• No results found

The role of school climate in explaining changes in social trust over time

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "The role of school climate in explaining changes in social trust over time"

Copied!
15
0
0

Loading.... (view fulltext now)

Full text

(1)

http://www.diva-portal.org

This is the published version of a paper published in Scandinavian Journal of Educational

Research.

Citation for the original published paper (version of record): Lundberg, E., Abdelzadeh, A. (2019)

The role of school climate in explaining changes in social trust over time

Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research, 63(5): 712-724

https://doi.org/10.1080/00313831.2018.1434824

Access to the published version may require subscription. N.B. When citing this work, cite the original published paper.

Permanent link to this version:

(2)

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at

https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=csje20

ISSN: 0031-3831 (Print) 1470-1170 (Online) Journal homepage: https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/csje20

The Role of School Climate in Explaining Changes

in Social Trust Over Time

Erik Lundberg & Ali Abdelzadeh

To cite this article: Erik Lundberg & Ali Abdelzadeh (2019) The Role of School Climate in Explaining Changes in Social Trust Over Time, Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research, 63:5, 712-724, DOI: 10.1080/00313831.2018.1434824

To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/00313831.2018.1434824

© 2018 The Author(s). Published by Informa UK Limited, trading as Taylor & Francis Group

Published online: 01 Mar 2018.

Submit your article to this journal

Article views: 819

View Crossmark data

(3)

The Role of School Climate in Explaining Changes in Social Trust

Over Time

Erik Lundberg aand Ali Abdelzadehb

a

School of Education, Health and Social Studies, Dalarna University, Falun, Sweden;bSchool of Humanities, Education and Social Sciences, Örebro University, Örebro, Sweden

ABSTRACT

School is often ranked highly among social agents that are central to the development of various citizenship values and norms in younger members of society. In this paper, we examined the impact of two dimensions of school climate on changes in social trust among adolescents over time, namely relationships and safety. Using a series of latent change models on 3-wave panel data concerning roughly 850 Swedish adolescents aged 16 to 18 years, we found that experiences of victimization in school mattered most in predicting changes in social trust across 3 time points, when relevant demographic factors and other aspects of the school climate were controlled. In particular, social trust declined most among adolescents who experienced more victimization at school.

ARTICLE HISTORY

Received 10 January 2017 Accepted 22 January 2018

KEYWORDS

Social trust; school climate; adolescents; teachers; victimization

One civic competency that has garnered much attention in the social sciences in recent decades as an invaluable resource and asset for individuals and society at large is social trust, defined as confidence in the belief that strangers– that is, fellow citizens about whom we have no specific information – will not take advantage of us. Research has shown, for example, that in societies with high levels of trust, people have better health and show a greater tolerance of differences (Putnam,2000; Uslaner,

2002). Social trust is moreover associated with various positive social behaviors, including partici-pation in public affairs, volunteering, and caregiving.

In turn, the importance of social trust has motivated scholars to identify its sources. With respect to adolescents, schools have been underscored as an important means of boosting young people’s trust in others, owing to their role in promoting democratic dispositions and feelings of membership in a collective body (Amnå,2012; Ehman,1980; Englund,2009; Tschannen-Moran & Hoy, 2000; Van Maele, Forsyth, & Van Houtte,2014). Empirical studies have also pointed out the importance of various factors related to the school climate in the development of social trust. Among them, Fla-nagan and Stout (2010) found that students’ perceptions of solidarity at school influence their

devel-opment of social trust across time. That same year, Flanagan, Stoppa, Syvertsen, and Stout (2010) argued that trusting relationships with teachers are related to social trust and, later, other scholars stressed the importance of school climate to social trust as well (Gunnarson & Loxbo,2012).

At the same time, others question the role of school by arguing that schools do not have much influence in the socialization process. For example, using a multivariate multilevel regression model, Isac, Maslowski, Creemers, and van der Werf (2014) concluded that schools only have a

© 2018 The Author(s). Published by Informa UK Limited, trading as Taylor & Francis Group

This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives License (http:// creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/), which permits non-commercial re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited, and is not altered, transformed, or built upon in any way.

CONTACT Erik Lundberg elb@du.se School of Education, Health and Social Studies, Dalarna University, SE-791 22 Falun, Sweden

This article was originally published with errors. This version has been amended. Please see Corrigendum (http://dx.doi.org/10. 1080/00313831.2018.1456035)

SCANDINAVIAN JOURNAL OF EDUCATIONAL RESEARCH 2019, VOL. 63, NO. 5, 712–724

(4)

limited impact on civic-related competences of students, especially on their attitudes toward citizen-ship and intended political and social participation. According to their findings, students’ civic com-petences were instead strongly influenced by individual characteristics, such as motivations, and to some extent by influences outside the school, for example from family and peers.

Despite previous research, however, the extent to which school plays a role in generating social trust among adolescents remains limited. Although previous studies have contributed greatly to cur-rent understandings of that dynamic, most studies – a notable exception is Flanagan and Stout (2010) – have been cross-sectional and therefore cannot provide opportunities to examine what explains changes in trust over extended periods (see also Marien, 2017). Consequently, the call for studies on the dynamics in linkages between trust and schools– for example, between teachers and students and between students– is highly relevant (Tschannen-Moran & Hoy,2000, p. 585).

