• No results found

The Negative Consequences of Alcohol Intoxication andits Relation to Impulsivity and Gender Differences

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "The Negative Consequences of Alcohol Intoxication andits Relation to Impulsivity and Gender Differences"

Copied!
20
0
0

Loading.... (view fulltext now)

Full text

(1)

The Negative Consequences of Alcohol Intoxication and its Relation to Impulsivity and Gender Differences

Filip Källström Örebro University

Supervisor: Delia Latina Psychology III

(2)

Abstract

Consuming alcohol may lead to negative consequences, such as aggression, violence or drunk driving. However, not all adolescents who consume alcohol engage in negative behaviors. The purpose of this study was to examine the role of gender and impulsivity as risk factors for engagement in negative conducts after alcohol intoxication. The sample of this study

comprised 1,002 Swedish adolescent students between 12 and 16 years of age (52% boys). Students filled in self-report questionnaire at two time points (T1 and T2), once a year. After controlling for previous levels of negative consequences of alcohol consumption, as well as for drinking habits at T1, the result showed that impulsivity had an impact on adolescent’s alcohol-related consequences after intoxication. Also, boys showed a higher tendency to report negative consequences after alcohol intoxication compared to girls. All in all, this longitudinal study shed new light on the personality aspects that make adolescents more likely to engage in negative conducts after alcohol consumption.

(3)

The Negative Consequences of Alcohol Intoxication and its Relation to Impulsivity and Gender Differences

We individuals know that during adolescence people drink alcohol. We also knowthat some adolescents may engage in different types of problems due to alcohol intoxications, such as violence, aggression, and unhealthy sexual conducts. However, not all adolescents experience such problems after drinking. What can explain why certain adolescents behave in a negative way when they get intoxicated by alcohol? In this study, I will investigate a

combination of two variables, namely impulsivity and gender, which make adolescents more likely to engage in negative conducts after alcohol intoxication.

In today's society, alcohol is a major problem during adolescence (World health organization, 2010). National institute on alcohol abuse and alcoholism (2017) explains that alcohol represents the most used substance by adolescents, causing a huge health hazard. In fact, alcohol consumption seems to be related to different forms of cancer and high blood pressure, and it seems to weaken immune system, and effect the brain negatively (National institute on alcohol abuse and alcoholism, 2017). For this reason, European countries spend approximately 152 billion euro each year on alcohol prevention (World health organization, 2010). In spite of it, adolescents continue to consume alcohol in an unhealthy way (Hingson, Heeren, Winter, & Wechsler, 2005). The reason seems clear: when we are sober, we get shy and feel guilty if we do something stupid and judgmental, but these feelings diminish after alcohol consumption (Fjær, 2015). Also, alcohol diminishes negative feelings about our everyday problems and concerns and gives us a temporary state of peace, quiet, and happiness (Fjær, 2015; Geiger & MacKerron, 2016). However, the lack of inhibition caused by alcohol consumption might also lead to negative consequences because with alcohol in our body we tend to release more emotions – especially negative ones – than usual. Indeed, the

(4)

(Fjær, 2015). In this respect, a recent study showed that alcohol consumption increased risk of violence, injuries, verbal inconvenience and unplanned sexual encounters (Iwamoto, Cheng, Lee, Takamatsu, & Gordon, 2011). In addition, adolescents intoxicated by alcohol report black outs and fights, unprotected sex and drunk driving (Clapp, Reed, & Ruderman, 2014; Martinez, Sher, & Wood, 2014). Due to these findings, the alcohol-related consequences continue to be a large concern in today’s society, leading researchers to wonder about the personality features that might explain such consequences.

Alcohol intoxication is a major concern in Sweden and a big part of Sweden’s adolescents have tasted alcoholic beverages on one or more occasions (Statens

Folkhälsoinstitut, 2008). Over the past five years, more adolescent’s in Sweden have been treated for alcohol poisoning and this due to an increase of alcohol consumption and that youngsters drink heavier than before (Statens Folkhälsoinstitut, 2008). Alcohol intoxication is more dangerous for young people than adults but considers this alcohol consumption

continues to increase for adolescents and in Sweden about 3000 people die due to alcohol intoxicated accidents like traffic and drowning, and this is the leading cause of death among young people in Sweden (Statens Folkhälsoinstitut, 2005). Alcohol intoxication and

consumption is one of the main reasons for Swedish young people to visit health care centers, but the number of the young people who do not seek help for this kind of problem is

unknown (Folkhälsomyndigheten, 2014).

