested. A simple example of a monothetic typology has two dimensions and the properties gender and age, which are both necessary and suf cient to describe the individuals in the cells of the typology.

In a polythetic typology, a specific characteristic is neither necessary nor sufficient. If every individual in a typology has a great number of the characteristics in the typology and if every characteristic is held by many individuals, the typology is polythetic. Furthermore, if none of the characteristics in the typology is held by all individuals in the typology, it is completely poly thetic.

A monothetic typology is often artificial and is seldom found in real life (Bailey, 1983). It is a simpli cation and often contains only a few variables. A polythetic typology, on the contrary, offers a better picture of reality. The main aim when constructing a typology is to try to form as homogeneous groups as possible. Great demands are made upon distinctness when choosing the types or concepts on which the classification is based. Proceeding from empirical data, where the studied objects may be similar in most variables, but possibly differ in a few, groups must be formed which are as perfect as reality allows.

When constructing a classical typology (Bailey, 1973), a typology is first created in theory, after which its correspondence with real life is studied. Thus, typologies are often monothetic to begin with, but are revised to polythetic after empirical

tests.

1 In this context, the concept of individual symbolises the content in the cells of the typology.

(31)

17

3 A TAXONOMY OF ROAD SAFETY CAMPAIGNS

3.1 Introduction

The input variables or the independent variables in all communication can be

illustrated in one sentence. According to Lasswell (in Rice and Atkin, 1989)

communication is:

Who says what, through which channel, to whom and with what aim?

The communication variables are thus:

- Source

- Message

Channel or Medium Receiver or Target group

- Aim

My aim is to develop a taxonomy for road safety campaigns. I have chosen to call it a taxonomy as it consists of several levels and can be developed even further. According to Rosengren (1993), a taxonomy is a more advanced typology. Completely developed, this typology will be called a taxonomy.

Campaigns are the type of communication which at least formerly was called mass communication. The variables included in road safety campaigns should thus be

the same as those mentioned above.

The variables chosen for my taxonomy on road safety information, based on a literature survey of road safety campaigns, will be treated and defined. There may be other and better ways of describing and defining all the variables in a campaign, but based on my empirical study these were of special interest. Furthermore, it should be emphasised that the definitions used are my own and may thus differ from other, more conventional definitions.

Finally, I shall try to illustrate the various levels in the taxonomy in three Figures showing a few examples of how the taxonomy may be used it in practice.

(32)

18 3.2 Input variables of the taxonomy 3.2.1 Source and Message

To make the taxonomy clearer and more useful, two of the variables found in communication have been reduced to background variables, on which the under-lying structure of the campaign is based. They are held constant in comparison

with the other variables. One of these variables is source, which I assumed to be

an authority in all road safety campaigns, e.g. the Swedish National Road Administration or the National Society for Road Safety. The other is the message, which I define as the subject treated by the campaign, e.g. alcohol and traffic. The message is used as a sorting instrument to form comparable groups of remaining

variables.

3 .2. 1. 1 Reinforcement

As my study is based on empiri, I have had to add another important variable from road safety campaigns, often combined with information. This is another type of

in uence, e.g. police supervision, legislation or competitions with prizes, often

considered a necessary and natural part of the whole package of road safety

information. In behavioural terms, I would call this variable reinforcement. The

reinforcement can be positive or negative depending on what it denotes to the individual. In the road safety context, police supervision with fines would be negative reinforcement, while a competition with prizes would be positive

rein-forcement.

3.2.2 Medium

The medium or channel chosen by the transmitter to disseminate his/her message is of course very important for achieving the best effect. Under this heading, there

is a number of different media or channels, such as TV, radio, the press and films.

There is a limited choice of these media where there is often a great distance between sender and receiver and where the contacts between sender and receiver are impersonal. This type of medium often requires some sort of technique for the

message to reach the receiver.

To make the picture of media more clear, I have reduced the number of levels to

two, i.e. written and audio/visual media. Written media implies e.g. advertise VTI REPORT 394A

(33)

19

ments in papers or brochures, while audio/visual media can be radio or TV. The components in these two groups can be speci ed in more detail if a finer structure is required.