In this paper, we contribute to the current discussion on factors essential to the formation of social trust in three ways. First, we contribute to research on the topic by examining the importance of two dimensions of a school’s climate: relationships (e.g. the role of fair and responsive teachers) and safety (e.g. students’ experience with victimization). Second, we examine the role of these school climate dimensions across time. By drawing on a set of longitudinal data, we contribute to knowledge of pat-terns in and explanations of changes in social trust among adolescents. Since most empirical research on social trust has been conducted on adults, few studies have analyzed how young people form social trust. That shortcoming is unfortunate, since adolescence is considered to be a formative, critical period for the emergence and formation of social trust (Hooghe & Wilkenfeld,2008; Stolle & Hooghe,2004). Third, we add to the theoretical discussion regarding the extent to which social trust is a product of experiences that change constantly in response to changing circumstances or so-called “sticky” phenomenon learned in early childhood that stabilizes over time (Bauer,2014; Dinesen,2012).

More specifically, we addressed the research question of what role young people’s own experi-ences with school climate play in explaining potential changes in social trust among adolescents over time. Given the acknowledged importance of social trust, empirical studies on the factors that influence it are vital. Furthermore, given the unique position of school among members of the younger generation, research on how to build trust or prevent its depletion is deemed highly sig-nificant among scholars and practitioners alike.

To answer the research question, we conducted a series of individual-level analyses using a three-wave longitudinal data representing adolescents over a three-year period. We collected data in Swe-den, whose citizens are known to exhibit high levels of trust and endorse extensive welfare-state pro-visions (Kumlin & Rothstein,2005). As such, Sweden provided us with a suitable point of departure for analyzing the role of school climate in the development of social trust.

The paper is structured as follows. After this introductory section, we present our theoretical points of departure, first by describing two divergent perspectives on the nature and character of social trust and, thereafter, by drawing on two dimensions of school climate outlined above. We next explain the research design by describing our methods and research strategy, after which we present our results, namely by describing the roles of three factors outlined in the theoretical section in the generation of social trust. In conclusion, we summarize the results and discuss their strengths, weaknesses, and implications for research.

1. Theoretical Framework: The Role of School Climate in Explaining Changes in Adolescents’ Development of Social Trust

1.1. Divergent Perspectives on Social Trust

The general theoretical question that we sought to answer seeks to identify the nature and character of social trust. Generally, two divergent perspectives in the literature have gained particular attention (Abdelzadeh & Lundberg,2017; Bauer,2014; Uslaner, 2008). The first is the cultural perspective, which implies that social trust is“part of an enduring culture” and thus a relatively “sticky” factor

(5)

that becomes exceptionally stable over time (Dinesen,2012; Uslaner,2002). According to Eriksson (1950), infants develop a sense of basic trust in, or mistrust of, the external world by way of a con-sistent, dependable relationship with parents. According to such thinking, social trust is formed in the early (i.e. oral) stages of an infant’s upbringing and tends to persist throughout life. Social trust is additionally argued to relate closely to personality traits and genetic factors and thereby presumed to constitute a relatively persistent trait that changes only slowly with experiences later in life (Dinesen, Nørgaard, & Klemmensen,2014; Oskarsson, Dinesen, Dawes, Johannesson, & Magnusson,2016).

By contrast, the experiential perspective states that social trust is not so much a social fact or per-sonality trait as a product of our environment and experiences– one that we modify in response to changing circumstances (Laurence,2015; Putnam,2000). According to Hardin (1992), individuals continually modify and update their trustful and distrustful attitudes and feelings due to changes in society. That view has been dubbed the lifelong openness model, which stipulates that attitudes remain open to influence during life and that individuals frequently modify attitudes in response to new information and experiences (Sears & Brown,2013).

1.2. School Climate

In relation to that fundamental debate on the nature and character of social trust, various factors have been deemed potential mechanisms of social trust. For adolescents, school represents an excep-tionally central social institution outside the family that could influence trust. In this paper, we there-fore examine two dimensions of school climate in the generation of social trust: relationships and safety. School climate has been referred to as “the quality and character of school life” and is based on patterns of people’s experiences at school and reflects norms, goals, values, interpersonal relationships, teaching and learning practices, and organizational structures (Cohen, McCabe, Michelli, & Pickeral,2009, p. 182).

Thus, school climate encompasses a wide range of dimensions and measurements (Kohl, Recchia, & Steffgen,2013). In a comprehensive review, Thapa, Cohen, Guffey, and Higgins-D’Alessandro (2013) identify five dimensions of school climate, namely: safety, relationships, teaching and learning, insti-tutional environment, and school improvement process. Similarly, Cohen et al. (2009) lists four dimensions: safety, teaching and learning, relationships, and school environmental. In this paper, we focus on relationships and safety. Each of these dimensions includes a variety of elements. Relation-ships concern elements related to how connected people feel to one another, the quality of the school collaboration, respect for diversity, parent participation, and peer norms. Likewise, safety includes physical elements, such as the existence of a crisis plan or feelings of safety, and social-emotional elements, such as students’ and adults’ attitudes, norms, and responses to bullying (Cohen, et al.,2009). As pointed out above, we focus on the role of student-teacher relationships or, to be precise, the role of fair and responsive teachers. With regards to safety, we draw attention to studentś experience with victimization at school. At this point, it is important to note that dimensions may overlap, and elements stressed in one dimension may relate to elements included in another. In this study, it is possible to view students’ experiences of victimization as an aspect of positive student-student relationships. However, we follow the literature on school climate more inclined to treat bullying and victimization as a part of safety than part of relationships (Cohen, et al.,2009; Thapa et al.,2013). 1.3. The Role of Fair and Responsive Teachers

The first dimension of school climate that relates to the generation of social trust concerns the stu-dent-teacher relationship. Teachers represent one of the first encounters in the radius of people out-side the family with whom adolescents have frequent, regular contact. Therefore, adolescents’ experiences of encounters with teachers could play a key role for their degree of trust in people who they do not know personally and have not met (Delhey, Newton, & Welzel,2011; Flanagan, Cumsille, Gill, & Gallay,2007, p. 423).

