• No results found

Ethnic Harassment and Bully Victimization in Immigrant Adolescents

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "Ethnic Harassment and Bully Victimization in Immigrant Adolescents"

Copied!
24
0
0

Loading.... (view fulltext now)

Full text

(1)

Ethnic Harassment and Bully Victimization in Immigrant Adolescents Karin Schloesser Tárano

Master’s Program in Prevention Science Örebro University

Submitted in Partial Fulfillment of the Master’s Program in Prevention Science Independent Research Course

Spring 2012 Supervisor: Metin Özdemir, Ph.D.

(2)

Abstract

The following study examined the effects of ethnic harassment and bully victimization on immigrant youths’ adjustment over the course of one year. Adjustment outcomes included depression, poor self-esteem, and self-harm. We asked: (1) Is bully victimization a risk for immigrant youths’ adjustment? and (2) Is ethnic harassment a risk for immigrant youths’ adjustment over and beyond bully victimization? Participants included 252 first and second-generation immigrant youths (52% female, 46% born abroad, M= 14.98 years) from seven schools in a mid-sized Swedish city. Multiple hierarchical regression analyses were conducted to test whether bully victimization and ethnic harassment predicted adjustment outcomes one year later and changes in these outcomes over the course of one year. A factor analysis revealed that bully victimization and ethnic harassment were separate constructs. Bully victimization predicted depression and poor self-esteem scores as well as increases in depression over the course of one year. Ethnic harassment predicted depression, poor self-esteem, and self-harm scores as well as increases in these three outcomes over the course of one year. Results suggest that ethnic harassment poses an added risk to the adjustment of immigrant youth over and above bully victimization. Findings underline the importance of assessing ethnic harassment when conducting research in ethnically diverse settings.

(3)

Ethnic Harassment and Bully Victimization in Immigrant Adolescents Being humiliated by peers at school is a painful experience for many youths. For instance, youths can be physically assaulted, teased, or purposefully excluded from social

activities (Roland, 1998). Some youth can be teased about their ethnic background. Children can be teased about the color of their skin, their name, the food they eat, or the gods they worship (Eslea & Mukhtar, 2000). Ethnic harassment occurs when people are exposed to insults and derogatory comments and actions that target them specifically due to their ethnic background (Özdemir & Stattin, 2011b).

Although anyone can be teased due to his or her ethnic background, research indicates that ethnic minority and immigrant children are more likely to report experiencing this type of harassment than those belonging to the ethnic majority (Boulton, 1995; Monks, Ortega-Ruiz, & Rodríguez-Hidalgo, 2008; Moran, Smith, Thompson, & Whitney, 1993; Verkuyten & Thijs, 2002). In the Netherlands, between 33 and 42% of immigrant children report being called names due to the color of their skin (Verkuyten & Thijs, 2002). For Dutch children, the incidence of racist name-calling is 21% (Verkuyten & Thijs, 2002). Similarly, between 26 and 30% of immigrant children report being excluded from social activities due to their ethnic background (Verkuyten & Thijs, 2002). For Dutch children, the incidence of social exclusion is 19% (Verkuyten & Thijs, 2002). In the UK, 50% of bullied Asians report being victims of racial name-calling compared to 0% of bullied Whites (Moran et al., 1993). Asian children in the UK are also significantly more likely than White children to report being teased about their color or race (Boulton, 1995). In Spain and England, 38% of ethnic minority children report

experiencing racial name-calling compared to 5% of the ethnic majority group; and 13% of ethnic minority children report being victims of social exclusion compared to 7% of the ethnic

(4)

majority group (Monks et al., 2008). Thus, ethnic harassment, especially in the form of racial name-calling but also in the form of social exclusion, appears to be a common experience for many immigrant and ethnic minority youth.

Swedish society is becoming increasingly diverse. In 1998, around 50,000 people immigrated to Sweden and, in 2010, the figure doubled to 100,000 (Statistics Sweden [SCB], 2008). In 2011, 15% of the Swedish population was born abroad and, by 2060, this figure is expected to rise to 18% (SCB, 2012). As far as immigrant youth are concerned, it is estimated that 7% of children in Sweden under the age of 17 are born abroad, which amounts to

approximately 127,000 children (SCB, 2011a). Another growing figure is the number of people born in Sweden to foreign-born parents, sometimes called second-generation immigrants. In 2008, 11% of the population born in Sweden had at least one parent that was foreign-born (SCB, 2011a). Thus, taken together, first and second-generation immigrants currently comprise a significant number of the Swedish population.