3.2.2.1 Method of presentation

The method of presenting the message on road safety also in uences the receivers receptiveness. Method of presentation refers e.g. to whether the message is presented with features of horror or humour, or if there are only facts with no affective component presented. This variable was classified in two sub groups, called affective and neutral methods of presentation, depending on how the information was presented.

3.2.2.2

Durability

The durability is also of importance to the impact. Durability of road safety campaigns may vary from a few days to several years. A categorisation is thus necessary and I have chosen to divide this variable into three levels:

S 1 month

2 5 months 2 6 months

3.2.3 Target group

Another variable, decisive for the effect of the message, is the target group to which the information is directed. In most road safety campaigns, one of the following categories of target groups has been used:

Road user category - Age category

- Interest category

The road user category includes the different types of road users, e.g. motorists, motorcyclists, moped riders, cyclists and pedestrians. The age category may vary, but a distinction is often made between young and old road users. A specific target

(34)

20

group can also be chosen, such as 18-24-year-olds, which is the most accident-prone group. The third category of target groups, the interest category, consists of those who have a common interest in road safety, for instance parents, who are expected to be interested in the road safety of their children or disabled, who face special problems in traffic.

To produce a clearer taxonomy, I have chosen to study one of these categories of target groups at a time, e. g. the road user category.

3.2.4 Aim and Results

The road safety campaigns I have examined had the following aims: - that the campaign is observed

to change knowledge - to change attitudes - to change behaviour

These are the four levels for which the aim varies in the road safety campaigns studied. The result or effect specifies the degree to which the aim has been achieved and is hopefully measured at these levels as well. This will be our

dependent variable, which in the best case can be measured in quantitative terms

and used as a measure of the total effect of the independent variables.

Even though the effects of many campaigns are only measured through observa-tion, this is still a prerequisite for all other types of effects, e.g. changes in know-ledge. The final aim of most campaigns should still be a change in behaviour, in any case as a last link in the process towards a society that is safer from the road safety aspect. The expectations of what can be achieved through information should be one of the decisive factors for the aim chosen. Consequently, observa-tion is eliminated as an aim of road safety campaigns, since observaobserva-tion is only a prerequisite for other effects.

3.3 Description of the taxonomy

Since the taxonomy consists of many variables and each variable has several components, the picture may be very complicated. A carefully prepared structure VTI REPORT 394A

(35)

21

is thus important.The taxonomy was thus constructed in different levels according to Lazarsfeldt and Barton (1951), starting with a rough structure and a few comprehensive categories. Looking for an answer on a more general level, this level could be sufficient. If, however, we want more detailed information on the design of the campaign, we continue to the next level with a finer structure showing which combinations of variables are the basis of the effects we obtain at the more general level. The great advantage of this procedure is that we will have both a general level and more detailed levels in the same taxonomy. As a defini-tion, this taxonomy or typology could be classi ed as polythetic. According to Bailey (1983), polythetic typologies offer a better picture of reality.

As a first step, I have chosen to show the main result as in Figure (2), where the

aim and the target group are variables in an example of message. The taxonomy

is then structured more precisely with two more variables, medium and rein-forcement (Figure 3), to include the variables method of presentation and durability (Figure 4) in the last step.

The results from the empiri will be entered in the cells of the taxonomy, preferably with quantitative measures, e.g. 17% of the campaigns on alcohol and traffic have had a signi cant effect on the knowledge of the road user category (Figure 2). If we wish to continue and see whether this is applicable to any specific category of road users, the category can be structured in more detail in subgroups, e.g. motorists and cyclists. If we then wish to know what medium has been used in these campaigns, we continue to the next level (Figure 3), where we learn whether the campaigns have been combined with some other sort of in uence (reinforcement). This second in uence can be found by structuring it into its sub-groups, e.g. police surveillance or so-called speed-reducing bumps in the road. If we continue further in the taxonomy, we will reach level 3 (Figure 4), where further information is obtained concerning the duration of the campaign and whether it has been presented in an affective or neutral way. These two variables can naturally be structured even further to determine for instance, whether the message was presented with humorous features and whether the campaigns lasted for ten days or three months.