(6)

At least two mechanisms by which teachers might play a role in adolescents’ social trust have been highlighted in the literature. First, fair treatment, which has gained attention in literature on social trust, suggests that if representatives entrusted with responsibility for the common interest (e.g. tea-chers) are perceived to be honest and fair, then trust in society will develop (Freitag,2003, p. 2018; Rothstein & Stolle,2008). By contrast, such representatives perceived to be corrupt and unfair or who generate experiences of discrimination or injustice have a negative influence on the development of social trust.

A relatively large number of studies on classroom justice have highlighted the impact of teacher fairness on young people’s attitudes, beliefs, and behaviors. For example, studies have shown that tea-cher fairness is related to increased satisfaction with the instructor and compliance with class rules (e.g. Colquitt,2001), smaller risk of aggressive workplace behavior (e.g. Skarlicki & Folger,1997), lesser ten-dency to seek revenge (e.g. Bies & Tripp,1996), and increased political interest (Claes & Hooghe,

2008). Thus, teacher fairness appears to be associated with a large number of positive outcomes. However, empirical studies on the extent to which teachers’ fair treatment influences social trust are sparse, and findings on the topic point in various directions. Damico, Conway, and Bowman Damico (2000) found that students’ perceptions of teachers and school administrators as fair people positively affects their degree of trust. However, in a study on the role of experiences among immi-grants from Turkey, Pakistan, and the former Yugoslavia living in Denmark, Dinesen (2010) found little support that being discriminated against by teachers in school influences students’ sense of social trust. Given that most previous studies have been cross-sectional, the extent to which fair treat-ment influences trust cannot be ruled out. Thus, it is possible that when teachers treat their students fairly, students will develop social trust.

More specifically, we assumed that a higher level of fair teacher treatment relates to an increase in social trust over time. However, what is perceived to be fair has been debated in the literature (e.g. Leventhal,1976; Thibaut & Walker,1975). In our paper, we focus on non-instrumental (i.e. value-expressive) aspects of fairness– that is, that institutions can be perceived to be fair regardless of what decisions they eventually make.

Second, there are reasons to believe that the extent to which teachers signal responsiveness and care for their students plays a role in the generation of social trust. According to Flanagan et al. (2007), the kinds of interactions that teachers have with students convey messages about core prin-ciples of democracy. By giving signals about respect, care, and the importance of different views, tea-chers express their expectations that students treat one another with respect. Thereby, teatea-chers function as examples of how people should treat one another and“appreciate the reciprocal relation-ship between trust and trustworthiness” (Flanagan et al.,2010, p. 314). Those findings suggest that teachers perceived to be responsive to the views and wellbeing of fellow students have a positive influence on students’ generation of social trust.

1.4. The Role of Victimization

The second dimension of school climate that relates to the generation of social trust concerns safety. In research on school climate, there is a broad consensus about the fundamentals of school that allow them to be perceived as safe places both physically and social-emotionally (Cohen, et al.,2009; Thapa et al.,2013). In this paper, we look at a specific aspect of safety– namely, the extent to which indi-viduals experience verbal and physical bullying and victimization (cf. Kutsyuruba, Klinger, & Hus-sain,2015; Thapa et al.,2013).

We assumed that individuals generalize from experiences with specific others. Accordingly, ado-lescents’ experiences with victimization cause changes in the perception of unspecified people as threats instead of supporters, which has a negative influence on social trust (Bauer,2014, p. 3; Mac-millan,2001, p. 22). However, it is possible that individuals’ experiences with victimization direct their mistrust toward individuals who share the characteristics of their offenders – that is, people who resemble their bullies and harassers (cf. Bauer,2014, p. 2).

(7)

Although empirical research on the correlation between social trust and victimization is sparse, studies have found a significant correlation between violence and peer victimization and a lack of supportive norm structures and relationships in schools (Thapa et al.,2013). Furthermore, Brehm and Rahn (1997, p. 1009, 1016) reported that perceptions of safety in the home and neighborhood were a significant predictor of trust in strangers. Similarly, in one of the most comprehensive studies on social trust, Delhey and Newton (2003, p. 93) concluded that“social trust tends to be high among citizens who believe that there are few severe social conflicts and where the sense of public safety is high.” However, using change score analysis combined with matching, Bauer (2014) found no causal effect that is substantially strong and consistent across panel data waves. Thus, given the arguments and empirical evidence outlined above, it is possible that adolescents’ experiences with victimization in school negatively influence their generation of social trust.

2. Methods

2.1. Participants and Procedure

The current study makes use of three waves of questionnaires from a longitudinal project. This com-munity-based and cohort-sequential project is being carried out in a medium-sized Swedish city with a total population of about 135,000. The motive for choosing this specific city is related to its repre-sentativeness with regard to national demographic characteristics. According to official statistics (Statistics Sweden, 2012), the city was similar, in 2010 when the first data collection was performed, to the national average in annual mean income (234,058 Swedish Crowns/person, compared with 237,186 Swedish Crowns/person for the whole country), rate of unemployment (8.6%, compared with 8.0% for the whole country), and percentage of foreign-born residents (14.8%, compared with 14.7% for the whole country).