Increased immigration and diversity present many challenges for Swedish society (SCB, 2008). Perhaps one of the most important challenges concerns the integration of immigrant children and youth into the Swedish school system. Since almost all immigrant children are introduced into the Swedish school system (SCB, 2011a), schools have a unique opportunity to influence immigrant youths’ development in a positive direction. Schools could be especially important for the development of immigrant children because school is the place where many immigrant children learn the Swedish language as well as the cultural ways and unwritten rules of Swedish society. If children have problems adjusting in school, it is possible that this could impact their future success and academic achievement.

(5)

One important threat to youths’ well being in school is bullying. Bullying is defined as aggressive behavior that is intentional, occurs repeatedly over a long period of time, and is characterized by a power difference between the aggressor and the victim (Olweus, 1994). The most commonly studied participant roles in bullying research are those of the bully, the victim, and the bully-victim (both bully and victim). However, some researchers have identified other participant roles such as assistant, reinforcer, defender, and outsider (Salmivalli, Lagerspetz, Björkqvist, Österman, & Kaukiainen, 1996). Forms of aggression considered as bullying include physical, verbal, and indirect aggression. Physical aggression includes behaviors such as hitting, kicking, beating, or assaulting; verbal aggression refers to insulting, teasing, mocking, or

humiliating; and indirect aggression refers to purposefully excluding someone from social activities or play.

It is estimated that 20 to 30% of children bully others or are victimized by bullies in schools across several developed countries (Gini & Pozzoli, 2009). In Nordic countries, about 5% of primary and secondary school students are bullied at least once a week (Roland, 1998). Also, in primary schools in the Nordics, the percentage of victims is higher for boys than girls and bully victimization tends to decrease with age (Roland, 1998).

Research suggests that bullying can have serious consequences for victims’ psychological health. The correlates of bully victimization are well documented and include depression,

loneliness, poor global and social self-worth, and generalized and social anxiety (Hawker & Boulton, 2000). Victims of bullying are also significantly more likely than children not involved in bullying to suffer from a variety of psychosomatic problems (Gini & Pozzoli, 2009) and to be left outside of social networks (Salmivalli, Huttunen, Lagerspetz, 1997). A study that used data from 48,000 students in seven countries concluded that victims were worse off than bullies and

(6)

neutrals in all the measures for all the samples (Eslea, Menesini, Morita, O’Moore, Mora-Merchán, Pereira, & Smith, 2003). Furthermore, research indicates that bully victimization can have significant long-term consequences for youth such as increasing depression, failure

expectation, and self-harm and decreasing self-esteem two years later (Özdemir & Stattin, 2011a).

There is a relatively large amount of research on bullying; however, we have a limited understanding of immigrant youths’ experiences of bullying and bully victimization. Studies on bullying in multiethnic contexts in Europe are limited and findings regarding the incidence of bullying are contradictory. Some studies have found that children belonging to ethnic minorities score higher on bullying and lower on victimization than children in the ethnic majority

(Fandrem, Strohmeier, & Roland, 2009; Vervoort, Scholte, & Overbeek, 2010). Other studies have found that children belonging to the ethnic majority are more often bullies and victims than immigrant children (Strohmeier & Spiel, 2003). Most studies, however, suggest that the

incidence of bullying does not vary significantly between ethnic groups (Boulton, 1995; Moran et al., 1993; Siann, Callaghan, Glissov, Lockhart, & Rawson, 1994; Wolke, Woods, Stanford, & Schultz, 2001; Monks et al., 2008). In short, the association between ethnicity and bullying is unclear and there is a need for further research in order to understand whether bully victimization is a risk for immigrant youth.

In addition to experiences with bullying and bully victimization, ethnic harassment could also be an important risk for immigrant youth. However, the correlates of ethnic harassment and its outcomes for immigrant youth are understudied. According to one study, youth who are victimized due to their ethnic origin have stronger and more negative momentary self-feelings than those who are victimized due to other personal characteristics (Verkuyten & Thijs, 2001).

(7)

Another study suggests that ethnic harassment is related to a less positive evaluation of the outgroup (Verkuyten, 2002). However, studies on ethnic harassment in immigrant populations in Europe are very few and the studies available focus on Turkish, Surinamese, and Moroccan minorities in the Netherlands. More studies are needed, especially longitudinal ones, to understand the correlates and outcomes of ethnic harassment for immigrant youth throughout Europe.