3.3.1 Practical application of the taxonomy

The development of the taxonomy is thus only the first step in the process towards a useful tool that can be applied in road safety information. Through literature or

(36)

22

empirical tests, the next step will be to fill the cells in the taxonomy with empirical data in order to obtain a picture of the sector: information on road safety.

It is also conceivable to enlarge the taxonomy with further variables that are of interest. The cells are then lled with empirical data. There is also a possibility of reducing the number of variables, depending on the results of the empiri. Some of

the included variables could be less interesting and other variables, not included in

the taxonomy at present, will appear to be of greater interest to road safety campaigns. In the development process towards a better understanding of the effects that can be expected of road safety campaigns, an interaction process between empiri and theory will be necessary.

As the basic data of the taxonomy is very comprehensive, it may be advisable to enter all the information into a data base. Data processing would be simplified considerably as it will be much easier to move between the different levels of the taxonomy. It would also be easier to make parallel comparisons between different types of messages and to find indications of connections not previously antici-pated.

(37)

23

MESSAGE: ALCOHOL AND TRAFFIC

Aim

Target group Change In Change m Change 1

Road users knowledge attitude behaviour

*

Motorist Motor-cyclists and moped riders Cyclists Pedestrians

Figure 2 Main result: variables Aim and Target group

In Figure 2, the results are presented in an overall form in order to make it possible to decide whether a specific message - in this example, a message on alcohol and traffic - had any effect on any specific road user group and if so, what type of effect, for example concerning knowledge or behaviour.

*I have chosen this cell as an example in order to continue to the next level inthe taxonomy (Figure 3 on the next page).

(38)

24

| MESSAGE: ALCOHOL AND ROAD USERS

AIM: TO INFLUENCE KNOWLEDGE

Reinforcement

Target group Positive Negative

Road users Media

>3 Written

Motorists

Audio/Visual

Figure 3 At this level, the variables Medium and Reinforcement are added

Figure 3 describes the effect of a message with a specific aim - in this example, a message on alcohol and traf c with the aim of in uencing knowledge - of a speci c road user group (motorists), with different types of reinforcement and different types of media.

*I have chosen this cell as an example in order to continue to the next level in the

taxonomy (Figure 4 on the next page).

(39)

25

MESSAGE: ALCOHOL AND ROAD USERS I

AIM: TO INFLUENCE KNOWLEDGE

Reinforcement: Positive

Method of

Target

Media

presenta'

Affective

Neutral

Road users information information

Durability

S 1 month

Motorists Written

2 - 5 months 2 6 months

Figure 4 At this level, the variables Method of presentation and Durability are

added

Figure 4 shows the effect of a message on alcohol and traffic with the aim of in uencing knowledge of the target group motorists with a written medium, by using different methods of presentation and different durability.

(40)

26 4 REFERENCES

Armour, B. et al. Evaluation of the 1983 Melbourne random breath testing campaign. Road traffic authority, Australia, report No. 8: 1985.

Asp, K. (ed). Vilken roll spelar information och kampanjer for trafiksakerhe-ten? Statens vag- och trafikinstitut (VTI), rapport 269, 1983.

Australian Road Traffic Board, Division of road safety and motor transport. Development and evaluation ofthe seat belt publicity campaign 1982. Walkerville, Australia: 1983.

Bailey, K.B. Sociological Classification and Cluster Analysis. Quality and Quantity, 17:251 268: 1983.

Bailey, K.B. Typlogies. Encyclopedia of Sociology, 4:2188 2194: 1992

Bailey, K.D. Monothetic and Polythetic Typologies and Their Relation to Conceptualization, Measurement and Scaling. American Sociological Review, 38:18-33: 1973.

Barton, A.H. The Concept of Property-Space in Social Research. I Lazarsfeld,

P.F. & Rosenberg, M. (Eds.) The Language of Social Research. A Reader in the

Methodology of Social Research, 40-53. New York: The Free Press: 1955.

Be rard Andersen, K. Promillekiiring og promilletra kanter - en litteraturstu-die. Transportekonomisk Inst., Oslo, 1984.