Three of seven senior high schools in the city were strategically selected to represent both public and private schools and students’ diverse socioeconomic and ethnic backgrounds. All first graders in the senior high schools were targeted in the study. This made for total of 1,052 individuals on the first measurement occasion. In the analytic sample, we only included respondents for whom we had data on social trust on at least two measurement occasions. The final analytic sample comprised 846 par-ticipants (50.7% girls; Mage= 16.60 at the first-time point), which means that 206 students were lost

through attrition between the three measurement points. To establish whether these students differ from those who participated at least at two-time points, we applied logistic regression analysis to all our independent variables (that is, sex, age, subjective socioeconomic status, immigrant status, and so forth). The only significant predictor of adolescent attrition was age. Youths with missing data were more likely to be older (OR = 1.30, P < 0.05). However, the Nagelkerke R2for the whole block of vari-ables was 0.03, indicating that attrition did not had a substantial impact on the results of the analyses. The respondents filled out the questionnaires during regular school hours in their classrooms. To meet the possible objection that the presence of teachers might influence the responses of students, we ensured that no teachers were in the classrooms during data collection. Trained research assist-ants distributed the questionnaires and informed the students that participation in the study was voluntary; the students were also assured of the confidentiality of their responses. No student was paid for participating in the study, but each class received a contribution to its class fund. The data were collected annually from 2010 to 2012.

2.2. Measures 2.2.1. Social trust

Our dependent variable, social trust, was measured using two items:“Most people can be trusted” and “Most people are fair and do not take advantage of you (Flanagan et al., 2010; Flanagan & Stout, 2010). Participants responded to the statements on a five-point scale, ranging from 1

(8)

(do not agree at all) to 5 (completely agree). Pearson’s correlations between these two items were .67, .69, and .72, at time points 1, 2, and 3, respectively.

2.2.2. School climate measures

The study focused on two dimensions of school climate: relationships and safety. Relationship con-cerned student-teacher relationship and is measured by two items: perception of fairness of teacher treatment and responsive teachers. Adolescents’ perceptions of fairness of teacher treatment were measured on the basis of agreement/disagreement with the following single statement:“Most of my teachers treat me fairly.” Furthermore, adolescents’ perceptions of teachers perceived as respon-sive to the views and wellbeing of students was measured by agreement/disagreement with two state-ments:“Most of my teachers listen to what I have to say” and “Most teachers are eager for their students to feel good.” The Pearson’s correlation between these two items was .53. The second dimension of school climate dealt with adolescents’ experience of victimization and was measured by asking adolescents whether they agreed/disagreed with the following three statements:“I feel alone and like a stranger in my class,” “There are class mates who harass me,” and “There are stu-dents in my class who bully me.” Cronbach’s alpha reliability for this scale was .73 at the first-time point. The response scales for the three dimensions of the school setting ranged from 1 (do not agree at all) to 5 (completely agree).

2.2.3. Sociodemographic characteristics

To consider, the effects of some other potentially important factors, we included a number of con-ventionally used control variables: age, gender, immigrant status, and subjective socioeconomic sta-tus. Gender was coded as 1 (girl) and 0 (boy). Immigrant status was measured as a dichotomous variable: 1 = immigrant – the adolescent or at least one of the parents born outside Sweden/the other Nordic countries and; 0 = Swedish– the adolescent or at least one of the parents born in Swe-den/the other Nordic countries). Subjective socioeconomic status was measured using the following five questions:“If you want things that cost a lot of money (e.g. a computer, skateboard, cell phone) can your parents afford to buy them if you want them?” “If you compare yourself with others in your class, do you have more or less money to buy things?” “Does your family have more or less money than other families where you are living?” “How often do you and your family go on vacation?” and “What are personal finances like in your family?” Cronbach’s alpha reliability for this scale was .81 at time point 1.

2.3. Analytic Strategy

To examine the roles voluntary associations and different dimensions of the school climate play in explaining the development of, or changes in, social trust among adolescents over time, we con-structed a series of latent change models (McArdle & Nesselroade,1994), using maximum likelihood estimation in Mplus 6 (Muthén & Muthén,1998–2010). These models, also known as latent growth curve models or latent curve analysis models (McArdle & Nesselroade, 1994), generate optimal means for studying changes over time. With longitudinal data, they focus mainly on patterns of growth and decline. More specifically, in the current study, growth or decline in social trust was con-sidered as a function of a latent intercept and a latent slope. The latent intercept reflects the average initial value of social trust at the start of the longitudinal change process. Put differently, the intercept shows the mean of social trust at the first time point. The latent slope indicates the average individual change rate (over the three measurement occasions). A positive mean value for the slope indicates that, on average, there is an increase in social trust over time, whereas a negative value indicates a decline. For this study, taken as a whole, latent growth curve analysis was a suitable statistical pro-cedure for investigating whether predictors from the voluntary associations and the school setting are able to explain the rate of change in social trust over time.

(9)

With regard to the latent growth curve analyses, two statistical clarifications are required. First, in order statistically to evaluate the models, we used the following goodness-of-fit indices (cf. Klein-baum & Klein,2010): Chi-square, the comparative fit index (CFI), the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA), and the standardized root mean squared residual (SRMR). Generally speaking, these fit indices are estimates of how well a theoretical model matches observed data. Thus, good model fit implies that the data are consistent with the assumptions of a hypothesized model. According to recommendations, a good fit to data is indicated by a non-significant chi-square, CFI equal to or higher than 0.95, RMSEA equal to or lower than .05, and SRMR equal to or lower than 0.08 (Hu & Bentler,1999).

Second, to handle missing data, we tested all our models using full information maximum like-lihood (FIML) estimation. By making use of all available data from every subject in a sample, FIML estimation provides more reliable standard errors than listwise deletion, pairwise deletion, or mean imputation (Little & Rubin,2014). The proportion of missing data values is examined by the

covari-ance“coverage” matrix in Mplus. The minimum acceptable covariance coverage value, for which

Mplus provides a robust estimate, is as low as 10% (Muthén & Muthén, 1998–2010). In this study, coverage was much higher, ranging from 60 to 89%.

Finally, to make sure that there was no multicollinearity between independent, variance inflation factor (VIF) and accompanying tolerance levels were assessed using a series of multiple regression analyses. Variance inflation factor values of 10 or higher and tolerance values below 0.1 indicate serious multicollinearity problems (cf. Field,2009). The results showed that the values for VIF ran-ged from 1.0 to 1.6, and the values of tolerance ranran-ged from 0.60 to 1.0, which indicate that there was no multicollinearity between the independent variables.