How can we explain the fact that some adolescents engage in negative behaviors after alcohol intoxication and other do not? In line with the models that have identified personality factors related to the negative consequences of alcohol intoxication, impulsivity represents one of the most cited variable (Curcio & George, 2011). A central feature of impulsivity is the inability to inhibit a behavior that is likely to lead to negative consequences (Baumeister & Vohs, 2004; Heatherton & Baumeister, 1996). Under the effect of alcohol, impulsive people

(5)

may be likely to use coping strategies that can provide relief in the short run, such as aggression or violence. However, such strategies may have potential long-term negative consequences (Hull & Slone, 2004). Three recent studies (Curcio & George, 2011; Jones, Chryssanthakis, & Groom, 2014; Magid, MacLean, & Colder, 2007) have supported such argument. Jones et al. (2014) examined the role of three different forms of impulsivity, namely urgency (i.e., engage in unplanned situations), sensation seeking (i.e., search for exciting new experiences) and lack of premeditation (i.e., not to do something intentionally), as the negative consequences of alcohol intoxication. They found that all the three forms of impulsivity were related to the negative consequences of alcohol use. However, while urgency was related to negative consequences regardless of how much alcohol was consumed, the relation of sensation seeking and lack of premeditation with the negative consequences of alcohol was dependent on how much alcohol adolescents consumed. Also, Curcio and George investigated the relationship between three facets of impulsivity, namely sensation seeking (i.e. search for exciting new experiences), positive urgency (i.e., engage in rash and risky behavior because of a positive mood) as well as negative urgency (i.e., engage in unplanned risky behavior because of distress) and negative consequences of alcohol consumption. They found that these three impulsivity traits uniquely predicted negative alcohol problems. Using impulsivity as single construct, Magid and colleagues found that impulsivity increase the likelihood for adolescents to report alcohol related problems. In addition, the authors found that impulsivity increased the likelihood to consume alcohol as a way to forget worries. In turn, this led to an increase in negative conducts after alcohol consumption.

Another variable that researchers have focused on when examining the negative consequences of alcohol intoxication in adolescence is gender. In this respect, research reports contradictory findings. According to some studies boys tend to vandalize and damage

(6)

property after consuming alcohol while girls are more likely to end up in verbal noise and insults (Isralowitz, Shpiegel, Reznik, & Laytin, 2009; Stoltenberg, Batien, & Birgenheir, 2008; West, Drummond, & Eames, 1990;). Also, males tend to engage in drunk driving more than females (Grittner, Kuntsche, Graham, & Bloomfield, 2012). Although males seem to be at greater risk of developing an unhealthy and more risky behaviors after alcohol

consumption, a few studies (Loxton, Bunker, Dingle, & Wong, 2015; Yarmush, Manchery, Luehring-Jones, & Erblich, 2016) indicated contradictory results. For example, one study showed that impulsive girls tend to consume more alcohol if opportunity is given which, in turn, can lead to negative conducts (Loxton et al., 2015). Also, some studies (e.g., Auerbach et al., 2010; Sasson & Mesch, 2016; Vardaki & Yannis, 2013; Yarmush et al., 2016) showed that boys usually act more carelessly than girls do in general. However, during alcohol intoxication, girls tend to do things that they might have not done in a sober state. Such behaviors can therefore lead to negative consequences in a drunken state.

Previous studies have shown that alcohol intoxication can lead to negative conducts. However, a few studies have focused on the aspects that might increase the likelihood for adolescents to develop such negative conducts. Most of the previous research focused on gender as factor that might explain why certain adolescents engage in negative behavior after drinking (Grittner et al., 2012; Isralowitz et al., 2009; Loxton et al., 2015; Stoltenberg et al., 2008; West et al., 1990; Yarmush et al., 2016). However, the results are inconsistent and due to these contradictory findings, further studies are needed to clarify the role of gender on the negative consequences related to alcohol consumption. Also, impulsivity represents a personality factor that plays a role. However, three studies have examined the relationship between impulsivity and maladaptive conducts after alcohol intoxication (Curcio & George, 2011; Jones et al., 2014; Magid et al., 2007). Although the mentioned studies shed new light on the relationship between impulsivity and negative consequences related to alcohol

(7)

intoxication, these studies are cross-sectional in nature and do not allow to draw conclusions about temporal relations and direction of effect. There is only one longitudinal study that have investigated impulsivity and alcohol related problems over time for adolescents (Moan, Norström, & Storvoll, 2013) but to my knowledge there is no longitudinal study that have analyzed both impulsivity and gender differences at the same time.