Berchem, S.P. A community campaign that increased helmet use among bicyclists. In Transportation Research Record 1141, Transport Research Board, Wash. DC. USA: 1987.

Boughton, C]. and Johnston, I.R. The effects of radio and press publicity on the safe carriage of children in cars. In SAE Techn. paper series, Society of

Automotive engineers, Warrendale, Penn., USA: 1979.

Cairney, P.T. Recall of road safety and driving publicity by Australian drivers

in October and November 1983. In Australian Road Research Tech. Note No. 2, 15(3), September 1985.

Campbell, B]. et a1. Seat belts. pay off: a community-wide research project to increase use of lap and shoulder belts. North Carolina Univ. Highway Safety Research Center: HSRC report 84: 1983.

Cousins, L.S. The effects of public education on subjective probability of

arrest for impaired driving. In Accident Analysis and Prevention, Vol. 12, No.

2, June 1980.

(41)

27

Downing C.S. Young driver alcohol education and publicity programmes in Britain. In Young drivers impaired by alcohol and other drugs, London: 1987.

Elliot, B., and South, D. The development and assessment of a drink-driving

campaign: a case study. Australian dept. of transport, Of ce of road safety, CR 26: 1983.

Elofson, S. Datanalys - en fallstudie. Hade TSV s kampanj "Barn i trafiken"

nagon kortsiktig effekt pa trafiksiikerheten? Statistiska institutionen, Stock-holms universitet: 197822.

Eriksson, L. Barn i trafiken. Barnens tra kvecka 1982 - Resultatet av en enkat. Trafiksakerhetsverket, PM 1, 1983.

Froyland, P. Trygg Tra kk s promilleaksjon i Ostfold i 1982. Transporteko

nomisk Inst., Oslo: 1983.

Glad, A. Effekten av Trygg Tra kk s kampanj "De svake trafikanterne". Transportekonomisk Inst., Oslo, projektrapport 1979.

Gundy C. M. The effectiveness of a combination of police encforcement and public information for improving seat-belt use. In Road user behaviour, Van

Gorcum, Assen: 1988.

Gundy, C.M. The effects of a combined enforcement and publication campaign on seat belt use. In Proceedings of the international workshop "Recent developments in road safety research", Haag: 19 november 1986. Inst. for road

safety research, SWOV, Leidschendam, 1987.

Gardenfors, P. En geometrisk model] for begreppsbildning och kategorisering.

Genetik och humaniora, 2: 105-1 16: 1990.

Harrison, W. Evaluation of a drink-drive public and enforcement campaign.

Road traffic authority, Hawthorne, Australia: 198812.

Haskins J.B. The role of massmedia in alcohol and highway safety campaigns. In Journal ofstudies on alcohol, suppl. No. 10, July 1985.

Huebner M.L. ROSTA' s motorcycle visibility campaign. In Proceedings, Vol.

II, Internat. motorcycle safety conference, Wash. DC, USA: May 18 23, 1980.

Hunter, W.W. et a1. Seat belts pay off: a follow-up survey of the seat belts pay off community program. North Carolina Univ. Highway Safety Research Center: 1985.

Hunter, W.W., Campbell, B]. and Stewart, J .R. Seat belts pay off: the

evalua-tion of a community-wide incentive program. In Journal of Safety Research, Vol. 17, 1986.

(42)

28

Jiggins S.G. Evaluation of Road Show. Road traf c safety research council,

Wellington, New Zealand: 1984.

Johansson, R. Education and enforcement. In Behavorial adaptations to changes in the road transport system. Road Transport Research Programme, OECD, 1990.

Jonah, BA. and Grant, B.A. Long-term effectiveness of selective traf c

enforcement program for increasing seat belt use. In Journal of Applied Psychology, Vol. 70, No. 2, 1985.

Jonah, B.A., Davidson, N.E. et al. Effects of a selective traffic enforcement

program on seat belt usage. In Journal of Applied Psychology, Vol. 67,

No. 11982.

