• No results found

Trends in childhood and adolescent internalizing symptoms: results from Swedish population based twin cohorts

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2022

Share "Trends in childhood and adolescent internalizing symptoms: results from Swedish population based twin cohorts"

Copied!
11
0
0

Loading.... (view fulltext now)

Full text

(1)

This is the published version of a paper published in BMC Psychology.

Citation for the original published paper (version of record):

Durbeej, N., Sörman, K., Norén Selinus, E., Lundström, S., Lichtenstein, P. et al. (2019) Trends in childhood and adolescent internalizing symptoms: results from Swedish population based twin cohorts

BMC Psychology, 7: 50

https://doi.org/10.1186/s40359-019-0326-8

Access to the published version may require subscription.

N.B. When citing this work, cite the original published paper.

Permanent link to this version:

http://urn.kb.se/resolve?urn=urn:nbn:se:umu:diva-165296

(2)

R E S E A R C H A R T I C L E Open Access

Trends in childhood and adolescent internalizing symptoms: results from Swedish population based twin cohorts

Natalie Durbeej 1,2 , Karolina Sörman 1 , Eva Norén Selinus 1,3 , Sebastian Lundström 4 , Paul Lichtenstein 5 , Clara Hellner 1 and Linda Halldner 5,6,7*

Abstract

Background: Previous research has noted trends of increasing internalizing problems (e.g., symptoms of depression and anxiety), particularly amongst adolescent girls. Cross-cohort comparisons using identical assessments of both anxiety and depression in youth are lacking, however.

Methods: In this large twin study, we examined trends in internalizing symptoms in samples of 9 year old children and 15 year old adolescents, gathered from successive birth cohorts from 1998 to 2008 (age 9) and 1994 –2001 (age 15). Assessments at age 9 were parent-rated, and at age 15 self- and parent-rated. We examined (i) the relation between birth cohorts and internalizing symptoms using linear regressions, and (ii) whether percentages of participants exceeding scale cut-off scores changed over time, using Cochrane Armitage Trend Tests.

Results: Among 9 year old children, a significantly increasing percentage of participants (both boys and girls) had scores above cut-off on anxiety symptoms, but not on depressive symptoms. At age 15, a significantly increasing percentage of participants (both boys and girls) had scores above cut-off particularly on self-reported internalizing symptoms. On parent-reported internalizing symptoms, only girls demonstrated a corresponding trend.

Conclusion: In line with previous studies, we found small changes over sequential birth cohorts in frequencies of depression and anxiety symptoms in children. Further, these changes were not exclusive to girls.

Keywords: Internalizing, Childhood, Adolescence, Prevalence, Epidemiology

Background

Epidemiological studies have indicated that mental health problems in youth are common and tend to persist into adulthood [1]. An estimated 10 –20% of children world- wide experience some form of mental health problems [2 – 4]. Mental health problems in youth are commonly di- vided into externalizing (e.g., impaired self-regulation, antisocial behavior) and internalizing (e.g., depression, anxiety, hypersensitivity, worry) problems [5]. Internaliz- ing symptoms are associated with a range of difficulties negatively affecting health and everyday life for youths

(e.g., impaired self-worth, lack of joy, disrupted appetite and sleep patterns), including increased risk of self-harm and suicide [4]. A relatively common finding is that in- ternalizing symptoms are more prevalent in girls than in boys, particularly during adolescence. Externalizing symp- toms, however, tend to be more prevalent in boys than in girls [6, 7]. A meta-analytic review of sex differences in emotion expression involving a large number of partici- pants (N = 21 709) from 166 studies in total, demonstrated that girls show more internalizing symptoms than boys overall, even though the differences were small [8]. The term internalizing, broadly referring to symptoms of anx- iety, depression and somatic symptoms, has been used for several decades in research [9]. Despite this, a clear-cut definition of the term is still lacking. In the present study, internalizing symptoms specifically refer to symptoms of

© The Author(s). 2019 Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

* Correspondence: linda.halldner@umu.se

Natalie Durbeej and Karolina Sörman contributed equally to this work.

5

Department of Medical Epidemiology and Biostatistics, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden

6

Child and Adolescent Psychiatry Research center, BUP Klinisk forskningsenhet, Stockholm, Sweden

Full list of author information is available at the end of the article Durbeej et al. BMC Psychology (2019) 7:50

https://doi.org/10.1186/s40359-019-0326-8

(3)

depression and anxiety assessed with three different scales.

Trends in mental health symptoms

Understanding time trends in mental health symptoms is one important aspect in the prevention of youth men- tal illness [1]. During the past decades, there has been an increase in diagnoses and treatment of youth psychi- atric disorders [1]. Whether or not this corresponds to increasing levels of internalizing symptoms in commu- nity samples is not studied sufficiently so far. Studies in- vestigating temporal trends in psychiatric symptoms have generated conflicting findings [2, 10]. In a system- atic review mental health in youth across studies from various cultural contexts (e.g., countries from northern Europe, Australia and North America) was investigated.