My research question is whether impulsivity and gender play a role in explaining why adolescents engage in negative consequences after alcohol intoxication. To investigate

whether there is a connection between impulsivity, gender, drinking and alcohol intoxication this study will use a longitudinal design and a multiple regression analysis.

To answer the research question and see whether impulsivity and gender play a role in adolescent’s negative behavior after getting drunk, two hypotheses will be tested. My first hypothesis is that impulsivity increases the likelihood to engage in problematic conducts after drinking alcohol. In addition, and due to the tendency of boys to report more physical

aggression (West et al., 1990) and risk behaviors (Grittner et al., 2012). My second hypothesis is that I expect males engage more likely in such conducts than females.

Method Participants

The sample for this study comes from the Seven Schools project, which included students from the 7th, 8th and 9th grade attending seven schools in a medium seized city in Sweden. Students filled in self-report questionnaires once a year, for four years. For this study, I used information from students in the 7th and 8th grade at two time points T1 (N = 1002) and T2 (N = 933), and ranging between 12 and 16 years of age. Overall, 1,121 students comprised the target sample at T1. Of the target sample, 89.4% of adolescents (N = 1,002, 52% boys), and the age of (M = 13.94, SD = 0.74) participated at T1. At T1 there was a dropout of N = 119

(8)

(10.6%) participants and at T2 there was a dropout of N = 188 (16.8%) participants.

Regarding individual and family characteristics, this study represented the national population with respect to socioeconomic background and family composition (Statistiska Centralbyrån, 2012). More specifically, 84% of the study participants were born in Sweden. About 72% of the fathers and 54% of the mothers of the participants were in fulltime employment, and about 9% of the fathers and 20% of the mothers were in part time employments, 4% of the

participant’s fathers and mothers were unemployed. Almost 65% of the study participants lived with both their mother and father, and 17 % with their mothers only and 2% with their fathers only and 14% lived sometimes with their father and sometimes with their mother, and 1% lived with a grown up other than their parents. In my sample, 22% of adolescents used to drink so much alcohol to get drunk at least once during T1. Also, 32% of adolescents at T1 and 26% of adolescents at T2 engaged in negative behaviors due to alcohol intoxication at least once. The students did not get paid for their participation, but they received a pen and refreshments.

Measures

To examine whether impulsivity and gender play a role in explaining why adolescents engage in negative consequences after alcohol intoxication this study used three different scales and also the variable gender to see how males and females interact different to the study variables. The participants filled in the questionnaire and mark one for male and two for female. The questionnaire included 13 items totally and consisted of three different parts related to the three variables (drinking problems, drinking and impulsivity) with different answer responses for each part.

Drinking problems. To investigate negative consequences after alcohol intoxication this study made use of a scale specifically developed for the Seven School Project. The participants were asked to answer questions about seven items regarding the extent of

(9)

negative behaviors when they were drunk. The response categories ranged from 1 (no, it has

not happened) to 3 (several times). Examples of items were: “you don’t remember what you

did or said the day after”, “you said stupid things to others that you were ashamed of afterwards”, “your personality change – you become a whole different person then you usually are”. Cronbach’s alpha for this scale was .89.

Drinking. To get information about drinking habits, adolescents were asked to answer the following question: “Have you had so much beer, liquor or wine that you got drunk during the past year?” The response category ranged from 1 (No, it has not happened) to 5 (More

than 10 times).

Impulsivity. Impulsivity was measured by using a subscale of the original version of the Youth Psychopathic Traits Inventory (Andershed, Kerr, Stattin, & Levander, 2002). The participants answered five items with a response category ranging from 1 (Don’t agree at all) to 4 (Agree completely). Examples of questions such as: “I often speak first and think later”, “I often do things without thinking ahead”, “if I get the chance of do something fun I’ll do it, regardless of what I am doing at the time”. For this scale, Cronbach’s alpha was .62.

Procedure

Before collecting the data, a request was sent out to the schools for approval, followed by that we needed the parent’s approval that their children may participate in the study. The parents were told about what the study was for and that they could say no if they did not want their children to participate. Only children who received their parents’ permission could be included in the study and could fill in the questionnaire. The children were informed that the study was voluntary and that they could stop if they wanted. Also, they were informed about the confidentiality of their answers. The time to answer the questionnaire was around five minutes.