The Current Study

The purpose of this study was to examine the effects of ethnic harassment and bully victimization on the adjustment of immigrant adolescents in Sweden longitudinally. The first question we asked was: Is bully victimization a risk for immigrant youths’ adjustment? In order to answer this question, we assessed depression, poor self-esteem, self-harm, and bully

victimization at baseline and one year later. We tested whether victimization was a predictor of (1) adjustment outcomes one year later and (2) changes in adjustment outcomes from the time of the first data collection (T1) to the time of the second data collection (T2). The second question we asked was: Is ethnic harassment a risk for immigrant youths’ adjustment over and beyond bully victimization? Ethnic harassment was assessed through a scale created specifically for the project. Depression, poor self-esteem, self-harm, and ethnic harassment were measured at baseline and one year later. We examined whether ethnic harassment predicted (1) adjustment outcomes one year later and (2) changes in adjustment outcomes over a one-year period.

Method Participants

This study used data from the Seven Schools Study, a longitudinal study on students’ experiences inside and outside of school and their relationships with peers, teachers, and parents.

(8)

The study was conducted in a mid-sized city in central Sweden with a population of about

137,000 (SCB, 2011b). According to national statistics on children and youth aged 0 to 19 in this city, 25% had a foreign background (either born abroad or both parents were born abroad), 91.7% were born in Sweden, and 0.2% were born in Nordic countries excluding Sweden (SCB, 2010). The original study surveyed all students in grades 7 through 9 in seven public schools. Schools located in neighborhoods with relatively large immigrant populations were selected to ensure representation of immigrant youths in the sample. Data collection took place in the spring of every year during four consecutive years. The present study used the data collected during the first two waves.

The sample included 252 first and second-generation immigrant youth (52% female and 48% male). Forty-six percent of youth were born abroad and 54% were born in Sweden but had at least one parent born abroad. Youth born in Norway, Denmark, or Finland and youth who had parents coming from these countries were not included in this study. The ages of the participants ranged from 12 to 19 with a mean of 13.2 years (SD = .61) at the time of the first data collection (T1) and 14.98 years (SD = .75) at the second year of the study (T2). Seven percent of the baseline participants were missing at the time of the second data collection. The attrition sample was compared to the retained sample on all study variables measured at T1. We found no significant difference between the attrition sample and the retained sample suggesting that attrition was not likely to lead to biased findings.

Measures

Bully victimization. To assess bully victimization, participants were asked three questions regarding whether they had been subject to physical, verbal, or indirect aggression at school or on the way to or from school. This measure was originally developed for a

(9)

cross-national survey of bullying in Switzerland and Norway (Alsaker & Brunner, 1999). Youth responded on a 4-point scale ranging from never to several times a week. The questions were "Have you been mocked, teased in an unpleasant way, or has anyone said nasty things to you at school or on the way to or from school (this semester)?" "Have you been beaten, kicked, or assaulted in a nasty way by anyone at school or on the way to or from school (this semester)?" and "Sometimes one can be ostracized by someone or some people and not be allowed to hang out with them. Has this ever happened to you (this semester)?". The Cronbach's alpha for the bully victimization scale was .74.

Ethnic harassment. An ethnic harassment measure was created specifically for the project. The measure included 6 questions: "Has anyone said anything derogatory about your origin, for example words like nigger, darky, damned immigrant, inkface, ching-chong, or something else?" "Has anyone looked at you in a way that makes you certain they have a negative attitude towards you, just because you or your parents come from another country?" "Has anyone said that you are doing something wrong because you don't do it like Swedish people do?" "Has anyone treated you very differently from Swedes, because you come from another country?" "Have you been exposed to things that made you wish you didn't come from another country or made you feel ashamed you came from another country?" "Has anyone made fun of you in a derogatory way because you come from another country?" and "Have you ever been blamed for something that isn't true, only because you come from another country?" Participants answered on a 5-point scale ranging from never to daily. The Cronbach's alpha for the ethnic harassment scale was .77.

Depression symptoms. The child depression scale from the Center for Epidemiological Studies (CES-D; Radloff, 1977) was used to measure depressed mood. This is a 20-item

(10)

depression scale developed for both adults and children. We used all the children’s items and one adult item: “I was bothered by things that usually don’t bother me.” Examples of other items are "(During the last week I have:) felt that I am not as good as everybody else; felt depressed and unhappy; felt like I wanted to cry; felt sad;" etc. Youth responded on a 4-point scale ranging from not at all to often. The Cronbach's alpha for the depression scale was .91 at T1 and .94 at T2.