3. Results

3.1. Correlations and Descriptive Data

As a first step toward answering our questions about the roles of school climate in the development of social trust among adolescents we examined the correlations between the study variables. As shown inTable 1, there were negative and significant associations of age and immigrant background with social trust at the first time point. These indicated that younger adolescents were more trustful than older, and that Swedes had higher levels of trust than adolescents with parents born outside Sweden/the Nordic countries. Moreover, there was a positive correlation between socioeconomic sta-tus and social trust, indicating that adolescents with higher socioeconomic stasta-tus scored higher on social trust.

Moving to the school climate variables, we found positive and significant associations of fairness of teacher treatment and responsive and caring with social trust. Adolescents scoring high on per-ceptions of fairness and responsiveness scored high on social trust across the three waves. A negative and significant correlation was found between victimization and social trust, meaning that adoles-cents who reported experiences from victimization scored low on social trust.

As can be seen inTable 1, the means of social trust were stable, which was also reflected in high correlations between the trust scores across the three time points. Overall, these findings provide a first indication of the potential associations of the two dimensions of school climate– relationships and safety– with social trust.

3.2. Explaining Changes in Social Trust

In order to identify the determinants of individual differences in social trust across the three time points, we first estimated an unconditional growth model, in which our focus was solely on the factor loadings of the intercept and slope. Thus, in this model, all predictors were excluded. To specify this unconditional growth model, we constrained the factor loadings of the intercept to 1, and the factor

(10)

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10

1 Gender

2 Age .01

3 Socioeconomic staus −.17*** −.04

4 Immigrant background −.03 .11** .05

5 Fairness of teacher treatment .00 .02 .04 .05

6 Responsiveness and care −.02 −.01 .05 .02 .63***

7 Victimization −.01 .07* −.11** −.10** −.17*** −.08* 8 Social trust T1 −.04 −.15*** .08* −.14*** .19*** .23*** −.15*** 9 Social trust T2 −.04 −.03 .01 −.18*** .18*** .15*** −.04 .56*** 10 Social trust T3 .02 −.07 .05 −.24*** .12** .16*** .02 .50*** .59*** Mean .51 16.60 .00 .20 3.23 3.07 1.29 2.99 2.98 2.90 SD .50 .69 .75 .40 .64 .58 .47 .85 .86 .95 *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001. S C A N DI NA V IAN JOUR NA L OF E DUC A T ION A L RE SE A R C H 719

(11)

loadings of the slope to 0, 1, and 2, which indicated the time lags between the measurement points. As shown inTable 2, this linear model of social trust fitted the data well (X2[1] = 3.053, CFI = .996, SRMR = .015, RMSEA = .049). The mean of the intercept gives the level of social trust at the first time point. The mean of the slope factor is negative and significant (β = −.05, p < .05). This negative value indicates that, on average, there was a decline in social trust over time. The significant variances of the intercept and slope factors indicate that individuals differed in their latent social scores at the first time point, and also in their growth trajectories. Put differently, there was variability in social trust both initially (as shown by the intercept) and in change over time (as shown by the slope). All in all, these findings indicate decline in social trust over the three measurement points, and also reasonable variability around the average intercept and slope.

Given the significant intercept and slope variances, there was justification for adding variables to the unconditional model to explain the variation in individual trajectories. To this end, we estimated a series of conditional latent growth models, where each predictor was initially added separately to examine its predictive value, while not taking into account other influences. As shown inTable 3, the first four single predictor models include the sociodemographic variables. The results revealed that only age (β = −.17, p < .001) and immigrant background (β = −.17, p < .01) were significantly and negatively related to the intercept of social trust. However, immigrant background alone significantly and negatively predicted the slope (β = −.23, p < .01), which means that adolescents who themselves or at least one of their parents were born in Sweden/the Nordic countries decreased less in social trust than the adolescents with immigrant background.

Looking at school climate, we noted that all three predictors were significantly related to the inter-cept of social trust. Whereas, the perinter-ceptions that teachers were fair and cared about their students were related to higher levels of social trust at baseline, experiences from victimization was related to a lower level of social trust (β = −.18, p < .001). However, only victimization had a significant effect on the slope (β = .29, p < .01), indicating that experiences from victimization resulted in a higher decrease in social trust across the three waves. Put differently, the level of social trust decreased more over time among adolescents who experienced more victimization at school than those who experienced less victimization. The school climate variables accounted all together for about 11% of the variance in the intercept and about 10% of the variance in the slope of social trust across the three time points.

At a final step (estimating the full model), we included all the predictors in one model to inves-tigate the unique variance of each predictor in relation to the development of social trust while con-trolling for the others. This model showed a good fit (X2[11] = 21.162, CFI = .986, SRMR = .011, RMSEA = .033). Only two variables (immigrant background and perceived victimization) proved to have a continued, significant effect on the slope of social trust. The model in total explained

Table 2.Unconditional latent growth curve model of social trust.

Unconditional model Maximum likelihood estimates µα(Mean intercept) 3.01***

µβ(Mean slope) −.05**

Ψαα(Variance intercept) .44***

Ψββ(Variance slope) .06*

r (intercept and slope) −.16ns

X2

(df) 3.053 (1)ns

CFI .996

SRMR .015

RMSEA .049

Note: Entries are unstandardized maximum likelihood estimates from the latent growth curve analysis.

*p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.

(12)

about 16% of the variance in the intercept and about 13% of the variance in the slope of social trust across the three time points. Victimization alone accounted for about 8% of the variance in the slope of the social trust over time. To summarize, when taking into account all the independent variables at once, victimization was the strongest predictor of the changes in social trust across the three waves.