(10)

Statistical analyses

To answer my research questions, I used multiple regression analyses by using the statistical program SPSS. More specifically, I entered impulsivity and gender at T1 as independent variables, and negative consequences after drinking as dependent variable at T2. In such model, I controlled for negative consequences after drinking and drinking habits at T1 to see whether there was a positive or negative effect between T1 and T2. Before answering my questions, I conducted a logistic regression analysis to examine whether dropping out at T2 was related to my variables of interest at T1. The drop out variable was labelled in the

following way: drop out = 0, retain = 1. The results showed that adolescents who dropped out at T2 were more likely to engage in negative consequences after drinking at T1 than those who did not drop out (OR -.82, 95% CI .28–.68; Nagelkerke R2 = .03). None of the other variables were significantly related to drop out at T2. Thus, I can conclude that there was a minimal effect of attrition on the results and that the drop out of the participants at T2 did not affect the form of the result.

Results

The purpose of this study was to investigate the role of impulsivity and gender in explaining why certain adolescents engage in problematic conducts after alcohol intoxication.

Table 1 reports the descriptive statistics among the study variables from T1 and T2 between boys and girls. By the result of the descriptive statistics can we see an increased mean value for boys from T1 to T2 for drinking problems and drinking. But girls have not increased drinking problems from T1 to T2 but have a higher mean value of drinking from T1 to T2. Impulsivity was used and only measured at T1 and therefore statistics of T2 are missing (see Table 1 for further information about impulsivity between girls and boys).

(11)

Table 1

Mean values (M) and standard deviations for the study variables

Variables Time 1 Time2

Boys Girls Boys Girls

M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD)

Drinking Problem 1.24 (0.44) 1.17 (0.35) 1.26 (0.47) 1.14 (0.29) Drinking 1.50 (0.72) 1.36 (0.83) 1.68 (0.60) 1.59 (0.53)

Impulsivity 2.36 (0.58) 2.42 (0.56) - -

To see how strong the relationship is and the direction between the study variables, a correlation analysis was made. As well to see if the study variables are consistent and that they have a good significance level (p = < 0.5) between each other. Almost all correlations between the study variables are significant. More specifically, males were more likely to get drunk and report negative consequences after intoxication than females, r (741) = .14, p < .001. Also, impulsivity was positively correlated with getting drunk, r (964) = .28, p <.001), as well as with reporting negative consequences after alcohol intoxication at T1, r (780) = .27,

p < .00l, and at T2, r (738) = .28, p <.001. Only exception was the correlation between

impulsivity and gender, r =.05, p = .09, reflecting a lack of gender differences in impulsivity (see Table 2 for further information).

(12)

Table 2

Descriptive and bivariate correlation coefficient between the study variables

1 2 3 4 5 M (SD) 1. Afterdrinking T1 - 1.20(0.40) 2. Afterdrinking T2 .45*** - 1.21(0.40) 3. Drinking T1 .62*** .50*** - 1.43(0.99) 4. Impulsivity T1 .27*** .28*** .28*** - 2.39(0.57) 5. Gender .09*** .14*** .07*** .05 -

Note. T1 = Time 1. T2 = Time 2; for variable gender, Females = 0 and males = 1.

* = p < .05, ** = p < .01, *** = p < .001,

To investigate the role of gender and impulsivity on negative behaviors after alcohol intoxication, a multiple regression analyses was conducted. After controlling for drinking habits and negative consequences after alcohol intoxication at T1, the result showed that 34% of variance of negative consequences after alcohol intoxication at T2 was explained by the predictors afterdrinking T1, drinking T1, impulsivity T1 and gender together, R² = .34, F (4, 602) = 77.84, p < .001). Impulsivity T1 explained 7% of the variance of negative

consequences after alcohol intoxication at T2, R² = .07, F (1, 738) = 60.63, p < .001. Gender explained 2 % of the variance of negative consequences after alcohol intoxication, R² = .02, F (2, 740) = 7.89, p < .001). Males were more likely to engage in negative consequences after drinking compared to females (β = .10, p < .01). In addition, impulsivity increased the likelihood for adolescents to engage in negative consequences after drinking (β = .17, p < .001).