Poor self-esteem. Youths’ poor self-esteem was assessed through the Rosenberg's Self-Esteem Scale (Rosenberg, 1979). This scale includes 10 items such as “On the whole (you) are satisfied with (yourself)” “(You) feel that (you) have a number of good qualities” “(You) feel (you) do not have much to be proud of” and “All in all, (you) are inclined to think (you) are a failure”. Participants responded on a 4-point scale ranging from don’t agree at all to agree completely. To convert the original self-esteem scale to a poor self-esteem scale, values were recoded in the opposite direction so that high scores indicated poor self-esteem. The Cronbach's alpha for the poor self-esteem scale was .84 at T1 and .83 at T2.

Self-harm. To measure self-harm, the nine-item version of the Deliberate Self-Harm Inventory (DSHI-9) was used. This is a revised version of the original DSHI (Gratz, 2001) which has been adapted for adolescents by Lundh and colleagues (2007). In this scale, youth are

presented with nine different kinds of self-harming behaviors such as cutting, burning, carving, head-banging, and punching oneself and are asked to respond to questions regarding their deliberate engagement in these behaviors. The behaviors did not include suicidal attempts, overdosing, self-starvation, or poisoning. Responses were given on a 7-point scale ranging from never to more than five times. The Cronbach's alpha for the self-harm scale was .92 at T1 and .93 at T2.

(11)

Procedure

Youth responded to questions during two consecutive class sessions. Participation was voluntary and students could decide to terminate participation at any point during or after the data collection. Passive consent of the parents was used. During data collection, trained test leaders monitored the students and teachers were not present in the classrooms. The regional ethical review board approved the measures and the procedure.

Analyses

A factor analysis with Promax rotation was conducted to determine whether bully victimization and ethnic harassment were separate constructs. Multiple hierarchical regression analyses were used to test if bully victimization and ethnic harassment predicted levels of adjustment difficulties one year later and increases in adjustment difficulties from T1 to T2. Changes in explained variance (R²) and standardized regression coefficients were interpreted.

Results Descriptive Statistics

The means, standard deviations, ranges, and correlations for all the variables are presented in Table 1. Around 30% of the youths reported at least one bully victimization

experience, and 60% of immigrant youths experienced ethnic harassment in at least one occasion at Time 1. The associations between the study variables were in the expected direction.

Specifically, bully victimization was positively correlated with depressive symptoms and poor self-esteem. On the other hand, ethnic harassment was significantly correlated with all

adjustment outcomes. It was positively correlated with both depressive symptoms and self-harm, and positively correlated with poor self-esteem, but only at T2. As for the demographic

(12)

Table 1. Inter-item Correlations, Reliabilities, and Descriptive Statistics For the Study Variables

Note. Values on the diagonal for the measures (except age and sex) are Cronbach's alpha, inter-item reliability, values. *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10

1. Age -

2. Sex (0=boys, 1=girls) .04 -

3. Bully victimization - T1 .03 -.11* .74 4. Ethnic harassment - T1 .09 -.09 .14** .77 5. Depression symptoms - T1 .06 .27*** .12* .20*** .91 6. Depression symptoms - T2 .17** .19** .23*** .19** .46*** .94 7. Poor Self-esteem - T1 .02 .13* .23*** .09 .52*** .38*** .84 8. Poor Self-esteem - T2 .05 .02 .24*** .27*** .37*** .54*** .55*** .83 9. Self-harm - T1 .05 -.03 .01 .16** .15** .06 .27*** .20*** .92 10. Self-harm - T2 .13* -.12 -.02 .29*** .08 .30*** .11 .27*** .29*** .93 Means 14.5 .51 1.15 1.53 1.61 1.74 1.78 1.90 .17 .28 Standard deviations 1.09 .50 .36 .49 .54 .66 .56 .59 .62 .88 Minimum 12 0 1 1 1 1 1 1 0 0 Maximum 19 1 4 4 4 4 4 4 6 6

(13)

Gender was negatively associated with bully victimization and positively associated with poor self-esteem at T1. Gender was also positively associated with depressive symptoms suggesting that girls were more likely to display depressive symptoms than boys. The correlation between bully victimization and ethnic harassment was relatively low (r = .14, p < .01) lending additional support for the idea that these two constructs refer to distinct experiences. The factor structure for bully victimization and ethnic harassment items is presented in Table 2.