4. Conclusion and Discussion 4.1. Conclusion

We examined the impact of two dimensions of school climate– relationships and safety – on changes in social trust over time among adolescents. Using a series of latent change models on three-wave panel data representing roughly 850 Swedish adolescents, we found that neither fairness of teacher treatment nor responsiveness among teachers can explain changes in social trust over time. Instead, adolescents’ experiences with victimization at school mattered most in predicting changes in social trust across three time points when relevant demographic aspects were controlled for. In other words, our results indicated that social trust declined more among adolescents who experienced more victimization in school.

4.2. Discussion

Our study contributes to previous research on the relationship between school and social trust in different ways. First, our results confirm previous research that school climate is relevant in explain-ing social trust (Englund,2009; Flanagan et al.,2010). Second, in contrast to many other studies in the field, ours increases our knowledge about what explains changes in social trust over time. By focusing on two dimensions of school climate at the same time, we show that victimization is the strongest predictor of changes in social trust across the three waves. That result suggests that experi-ences with victimization in school reduce adolescents’ social trust, as consistent with a study by Fla-nagan and Stout (2010), who used longitudinal data to show that adolescents’ perception of school solidarity boosts social trust. Our finding might suggest that when such solidarity is compromised by experiences with victimization, social trust declines. To some degree, our finding is also in line with results from a Swedish study showing that experiences of being offended by individuals and govern-mental agencies negatively correlates with trust in other people (Grosse,2012). Consequently, given the importance of social trust for individuals and society at large, our results provide important information for governmental institutions aiming to uphold or increase social trust among young people through various programs and interventions aiming at reducing victimization and foster without supportive norms, structures, and relationships among students.

Table 3.Conditional latent growth curve model of social trust.

Single predictor Full model Intercept Slope Intercept Slope Time-invariant covariates β SE β SE β SE β SE Sociodemographic characteristics Gender −.06 .05 .08 .07 −.04 .04 .08 .07 Age −.17*** .04 .13 .07 −.13** .04 0.12 .07 Socioeconomic status .09* .05 −.06 .07 .06 .04 .00 .07 Immigrant background −.17*** .05 −.23** .08 −.16*** .04 −.18* .07 School climate Fairness of teacher treatment .25*** .05 −.11 .08 .11 .06 .02 .08 Responsiveness and care .27*** .04 −.13 .07 .18** .05 −.12 .09 Victimization −.18*** .05 .29** .09 −.15** .04 .24** .08 Note: Entries for covariates are standardized maximum likelihood estimates and standard errors.

*p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.

(13)

Our study furthermore poses some important theoretical consequences for research on social trust. For one, our findings support the notion that social trust remains open to influences that we modify in response to experiences (Dinesen,2012; Laurence,2015). Such thinking contradicts findings showing no effects of negative experiences on social trust (Bauer,2014; van Ingen & Bek-kers,2015). Notably, our results draw attention to adolescence, which represents a period of dramatic changes when political values and beliefs are shaped (Niemi & Hepburn,1995; Sears & Brown,2013, p. 75). Thus, somewhat mixed results on the nature and character of social trust in different studies might be explained by life cycle effects (Visser & Krosnick,1998). Clearly, more research is needed to understand the nature and character of social trust, in general, and the dynamics in the links between trust and schools in particular, for example, additional research is needed that takes into account factors related to the school climate, including teacher-student and student-student relationships.

Our study has some limitations and strengths that warrant attention. One potential limitation concerns whether findings from our sample can be generalized to a wider population. As noted ear-lier, the data used in our study come from a community-based sample recruited from a Swedish city. Arguably, the findings therefore cannot be generalized from this context. In other words, it could be inherently difficult to know to what extent findings can be generalized to other institutional settings. At the same time, the research project from which the data in our study were obtained employed a based approach. Given the longitudinal design of the project, that kind of a community-based approach is more suitable and practically feasible.

The data used in our study also seem to be representative of the general population in terms of several relevant characteristics for the relevant age group and have adequate validity. In addition, there are few reasons to a priori assume that Swedish schools are atypical compared with other school contexts (Ainley, Schulz, & Friedman,2013). Thus, there are good reasons to believe that pat-terns of results regarding the aspects tested here on changes in social trust might be similar in various educational and political settings.

Our study also has several strengths. First, by adopting a longitudinal design, we pointed out the added value of using longitudinal data to explain the development of social trust over time. A second strength concerns the focus on adolescents instead of adults. In so doing, we could study social trust during a time when it is probably most vulnerable to external influences, including experiences with victimization. Lastly, by focusing on different aspects of the school climate and controlling for other relevant factors, we gained a better understanding of which factor is most important in the develop-ment of social trust. Such knowledge is vital for teachers and other school personnel to boost social trust. Based on the insights of this study, future research should pay more attention to the role of peer victimization in school on the development of social trust.

Acknowledgments

This study was made possible by access to data from the Political Socialization Program, a longitudinal research pro-gram at Youth & Society at Örebro University, Sweden. Responsible for the planning, implementation, and financing of the collection of data in this project were Professors Erik Amnå, Mats Ekström, Margaret Kerr, and Håkan Stattin. The authors of this article would like to thank the anonymous reviewers for valuable comments on earlier drafts.

Disclosure Statement

No potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors.

Funding

The data collection was supported by grants from the Bank of Sweden Tercentenary Foundation (grant number M2008-0073:1-PK).

(14)

ORCID

Erik Lundberg http://orcid.org/0000-0002-0627-1245

References

Abdelzadeh, A., & Lundberg, E. (2017). Solid or flexible? Social trust from early adolescence to young adulthood. Scandinavian Political Studies, 40(2), 207–227.doi:10.1111/1467-9477.12080

Ainley, J., Schulz, W., & Friedman, T. (Eds.). (2013). ICCS 2009 Encyclopedia. Amsterdam: International Association for the Evaluation of Educational Achievement.