(13)

Discussion

The purpose of this study was to investigate the personality characteristic which explains what factors that’s contributes more to adolescent’s negative conducts after consuming alcohol. After controlling for previous levels of negative consequences and drinking habits, the results showed that impulsivity had an impact on adolescent’s alcohol-related

consequences after intoxication, increasing the risk for adolescents to report such

consequences. Also, boys seemed to have a higher tendency to report negative consequences after alcohol intoxication compared to girls.

This study did not investigate the mediating mechanisms that explain why impulsivity leads to alcohol related problems. In their cross-sectional study, Magid and colleagues (2007) reported that impulsivity increased the likelihood for adolescents to use alcohol as a tool to forget worries. This, in turn, increased the likelihood to engage in alcohol related problems. Future studies could investigate such relations by using a longitudinal design, capturing temporal paths. Also, in this study I didn’t look at the conditions that might explain why boys are more likely to have alcohol related problems. Peer pressure may be one of such

conditions. Adolescent boys may behave more negatively if they are in a group of peers. They might do stupid things just to look brave in the eyes of peers. They might also do things they would have not done just because of peer pressure. For all the above mentioned reasons, future studies should investigate the role of peer pressure in explaining why males engage in negative behaviors after alcohol consumption. Also, different kinds of individual aspects which in turn can develop negative consequences after alcohol intoxication like stress and social anxiety and see how these properties are affected over time and in relation to alcohol for adolescents.

Impulsivity is a personality characteristic that leads people to behave without thinking ahead, and which can make people end up in unplanned situations (Baumeister & Vohs,

(14)

2004). This factor may have major consequences if people use alcohol to release tension and because of distress. In this situation and in line with this study’s results shows that impulsive people may engage in negative behaviors while in a drunk condition (Jones et al., 2014). In line with this conceptualization, my results showed that impulsivity leads adolescents to report negative consequences such as violence, verbal insults and unprotected sex after alcohol intoxication. These findings are in line with the result of previous studies (Crucio & George, 2011; Jones et al., 2014; Magid et al., 2007). To my knowledge, these are the only studies that have examined the personality characteristics that can explain the negative consequences of alcohol use. However, and contrary to the above mentioned studies, my study represents the first attempt in addressing such issues using a longitudinal design.

In the analysis of possible gender differences, the result showed that that adolescent males were at increased risk of alcohol related problems than females. A few studies have shown a difference in gender after alcohol intoxication. More specifically, some have reported that males have more problems (Grittner et al., 2012; Isralowitz et al., 2009; West et al., 1990). On the contrary, others have reported that females have more problems (Loxton et al., 2015; Yarmush, et al., 2016). My study supports the first line of research.

During adolescence, boys tend to adopt more risky behaviors than girls (Auerbach et al., 2010). For example, that male adolescents share personal information online with the aim of meeting unknown people more than females (Sasson & Mesch, 2016). Also, boys tend to drive a car at faster speed than girls, violating the traffic regulations, and without thinking about the possible negative consequences of such type of driving (Vardaki & Yannis, 2013). The males’ general tendency to engage in risky behaviors can explain why males report more negative consequences after alcohol intoxication. In these situations, males are even more likely to enact risky conducts due to the effects of alcohol

.

(15)

This study has some limitations that need to be addressed. First, for this study I made used self-report questionnaires filled in by adolescent students. This might have introduced biases since the answers represent a subjective perception of individual students. Another limitation of the use of self-report refers to the fact that some of the study questions are sensitive and can contain information that the participants may not feel comfortable with sharing. Also, the Cronbach’s alpha for the impulsivity scale was a bit low (α = .62.) and it can be due to that participants did not understand the impulsivity questionnaire correctly.

In spite of these limitations, this study has also strengths. First of all, I used information from a normative sample of adolescents. This sample reflects the Swedish population (Statistiska centralbyrån, 2012). Therefore, these results can be generalized to other young people because of a good validity. Second, and most importantly, to my knowledge this represents the first longitudinal study that investigated relevant personality aspect that increase the likelihood for adolescents to report negative consequences after alcohol intoxication with the major role of gender differences at the same time. I showed that being a male, as well as being impulsive, increase the risk for negative consequences after drinking. Also, a good reliability value of drinking problems scale shows good appreciation of the drinking problem questionnaire. Since this study is of longitudinal nature and it follows a certain age group of adolescents, it shows the validity is of high importance.