Lacey, J.H. Enforcement and public information strategies for driving while intoxicated general deterrence. DOT: H S 807 191, US. Dept. of transport, Nat. highway traffic safety adm., Washington DC: 1987.

Lane, J.M. et al. Evaluation of the 81/82 rear seat belt campaign. Road Safety

and traffic authority, Victoria, Australia: 1983.

Lane, J.M., Milne, B. and Wood, H.T. Evaluation of a successful rear seat belt

publication campaign. In Safety, Australian Road Research Board, Vol. 12, Part 7, 1984.

Lazarsfeld, P.F. & Barton, A.H. Qualitative Measurement in the Social

Sciences: Classi cation, Typologies and Indices. I Lerner, D. & Lasswell, H.D. The Policy Sciences, Stanford, 155-192: 1951.

Lazarsfeld, RF. (1937). Some Remarks on Typological Procedures in Social Research. Zeitschriftfiir Sozialforschung, 6: 1 19-139: 1937.

Levens, GB. The application of research in the planning and evaluation of road safety publicity. In The application ofmarket and social research for more e icient planning Congress 1973, Budapest: 1973.

Linklater, D.R. Road safety media campaigns to protect the young and old.

Traffic Authority, research note 1980:8, New South Wales: 1980.

Mackie, A.M. and Valentine, S.D. Effectiveness of different appeals in road

safety propaganda. Transport and road research laboratory, report 669,

Crowthorne, Berkshire, England: 1975.

Manders, S. An evaluation of an anti-speeding publicity campaign. Road

Traffic Authority, Hawthorn, Australia, Report No. 1:1983.

(43)

29

McGregor, 1. Evaluation of adult and child restraint programs 1988. Australian Dept. of transport, 1988: 12.

Moe, D. et a1. Evaluering av utf'orkjiiringskampanjen 1986. Norges tekn.

h6g-skole, SINTEF, avd. 63, A87006, Trondheim: 1987.

Nelson, G.D. and Moffit, P.B. Safety belt promotion: theory and practice. In

Accident Analysis and Prevention, Vol. 20, No.1, 1988.

Norstrom, T. An evaluation of an information program against drunken driving. In Alcohol, drugs and tra ic safety, Vol. III, 8th Int. conference,

Stock-holm: June 15 19, 1980.

OECD. Road safety campaigns: design and evaluation. OECD, Paris: 1971. OECD. Road safety research: a synthesis. OECD, Paris: 1986.

Perkins, WA. The 1975 Drinking and driving publicity campaign. Traffic

research report No. 24, Min. of transport, Wellington, New Zealand, u.2°1.

Perry, R.L. et a1. Impact of a child passenger restraint law and a public information program. Tennessee Univ., Knoxwille, Tenn. USA: 1980.

Rein, J.G. Virkninger av vervakning, straff, belonning og offentlig informa-tion pa tra kanternes adferd. Transportekonomisk Inst., Oslo, projektrapport,

1986.

Rice, R.E. & Atkin, C.K. Public Communication Campaigns. Sage Publ. Inc. Newbury Park, CA. USA: 1981.

Rothman, J. and Kearns, I. Crossing roads isn t child s play: An adult

aware-ness campaign. Traffic Accident research unit, research note 2: 1985, Traf c

Authority, Rosebery, N.S.W. Australia: 1985.

Rydgren, H. Matning av effekter hos malgruppen huvudskyddsombud av kampanjen "till och frz in arbetet". Trafiksakerhetsverket, Intern PM 172:

1978a.

Rydgren, H. Penetrationsstudie av f oraldrabroschyren "Barn r mjuka. Bilar

ar harda". Trafiksakerhetsverket, Intern PM 183: 1982a.

Rydgren, H. Penetrationsundersokning av kampanjen "Barn i trafiken"

hosten 1981. Trafiksakerhetsverket, Intern PM 184: 1982b.

Rydgren, H. Penetrationsundersokning av kampanjen om dagen-efter-effek-ter, december 1980 - januari 1981. Trafiksakerhetsverket, Intern PM 186:

(44)

3O

Rydgren, H. Resultat fran for- och eftermiitning i samband med 1979/80-ars informationskampanj om hastighetsanpassning. Trafiks'akerhetsverket, Intern PM 185:1982d.