Table 2. Factor Structure of Bully Victimization and Ethnic Harassment Items

The effects of Bully Victimization on Youth Adjustment

Is bully victimization a risk for youth’s adjustment? To test this, multiple hierarchical regression analyses were conducted. The dependent variables were the level of the adjustment problem at T2 and the change in the adjustment outcome from T1 to T2. We controlled for age,

Factor 1 Factor 2 Bully victimization Item 1 .86 Item 2 .74 Item 3 .56 Ethnic harassment Item 1 .72 Item 2 .72 Item 3 .71 Item 4 .52 Item 5 .48 Item 6 .47 Factor correlation .18

(14)

gender, and the level of the adjustment problem at T1 by entering these variables in the first block of the regression model. In the second block, we entered bully victimization.

Victims of bullying were more depressed one year later and they also showed an increase in depression over the course of one year. We found that bully victimization predicted the level of depressive symptoms at T2 (β = .23, p < .001, see Table 3) as well as the change in depressive symptoms from T1 to T2 (β = .16, p < .01, see Table 4). Regarding poor self-esteem, adolescents who were victimized had lower self-esteem one year later (β = .22, p < .001); however,

victimization did not predict changes in poor self-esteem over the course of one year. Contrary to the effect of victimization on depressive symptoms and poor self-esteem, no significant correlations were found between bully victimization and self-harm.

Table 3. Hierarchical Multiple Regression Table Predicting Adjustment From Bully Victimization and Ethnic Harassment

Adjustment Problems at T2

Depression Poor Self-Esteem Self-Harm

Predictor ∆R² β ∆R² β ∆R² β Step 1 .07 .02 .03 Age .09 .01 .13 Gender .29*** .10 -.03 Step 2 .07*** .13*** .01 Victimization .23*** .22*** -.08 Step 3 Ethnic harassment .06*** .20** .04*** .25*** .10*** .33*** Total R² .18*** .19*** .13 N 224 222 224 *p<.05. **p<.01. ***p<.001.!

(15)

Table 4. Hierarchical Multiple Regression Table Predicting Change in Adjustment Problems From Bully Victimization and Ethnic Harassment

Change in Adjustment Problems

Depression Poor Self-Esteem Self-Harm

Predictor ∆R² β ∆R² β ∆R² β Step 1 .27*** .31*** .19*** Adjustment problem at T1 .40*** .52***! .24*** Age .08 .06 .14 Gender .19** .05 .00 Step 2 .03*** .01 .00 Victimization .16** .09 -.05 Step 3 Ethnic harassment .04** .12* .05*** .19*** .04** .22*** Total R² .34*** .36*** .20*** N 221 215 216 *p<.05. **p<.01. ***p<.001

The Effects of Ethnic Harassment on Youth Adjustment

Is ethnic harassment a risk for immigrant youth’s adjustment, over and beyond the effects of bully victimization? To test this, we entered ethnic harassment as a predictor variable in the third block of the regression model.

The results suggested that immigrant youth who experienced ethnic harassment were more likely to display higher depressive symptoms one year later (β = .20, p < .01) and they also showed increases in depression over one year (β = .12, p < .05), even after controlling for bully victimization. Next, ethnic harassment predicted poor self-esteem one year later (β = .25, p < .001) as well as increases in poor self-esteem over the course of one year (β = .19, p < .001),

(16)

even after controlling for victimization. Contrary to the effects of bully victimization, youths who experienced ethnic harassment displayed higher levels of deliberate self-harm behaviors (β = .33, p < .001). In addition, these youths also displayed an increase in self-harm behaviors over a one-year period (β = .22, p < .001).

In short, results indicated that both bully victimization and ethnic harassment were risk factors for youths’ adjustment. Bully victimization predicted depression and poor self-esteem scores as well as increases in depression over the course of one year. Ethnic harassment predicted depression, poor self-esteem, and self-harm scores as well as increases in these three outcomes over the course of one year for immigrant youths, suggesting that ethnic harassment experiences pose an added risk to the adjustment of immigrant youths over and above the negative consequences of bully victimization.

Discussion

Being repeatedly teased at school is a difficult experience for many youths. The goal of this study is to focus on immigrant youth in Sweden and how being teased by their peers affects their well being. Specifically, we extend the research on bullying by focusing on immigrant youths, by comparing bully victimization and ethnic harassment, and by conducting a

longitudinal study. Past research in the area of bully victimization in multiethnic contexts has focused on comparing the incidence of bullying and bully victimization between ethnic groups (Boulton, 1995; Fandrem, Strohmeier, & Roland, 2009; Monks et al., 2008; Moran et al., 1993; Siann, Callaghan, Glissov, Lockhart, & Rawson, 1994; Strohmeier & Spiel, 2003; Vervoort, Scholte, & Overbeek, 2010; Wolke, Woods, Stanford, & Schultz, 2001). To date, no study has examined the effects of bully victimization and ethnic harassment simultaneously. Our findings

(17)

indicate that ethnic harassment is even more important than bully victimization in predicting immigrant youths’ adjustment problems.