Amnå, E. (2012). How is civic engagement developed over time? Emerging answers from a multidisciplinary field. Journal of Adolescence, 35(3), 611–627.doi:10.1016/j.adolescence.2012.04.011

Bauer, P. C. (2014). Negative experiences and trust: A causal analysis of the effects of victimization on generalized trust. European Sociological Review, 31(4), 397–417.doi:10.1093/esr/jcu096

Bies, R. J., & Tripp, T. M. (1996). Beyond distrust:“Getting even” and the need for revenge. In R. M. Kramer, & T. R. Tyler (Eds.), Trust in organizations (pp. 246–260). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Brehm, J., & Rahn, W. (1997). Individual-level evidence for the causes and consequences of social capital. American Journal of Political Science, 41(3), 999–1023.

Claes, E., & Hooghe, M. (2008, August). Citizenship education and political interest: Political interest as an intermediary variable in explaining the effects of citizenship education. Paper presented at the Conference on Civic Education and Political Participation, American Political Science Association, Boston.

Cohen, J., McCabe, L., Michelli, N. M., & Pickeral, T. (2009). School climate: Research, policy, practice, and teacher education. Teachers College Record, 111(1), 180–213.

Colquitt, J. A. (2001). On the dimensionality of organizational justice: A construct validation of a measure. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86(3), 386–400.doi:10.1037//0021-9010.86.3.386

Damico, A. J., Conway, M. M., & Bowman Damico, S. (2000). Patterns of political trust and mistrust: Three moments in the lives of democratic citizens. Polity, 32(3), 377–400.doi:10.2307/3235357

Delhey, J., & Newton, K. (2003). Who trusts? The origins of social trust in seven societies. European Societies, 5(2), 93– 137.doi:10.1080/1461669032000072256

Delhey, J., Newton, K., & Welzel, C. (2011). How general is trust in‘most people’? Solving the radius of trust problem. American Sociological Review, 76(5), 786–807.

Dinesen, P. T. (2010). Upbringing, early experiences of discrimination and social identity: Explaining generalised trust among immigrants in Denmark. Scandinavian Political Studies, 33(1), 93–111. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9477.2009. 00240.x

Dinesen, P. T. (2012). Does generalized (dis)trust travel? Examining the impact of cultural heritage and destination-country environment on trust of immigrants. Political Psychology, 33(4), 495–511.doi:10.1111/j.1467-9221.2012. 00886.x

Dinesen, P. T., Nørgaard, A. S., & Klemmensen, R. (2014). The civic personality: Personality and democratic citizen-ship. Political Studies, 62(suppl. 1), 134–152.doi:10.1111/1467-9248.12094

Ehman, L. H. (1980). The American school in the political socialization process. Review of Educational Research, 50(1), 99–119.doi:10.3102/00346543050001099

Englund, T. (2009). The general school system as a universal or a particular institution and its role in the formation of social capital. Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research, 53(1), 17–33.doi:10.1080/00313830802628307 Erikson, E. H. (1950). Childhood and society. New York & London: WW Norton & Company.

Field, A. (2009). Discovering statistics using SPSS: (And sex and drugs and rock‘n’ roll) (3rd ed.). Los Angeles, CA: Sage. Flanagan, C. A., Cumsille, P., Gill, S., & Gallay, L. S. (2007). School and community climates and civic commitments: Patterns for ethnic minority and majority students. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99(2), 421–431.doi:10.1037/ 0022-0663.99.2.421

Flanagan, C. A., & Stout, M. (2010). Developmental patterns of social trust between early and late adolescence: Age and school climate effects. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 20(3), 748–773.doi:10.1111/j.1532-7795.2010.00658.x Flanagan, C. A., Stoppa, T., Syvertsen, A. K., & Stout, M. (2010). Schools and social trust. In L. R. Sherrod, J.

Torney-Purta, & A. C. Flanagan (Eds.), Handbook of research on civic engagement in youth (pp. 307–330). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.

Freitag, M. (2003). Beyond Tocqueville: The origins of social capital in Switzerland. European Sociological Review, 19 (2), 217–232.doi:10.1093/esr/19.2.217

Grosse, J. (2012). Kommer tid kommer tillit? Unga vuxnas och medelålders erfarenheter [Trust from a life course per-spective: Young and middle-aged Swedes’ experiences]. Stockholm: Stockholm University.

Gunnarson, C., & Loxbo, K. (2012). School and the promotion of generalised trust: Experiences from Sicily. Journal of Trust Research, 2(2), 171–201.doi:10.1080/21515581.2012.708506

(15)

Hooghe, M., & Wilkenfeld, B. (2008). The stability of political attitudes and behaviors across adolescence and early adulthood: A comparison of survey data on adolescents and young adults in eight countries. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 37(2), 155–167.doi:10.1007/s10964-007-9199-x

Hu, L. T., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 6(1), 1–55. doi:10.1080/ 10705519909540118

van Ingen, E., & Bekkers, R. (2015). Generalized trust through civic engagement? Evidence from five national panel studies. Political Psychology, 35(3), 277–294.doi:10.1111/pops.12105

Isac, M. M., Maslowski, R., Creemers, B., & van der Werf, G. (2014). The contribution of schooling to secondary-school students’ citizenship outcomes across countries. School Effectiveness and School Improvement, 25(1), 29–63.doi:10. 1080/09243453.2012.751035

Kleinbaum, D. G., & Klein, M. (2010). Logistic regression: A self-learning text. New York, NY: Springer.