Rydgren, H. Resultat fran for- och eftermatning i samband med information i

Orebro om barns sakerhet i bil. Trafiksakerhetsverket, Intern PM 182: 1981.

Rydgren, H. Resultat fran for- och eftermatning i samband med TSV's tra k-nykterhetskampanj 1979. Trafiksakerhetsverket, 1980.

Rydgren, H. Tre matningar avseende malgruppen anstallda i kommunen "till

och fran arbetet". Trafiks'akerhetsverket, Intern PM 175: 1978b.

Samdahl, DR. and Daff, M.R. Effectiveness of a neighbourhood road safety campaign. In Safety, Australian Road Research Board, proceedings 13, 1986:9. Sheppard D. and Harryson, C. Readability of road safety pubications. Transport

and road research laboratory, report 944, Crowthorne, Berkshire, England: 1980.

Sheppard, D. Audience reactions to a television feature program on road

acci-dents. Transport and road research laboratory, report 1041, Crowthorne, Berkshire, England: 1982.

Solvi, E. Evaluering av kampanjen "Starter du pa griint sa rekker du over".

Norges tekn. hogskole, SINTEF, avd. 63, 1988: 1 1.

Stene, T.M. Evaluering av fotgjengerkampanjen 1987. Norges tekn. hogskole,

SINTEF, avd. 63, 1988:6.

Suppe, F. Classi cation. I Intemat. Encyclopedia of Communication. Vol. 1, s.

292-295. Oxford Univ. Press. London: 1981.

SWOV. In uencing road users' behaviour. SWOV, Netherlands: 1976 IE.

Tingvall, C. Undersiikning av forandring av anvandning av fotgangarre exer under morker 1977-78 i anslutning till information om re exanvandning.

Trafiks'akerhetsverket, PM nr 18: 1979.

Tiryakian, E.A. Typologies. I International Encyclopedia of the Social Sciences, 16:177-186. New York: Macmillan: 1968.

TRRL. Opinion polls. Road safety posters over the past decade. Transport and

road research laboratory, translation 3221, Crowthorne, Berkshire, England: 1985.

TSV. Utvardering av kampanjen "Barn i trafiken". Trafiksakerhetsverket, Stockholm: 1977.

(45)

31

Tuottey, M.H. Restraint program clicks at Kodak. In Tra ic Safety, July/Aug.: 1982.

Oral references

Rosengren, K E. Lecture at the Media and Communication Science, University of

(46)
(47)
(48)
(49)
(50)
(51)
(52)

Figure

Figure 1 The attribute space Language-Mathematics
Figure 2 Main result: variables Aim and Target group
Figure 3 At this level, the variables Medium and Reinforcement are added
Figure 4 At this level, the variables Method of presentation and Durability are added

References

Related documents

MANAGING THE COMPETITIVE ENVIRONMENT Focus within industry Differentiation or Cost-cutting RED OCEAN STRATEGY Create new untapped market

On the other hand, the conclusion that the reform message has a weak presence is also reasonable, since our qualitative analyses show that the reform message is not presented

Risken för köldvågor är konsekvent den typ av risk som svenska branscher är mest exponerade mot i sina leverantörskedjor (denna risk finns även i Sverige). Detta kan mycket

spårbarhet av resurser i leverantörskedjan, ekonomiskt stöd för att minska miljörelaterade risker, riktlinjer för hur företag kan agera för att minska miljöriskerna,

46 Konkreta exempel skulle kunna vara främjandeinsatser för affärsänglar/affärsängelnätverk, skapa arenor där aktörer från utbuds- och efterfrågesidan kan mötas eller

In the latter case, these are firms that exhibit relatively low productivity before the acquisition, but where restructuring and organizational changes are assumed to lead

The increasing availability of data and attention to services has increased the understanding of the contribution of services to innovation and productivity in

Av tabellen framgår att det behövs utförlig information om de projekt som genomförs vid instituten. Då Tillväxtanalys ska föreslå en metod som kan visa hur institutens verksamhet