This study adds to the literature on bully victimization and ethnic harassment in several ways. First, our study suggests that bully victimization and ethnic harassment are separate constructs. Second, this study sheds light on the incidence of bully victimization and ethnic harassment among immigrant groups in Northern Europe, and specifically in Sweden. Third, our study indicates that bully victimization is a risk factor for immigrant youths’ adjustment,

especially when it comes to depression. Finally, results suggest that ethnic harassment is a unique risk for immigrant youths’ adjustment, over and beyond bully victimization.

According to our results, ethnic harassment is even more detrimental to youths’ well being than bully victimization. Immigrant youths who experience ethnic harassment show higher levels of adjustment problems across all three outcomes examined as well as an increase in these problems over the course of one year. The reason for this is not known, but some researchers point to a possible link between ethnic harassment and ethnic identity (Özdemir & Stattin, 2011b). Research in the area of social identity suggests that threats to one’s ethnic identity lead to lower self-esteem about one’s ethnic group (Ethier & Deaux, 1994). Ethier and Deaux report that individuals with a weaker ethnic identity perceive more threat in the environment than those with a stronger ethnic identity and show decreases in group self-esteem, ultimately leading to a lower identification with their ethnic group. Perhaps the strength of one’s ethnic identity could also be related to the way in which ethnic harassment is perceived. Furthermore, it is possible that ethnic harassment could lead to a lower identification with one’s ethnic group, and that this could negatively affect one’s feelings about oneself. The process by which ethnic harassment affects one’s social and personal identity is a topic for future research. Our findings underline the

(18)

importance of assessing ethnic harassment when conducting research in ethnically diverse settings.

Limitations and Strengths

One of the limitations of this study is that all immigrant youth, excluding those from Nordic countries, are grouped into one category. We are aware that immigrants are not a

homogeneous group; they differ in important and relevant ways other than their country of origin such as their reason for immigration, length of stay in Sweden, level of education, etc. However, in Sweden, there may be over 100 nationalities represented in a single school, but only a few students from each country. This makes it difficult to use meaningful subcategories that are at the same time large enough to withstand statistical analyses. Future research could focus on selecting a sample of youth from particular countries. For instance, studies could focus specifically on immigrants from Finland, Iraq, Poland, or the former Yugoslavia since they comprise some of the largest immigrant groups in Sweden.

Our study leaves some questions unanswered such as: why are immigrant or ethnic minority youth more likely to become victims of ethnic harassment? What are the protective factors for ethnic harassment? Research indicates that having friends and being liked by peers are protective factors for bully victimization (Pellegrini, Bartini, & Brooks, 1999) and that the

number of reciprocal friendships buffers victimization (Pellegrini & Long, 2002). Perhaps being liked by peers and having reciprocal friends could also be protective factors for ethnic

harassment. More research is needed to understand why ethnic harassment affects immigrant youth and what factors could serve to help individuals in the face of ethnic harassment.

Despite these limitations, our study has several strengths. One of the strengths is the longitudinal design. This allowed us to examine changes in adjustment outcomes over time and

(19)

whether bullying and ethnic harassment predicted these changes. Another strength is that we used a 6-item ethnic harassment scale that included, but was not limited to, questions on racial name-calling and social exclusion. Previous studies on ethnic harassment have focused exclusively on racial name-calling and/or social exclusion, but research suggests that ethnic harassment can also occur in other ways such as being teased about one’s name, clothing, or the gods one worships (Eslea & Mukhtar, 2000). Thus, it is important for ethnic harassment

measures to focus on the experience of being a victim of aggressive behavior due to one’s ethnic origin, regardless of whether the aggression is physical, verbal, or indirect and regardless of the particular content of the teasing (teasing about one’s skin color, clothing, name, gods, food, etc.). Finally, another strength is that we included a relatively large percentage of first and second-generation immigrant youth.

Conclusion

For immigrant youths, being teased or left out because they are born in another country or have parents that are born in another country can be very hurtful. Being a victim of ethnic

harassment can lead some youths to feel unsatisfied, lonely, unhappy, or even to cause

themselves bodily harm. Ethnic harassment is a real threat to immigrant youths’ well being and should therefore be taken into account when studying bullying and bully victimization among youths in ethnically diverse settings. Examining the causes and consequences of ethnic

harassment is important not only to improve our understanding of the experiences of immigrant and ethnic minority youth, but also to design effective interventions to prevent all kinds of victimization in an increasingly diverse world.