All in all, this study has provided knowledge about intrapersonal characteristics that might lead to alcohol problem and consequences after getting drunk during adolescence. The result of the current study, can contribute to a wider understanding of alcohol consumption among adolescents and its negative influence on their behaviors. Furthermore, with help of these findings we can generate better alcohol education and prevention methods for

(16)

References

Andershed, H. A., Kerr, M., Stattin, H., & Levander, S. (2002). Psychopathic traits in non- referred youths: A new assessment tool. In E. Blaauw & L. Sheridan (Eds.),

Psychopaths: Current international perspectives (pp. 133-158). Den Haag:

Elsevier.

Auerbach, R. P., Tsai, B., & Abela, J. Z. (2010). Temporal relationships among depressive symptoms, risky behavior engagement, perceived control, and gender in a sample of adolescents. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 20(3), 726-747. http://dx.doi.org.db.ub.oru.se/10.1111/j.1532-7795.2010.00657.x

Baumeister, R. F., & Vohs, K. D. (2004). Sexual economics: Sex as female resource for social exchange in heterosexual interactions. Personality and Social Psychology

Review, 8(4), 339-363.

http://dx.doi.org.db.ub.oru.se/10.1207/s15327957pspr0804_2

Clapp, J. D., Reed, M. B., & Ruderman, D. E. (2014). The relationship between drinking games and intentions to continue drinking, intentions to drive after drinking, and adverse consequences: Results of a field study. The American Journal of Drug

and Alcohol Abuse, 40(5), 374-379.

http://dx.doi.org.db.ub.oru.se/10.3109/00952990.2014.933838

Curcio, A. L., & George, A. M. (2011). Selected impulsivity facets with alcohol use/problems: The mediating role of drinking motives. Addictive Behaviors,

36,959-964. http://dx.doi.org.db.ub.oru.se/10.1016/j.addbeh.2011.05.007

Fjær, E. G. (2015). Moral emotions the day after drinking. Contemporary drug problems: An

Interdisciplinary Quarterly, 42(4), 299-313.

http://dx.doi.org.db.ub.oru.se/10.1177/0091450915604988

(17)

https://www.folkhalsomyndigheten.se/contentassets/42a5882b2540492bb6b1fd 1163726c84/studenter-och-hog-alkoholkonsumtion-webb.pdf

Geiger, B. B., & MacKerron, G. (2016). Can alcohol make you happy? A subjective wellbeing approach. Social Science & Medicine, 156, 184-191. http://dx.doi.org.db.ub.oru.se/10.1016/j.socscimed.2016.03.034

Grittner, U., Kuntsche, S., Graham, K., & Bloomfield, K. (2012). Social inequalities and gender differences in the experience of alcohol-related problems. Alcohol and

Alcoholism, 47(5), 597-605.

http://dx.doi.org.db.ub.oru.se/10.1093/alcalc/ags040

Heatherton, T. F., & Baumeister, R. F. (1996). Self-regulation failure: Past, present, and future. Psychological Inquiry, 7(1), 90-98.

http://dx.doi.org.db.ub.oru.se/10.1207/s15327965pli0701_20

Hingson, R., Heeren, T., Winter, M., & Wechsler, H. (2005). Magnitude of alcohol-related mortality and morbidity among US college students age 18-24: Changes from 1998-2001. Annual Review of Public Health, 26, 259-279.

https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.publhealth.26.021304.144652

Hull, J. G., & Slone, L. B. (2004). Alcohol and self-regulation. In R. F Baumeister, & K. D. Vhos (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation: Research, theory and applications (pp. 466-491).NY: Guilford Press.

Isralowitz, R., Shpiegel, S., Reznik, A., & Lavtin, A. (2009). Ethiopian youth in Israel: Gender-related alcohol use and related problem behavior. Drugs: Education,

Prevention & Policy, 16(3), 273- 278.

http://dx.doi.org.db.ub.oru.se/10.1080/09687630701869211

(18)

getting drunk: The role of masculine norms, alcohol intoxication and alcohol-related problems among college men. Addictive behaviors, 36(9), 906-911.

http://dx.doi.org.db.ub.oru.se/10.1016/j.addbeh.2011.04.055

Jones, K. A., Chryssanthakis, A., & Groom, M. J. (2014). Impulsivity and drinking motives predict problem behaviours relating to alcohol use in university students.

Addictive Behaviors, 39(1), 289-296.