Kohl, D., Recchia, S., & Steffgen, G. (2013). Measuring school climate: An overview of measurement scales. Educational Research, 55(4), 411–426.doi:10.1080/00131881.2013.844944

Kumlin, S., & Rothstein, B. (2005). Making and breaking social capital: The impact of welfare-state institutions. Comparative Political Studies, 38(4), 339–365.doi:10.1177/0010414004273203

Kutsyuruba, B., Klinger, D. A., & Hussain, A. (2015). Relationships among school climate, school safety, and student achievement and well-being: A review of the literature. Review of Education, 3(2), 103–135.doi:10.1002/rev3.3043 Laurence, J. (2015). (Dis)placing trust: The long-term effects of job displacement on generalised trust over the adult

lifecourse. Social Science Research, 50, 46–59.doi:10.1016/j.ssresearch.2014.11.006

Leventhal, G. S. (1976). Fairness in social relationships. Morristown, NJ: General Learning Press.

Little, R. J., & Rubin, D. B. (2014). Statistical analysis with missing data. Cambridge, MA: John Wiley & Sons. Macmillan, R. (2001). Violence and the life course: The consequences of victimization for personal and social

devel-opment. Annual Review of Sociology, 27, 1–22.doi:10.1146/annurev.soc.27.1.1

Marien, S. (2017). Assessing the role of television, the family, and the school in the development of political trust in adolescence. Social Science Quarterly, 11(2), 818–835.doi:10.1111/ssqu.12433

McArdle, J. J., & Nesselroade, J. R. (1994). Using multivariate data to structure developmental change. In S. H. Cohen, & H. W. Reese (Eds.), Life-span developmental psychology: Methodological contributions (pp. 223–267). New York: Erlbaum, Hillsdale.

Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (1998–2010). Mplus user’s guide. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén.

Niemi, R. G., & Hepburn, M. A. (1995). The rebirth of political socialization. Perspectives on Political Science, 24(1), 7–16.doi:10.1080/10457097.1995.9941860

Oskarsson, S., Dinesen, T., Dawes, C. T., Johannesson, M., & Magnusson, K. E. P. (2016). Education and social trust: Testing a causal hypothesis using the discordant twin design. Political Psychology, 38(3), 515–531.doi:10.1111/pops. 12343

Putnam, R. D. (2000). Bowling alone: The collapse and revival of American community. New York: Simon & Schuster. Rothstein, B., & Stolle, D. (2008). The state and social capital: An institutional theory of generalized trust. Comparative

Politics, 40(4), 441–459.doi:10.5129/001041508X12911362383354

Sears, D. O., & Brown, C. (2013). Childhood and adult political development. In L. Huddy, D. O. Sears, & J. Levy (Eds.), Oxford handbook of political psychology (pp. 59–95). New York: Oxford University Press.

Skarlicki, D. P., & Folger, R. (1997). Retaliation in the workplace: The roles of distributive, procedural, and interac-tional justice. Journal of Applied Psychology, 82(3), 434–443.

Stolle, D., & Hooghe, M. (2004). The roots of social capital: Attitudinal and network mechanisms in the relation between youth and adult indicators of social capital. Acta Politica, 39(4), 422–441.doi:10.1057/palgrave.ap.5500081 Thapa, A., Cohen, J., Guffey, S., & Higgins-D’Alessandro, A. (2013). A review of school climate research. Review of

Educational Research, 83(3), 357–385.doi:10.3102/0034654313483907

Thibaut, J., & Walker, L. (1975). Procedural justice: A psychological analysis. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Tschannen-Moran, M., & Hoy, W. K. (2000). A multidisciplinary analysis of the nature, meaning, and measurement of trust. Review of Educational Research, 70(4), 547–593.doi:10.3102/00346543070004547

Uslaner, E. M. (2002). The moral foundations of trust. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Uslaner, E. M. (2008). Where you stand depends upon where your grandparents sat the inheritability of generalized trust. Public Opinion Quarterly, 72(4), 725–740.doi:10.1093/poq/nfn058

Van Maele, D., Forsyth, P. B., & Van Houtte, M. (Eds.). (2014). Trust and school life: The role of trust for learning, teaching, leading, and bridging. Dortrecht, Heidelberg, New York London: Springer Science & Business Media. Visser, P. S., & Krosnick, J. A. (1998). Development of attitude strength over the life cycle: Surge and decline. Journal of

Personality and Social Psychology, 75(6), 1389–1410.

Figure

Table 2. Unconditional latent growth curve model of social trust.
Table 3. Conditional latent growth curve model of social trust.

References

Related documents

Omvendt er projektet ikke blevet forsinket af klager mv., som det potentielt kunne have været, fordi det danske plan- og reguleringssystem er indrettet til at afværge

This result becomes even clearer in the post-treatment period, where we observe that the presence of both universities and research institutes was associated with sales growth

För att uppskatta den totala effekten av reformerna måste dock hänsyn tas till såväl samt- liga priseffekter som sammansättningseffekter, till följd av ökad försäljningsandel

The increasing availability of data and attention to services has increased the understanding of the contribution of services to innovation and productivity in

Generella styrmedel kan ha varit mindre verksamma än man har trott De generella styrmedlen, till skillnad från de specifika styrmedlen, har kommit att användas i större

Parallellmarknader innebär dock inte en drivkraft för en grön omställning Ökad andel direktförsäljning räddar många lokala producenter och kan tyckas utgöra en drivkraft

Närmare 90 procent av de statliga medlen (intäkter och utgifter) för näringslivets klimatomställning går till generella styrmedel, det vill säga styrmedel som påverkar

Den förbättrade tillgängligheten berör framför allt boende i områden med en mycket hög eller hög tillgänglighet till tätorter, men även antalet personer med längre än