(20)

References

Alsaker, F. D., & Brunner, A. (1999). Switzerland. In Smith P.K., Morita, Y., Junger-Tas, J., Olweus, D., Catalano, R., Slee, P. (Eds.), The Nature of School Bullying: A

Cross-National Perspective (pp. 250-263). London: Routledge.

Boulton, M. J. (1995). Patterns of bully/victim problems in mixed race groups of children. Social Development, 4(3), 277–293.

Eslea, M., Menesini, E., Morita, Y., O’Moore, M., Mora-Merchán, J. A., Pereira, B., Smith, P. K. (2003). Friendship and loneliness among bullies and victims: Data from seven countries. Aggressive Behavior, 30, 71–83. doi:10.1002/ab.20006

Eslea, M., & Mukhtar, K. (2000). Bullying and racism among Asian schoolchildren in Britain. Educational Research, 42, 201-217. doi: 10.1080/001318800363845

Ethier, K. A., & Deaux, K. (1994). Negotiating social identity when contexts change:

Maintaining identification and responding to threat. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67(2), 243-251. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.67.2.243

Fandrem, H., Strohmeier, D., & Roland, E. (2009). Bullying and victimization among native and immigrant adolescents in Norway: The role of proactive and reactive

aggressiveness. The Journal of Early Adolescence, 29(6), 898-923. doi:10.1177/0272431609332935

Gini, G. & Pozzoli, T. (2009). Association between bullying and psychosomatic problems: A meta-analysis. Pediatrics, 123(3), 1059-1065. doi: 10.1542/peds.2008-1215

Gratz, K. L. (2001). Measurement of deliberate self-harm: Preliminary data on the deliberate self-harm inventory. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 23(4), 253-263. doi:10.1023/A:1012779403943

(21)

Hawker, D. S. J., & Boulton, M. J. (2000). Twenty years research on peer victimization and psychosocial maladjustment: A meta-analytic review of cross-sectional studies. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41(4), 441-455. doi:10.1111/1469-7610.00629 Lundh, L., Karim, J., & Quilisch, E. (2007). Deliberate self-harm in 15-year-old adolescents: A

pilot study with a modified version of the deliberate self-harm inventory. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 48(1), 33-41. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9450.2007.00567.x

Monks, C., Ortega-Ruiz, R., & Rodríguez-Hidalgo, A.J. (2008). Peer victimization in multicultural schools in Spain and England. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 5, 507-535. doi: 10.1080/17405620701307316

Moran, S., Smith, P. K., Thompson, D., & Whitney, I. (1993). Ethnic differences in experiences of bullying: Asian and white children. The British Journal of Educational Psychology, 63, 431–440.

Olweus, D. (1994). Annotation: Bullying at school: Basic facts and effects of a school-based intervention program. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 35, 1171–1190. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1994.tb01229.x

Pellegrini, A. D., Bartini, M., & Brooks, F. (1999). School bullies, victims and aggressive victims: Factors relating to group affiliation and victimization in early adolescents. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 216–224.

Pellegrini, A. D., & Long, J. A. (2002). A longitudinal study of bullying, dominance, and victimization during the transition from primary to secondary school. The British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 20, 259–280. doi:10.1348/ 026151002166442 Radloff, L. S. (1977). The CES-D scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general

(22)

population. Applied Psychological Measurement, 1(3), 385-401. doi:10.1177/014662167700100306

Roland, E. (1998). School influences on bullying (Doctoral dissertation, Durham University). Retrieved from http://etheses.dur.ac.uk/1047/

Rosenberg, M. (1979). Conceiving the Self. New York: Basic Books.

Salmivalli, C., Huttunen, A., & Lagerspetz, K. (1997). Peer networks and bullying in schools. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 38, 305–312.

Salmivalli, C., Lagerspetz, K. M., Björkqvist, K., Österman, K., & Kaukiainen, A. (1996). Bullying as a group process: Participant roles and their relations to social status within the group. Aggressive Behaviour, 22, 1–15.