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2013.10.024

Loxton, N. J., Bunker, R. J., Dingle, G. A., & Wong, V. (2015). Drinking not thinking: A prospective study of personality traits and drinking motives on alcohol consumption across the first year of university. Personality and Individual

Differences, 79, 134-139. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2015.02.010

Magid, V., MacLean, M. G., & Colder, C. R (2007). Differentiated between sensation seeking and impulsivity through their mediated relations with alcohol use and problems.

Addictive Behaviors, 32, 2046-2061. 

http://dx.doi.org.db.ub.oru.se/10.1016/j.addbeh.2007.01.015 

Martinez, J. A., Sher, K. J., & Wood, P. K. (2014). Drinking consequences and subsequent drinking in college students over 4 years. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors,

28(4), 1240-1245. http://dx.doi.org.db.ub.oru.se/10.1037/a0038352

Moan, I. S., Norström, T., & Storvall, E. E. (2013). Alcohol use and drunk driving: The modifying effect of impulsivity. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs,

74(1), 114-119. http://dx.doi.org/10.15288/jsad.2013.74.114

National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism. (2017). Publications & Multimedia:

Underage drinking. Retrieved from

https://pubs.niaa.nih.gov/publications/Underagedrinking/underagefact.htm Sasson, H., & Mesch, G. (2016). Gender differences in the factors of explaining risky

(19)

behavior online. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 45(5), 973-985. http://dx.doi.org.db.ub.oru.se/10.1007/s10964-016-0465-7

Statens Folkhälsoinstitut. (2005). Alkohol och hälsa: En kunskapsöversikt om alkoholens

positiva och negativa effekter på vår hälsa. Retrieved from

https://www.folkhalsomyndigheten.se/contentassets/a643d702ed1b4ef1a81281 56f44342f6/r200511alkoholhalsa0601.pdf

Statens Folkhälsoinstitut. (2008). Psykisk ohälsa och alkoholkonsumtion: Hur hänger det

ihop. Retrieved from

https://www.folkhalsomyndigheten.se/contentassets/b8429d33045f4905934aca c0258f2809/r200837_psykisk_ohalsa_0812.pdf

Statistiska centralbyrån. (2012). Statistisk årsbok för Sverige. Retrieved from

http://www.scb.se/sv_/Hitta-statistik/Publiceringskalender/Visa-detaljerad-information/?publobjid=17399

Stoltenberg, S. F., Batien, B. D., & Birgenheir, D. G. (2008). Does gender moderate

associations among impulsivity and health-risk behaviors? Addictive Behaviors,

33(2), 252-265. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2007.09.004

Vardaki, S., & Yannis, G. (2013). Investigating the self- reported behavior of drivers and their attitudes to traffic violations. Journal of Safety Research, 461-11. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jsr.2013.03.001 

West, R., Drummond, C., & Eames, K. (1990). Alcohol consumption, problematic drinking and antisocial behavior in a sample of college students. British Journal of

Addiction, 85(4), 479-486. http://dx.doi.10.1111/j1360-0443.1990.tb01668.x

World health organization. (2010). European status report on alcohol and health. Retrieved from www.euro.who.Int/healthtopics

(20)

impulsivity: Effects on cue-induces alcohol craving. Alcoholism: Clinical and

References

Related documents

46 Konkreta exempel skulle kunna vara främjandeinsatser för affärsänglar/affärsängelnätverk, skapa arenor där aktörer från utbuds- och efterfrågesidan kan mötas eller

Exakt hur dessa verksamheter har uppstått studeras inte i detalj, men nyetableringar kan exempelvis vara ett resultat av avknoppningar från större företag inklusive

Both Brazil and Sweden have made bilateral cooperation in areas of technology and innovation a top priority. It has been formalized in a series of agreements and made explicit

The increasing availability of data and attention to services has increased the understanding of the contribution of services to innovation and productivity in

Industrial Emissions Directive, supplemented by horizontal legislation (e.g., Framework Directives on Waste and Water, Emissions Trading System, etc) and guidance on operating

The EU exports of waste abroad have negative environmental and public health consequences in the countries of destination, while resources for the circular economy.. domestically

Accordingly, this paper aims to investigate how three companies operating in the food industry; Max Hamburgare, Innocent and Saltå Kvarn, work with CSR and how this work has

http://urn.kb.se/resolve?urn=urn:nbn:se:bth-21705.. [Context and Motivation] Software requirements are affected by the knowledge and confidence of software engineers. Analyzing