Siann, G., Callaghan, M., Glissov, P., Lockhart, R., & Rawson, L. (1994). Who gets bullied? The effect of school, gender and ethnic group. Educational Research, 36, 123 – 134. Statistics Sweden. (2008). Integration – a description of the situation in Sweden. Retrieved

from http://www.scb.se/statistik/_publikationer/LE0105_2008A01_BR_BE57BR0801 .pdf

Statistics Sweden. (2010). Registret över totalbefolkningen (RTB): AA- Registerdata för

Integration: Statistik med inriktning mot demografi. Fördelning efter kommun och ålder. År 1997-2010. Retrieved from http://www.ssd.scb.se/databaser/makro/visavar.asp?yp= duwird&xu=c5587001&lang=1&langdb=1&Fromwhere=S&omradekod=AA&huvudtabe ll=IntGr3Kom&innehall=Int5&prodid=AA0003&deltabell=K2&fromSok=&preskat=O Statistics Sweden. (2011a). Integration – foreign born persons in upper secondary school.

Retrieved from http://www.scb.se/statistik/_publikationer/LE0105_2011A01_BR_BE57 BR1101.pdf

(23)

Statistics Sweden. (2011b). Folkmängd i riket, län och kommuner 31 december 2011 och befolkningsförändringar 2011. Retrieved from http://www.scb.se/Pages/TableAndChart ____308468.aspx

Statisics Sweden. (2012). The future population of Sweden 2012-2060. Retrieved from

http://www.scb.se/Pages/PublishingCalendarViewInfo____259923.aspx?PublObjId=165 92

Strohmeier, D., & Spiel, C. (2003). Immigrant children in Austria: Aggressive behavior and friendship patterns in multicultural school classes. Journal of Applied School

Psychology, 19, 99–116.

Verkuyten, M. (2002). Ethnic attitudes among minority and majority children: The role of ethnic identification, peer group victimization and parents. Social Development, 11(4),

558-570. doi:10.1111/1467-9507.00215

Verkuyten, M., & Thijs, J. (2001). Peer victimization and self-esteem of ethnic minority group children. Journal of Community & Applied Social Psychology, 11(3), 227-234.

doi:10.1002/casp.628

Verkuyten, M., & Thijs, J. (2002). Racist victimization among children in The Netherlands: The effect of ethnic group and school. Ethnic and Racial Studies, 25, 310–331. doi:10.1080/ 01419870120109502

Vervoort, M.H.M., Scholte, R.H.J., Overbeek, G. (2010). Bullying and victimization among adolescents: The role of ethnicity and ethnic composition of school class. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 39, 1-11. doi: 10.1007/s10964-008-9355-y

Wolke, D., Woods, S., Stanford, K., & Schulz, H. (2001). Bullying and victimization of primary school children in England and Germany: Prevalence and school factors. The British

(24)

Journal of Psychology, 92, 673–696. doi:10.1348/000712601162419

Özdemir, M., & Stattin, H. (2011a). Bullies, victims, and bully-victims: A longitudinal examination of the effects of bullying-victimization experiences on youth well-being. Journal of Aggression, Conflict and Peace Research, 3(2), 97-102.

http://search.proquest.com/docview/882110252?accountid=8028

Özdemir, M., & Stattin, H. (2011b). The role of ethnic harassment and bully victimization in immigrant adolescents’ adjustment problems. Paper presented at the International Association for Cross-Cultural Psychology (IACCP) conference on June 30th - July 3rd in Istanbul.

References

Related documents

Based on prior research suggesting that sibling relationships can influence mother-youth relationships (Bank &amp; Burraston, 2001; Brody, 2004; Uppdegraff et al., 2005b), the

In the controls (without plugs) after 5 days, newly regener- ated bone marrow-like tissue fi lled the emptied marrow space, and cells producing extracellular matrix could be seen

Här avses den kompetens (formell och faktisk) som en individ innehar och som hon/han omsätter och använder i praktiken. Med arbetet i fokus, kan yrkeskunnande avse

Cnattingius ikonografiska överblick av Hörbergs motiv Nedtagandet från korset är, som nämnt, mycket kortfattad och om man inte är familjär med bibeltexterna eller den teologi

However, when attendance was linked to contextual and environmental factors, it became evident that during associative play preschoolers with hyperactivity were observed in

DB är också positiva till offentlig vård men är oroliga för att övriga samhället ska få betala kostnaden som uppstår när individer som nu får tillgång till en

Det som kännetecknar erfarenheten hos de tre nämnda grupperna, såsom de avspeglas i Mellanförskapets bidrag till an- tologin, är en känsla av att vara icke-autentisk, av att inte

Mutual flirtation, romance or consenting sexual relations between, for example, a teacher and a student or a manager and an employee are not legally considered to be