It encompassed a ten-year period or longer, and demon- strated that temporal fluctuations are dependent on multiple factors including developmental phase, sex and type of symptom [2]. The studies included in this review demonstrated overall increases in internalizing symp- toms during the past decade(s) among girls, with more mixed findings for boys [2]. A literature review on sur- veys conducted in Sweden indicated an increase in men- tal health problems (e.g., depressive symptoms and worry) among adolescents aged 11–15 between the mid- 1980’s and mid-2000’s, with increasing levels particularly among girls [11]. Another Swedish study that included a large sample of adolescents (N = 15,000; 15–16 years old) used repeated cross-sectional assessments of psycho- somatic health problems during 1988–2005. The results demonstrated successive increases in psychosomatic health problems specifically in girls across the study period [12]. In parallel, a steady increase in mental health care consumption for children aged 13–17 in Stockholm county (where approximately 20% of Swedish children and adolescents reside) has been firmly estab- lished since year 2002 [11]. It is unclear whether this re- flects an increase in some form of mental health problem or an increased inclination to seek mental health services. Overall, previous research indicates mixed findings on temporal trends in psychiatric symp- toms, with findings partly associated with differences in sex and developmental time period. Cross-cohort com- parisons can be used to better understand patterns be- yond diagnostic changes [1]. There is a lack of studies investigating symptoms of depression and anxiety in rep- resentative community pre-adolescent samples using re- peated cross-sectional assessments [2, 11].

Methods Study rationale

The rationale for this large population-based study was to examine trends in internalizing symptoms, both

anxiety and depression, in consecutive birth cohorts at age 9 (parent-rated) and age 15 (self- and parent-rated).

We investigated (i) the relation between birth cohorts and internalizing symptoms, and (ii) whether the per- centage of participants exceeding cut-off scores of the scales changed over time. Investigating temporal trends of internalizing symptoms is an important research en- deavor to explore whether the increase in clinical param- eters (i.e., diagnoses and treatment) of anxiety and depression during the past decades corresponds to ac- tual increasing levels of internalizing symptoms.

Participants

Participants were recruited from the Child and Adoles- cent Twin Study in Sweden (CATSS), which emanates from the Swedish Twin Registry (STR). The CATSS is an ongoing and nation-wide longitudinal study that aims to investigate mental and somatic health in childhood and adolescence, including all twins born in Sweden since July 1992 [13]. When the twins are 9 years old, their par- ents are invited to participate in a telephone interview that assesses the child’s health status and social environ- ment. At age 15, all twins and their parents are invited to take part in a questionnaire follow-up.

The current study involved samples of children and adolescents 9 and 15 years old, gathered from successive birth cohorts from 1998 to 2008 (age 9) and 1994–2001 (age 15).

The response rate in the total sample at age 9 was 63.5%. The response rates for each birth cohort 1998–

2008 respectively were: 69.9, 73.2, 71.4, 69.6, 73.0, 68.1, 60.3, 63.8, 69.2, 59.7, and 44.2%. Furthermore, the re- sponse rate in the total sample at age 15 was 60.1%. The response rates for each birth cohort 1994–2001 respect- ively was: 60.0, 57.4, 72.4, 61.2, 63.1, 54.7, 57.5, and 55.2%. The data collection took place between 2007 and 2017 for children age 9, and between 2009 and 2016 for adolescents age 15.

Assessment at age 9

The telephone-interview at age 9 included the following parent-rated scales:

The Screen for Child Anxiety Related Emotional Disor- ders (SCARED) is a 41-item screening instrument for anxiety symptoms, based on criteria in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders 4th edition (DSM-IV), in children and adolescents aged 9–18 [14, 15]. Each item is rated on a 3-point Likert scale: from 0 (= not true), to 2 (= true), with a maximum score of 82.

SCARED is one of the most commonly used scales to

assess anxiety symptoms in children [16]. It is consid-

ered well-suited for the use in community samples [17],

across a wide range of cultural contexts [18]. SCARED

has demonstrated overall satisfactory psychometric

(4)

properties (e.g., internal consistency, test-retest reliabil- ity) across different types of samples [19, 20], and good discriminant validity (i.e., ability to differentiate children with and without anxious disorders) [19]. Cut-off values for clinical significance range from a total score of 25 (i.e., reflecting suspected anxiety disorder) to 33 [15, 20].

We chose to use the lower cut-off score, given that our sample is a community sample, and also that the scores were low overall. SCARED revealed satisfactory scale re- liability (Coefficient H = .90 for all participants, .89 for boys and .89 for girls [21].

The Short Mood and Feelings Questionnaire (sMFQ) [22] is a 13-item short form of the original 33-item Mood and Feelings Questionnaire (MFQ) [23], developed to assess depressive symptoms in children and adoles- cents 8–18 years old. Each item is rated on a 3-point Likert scale, from 0 (= no), to 2 (= yes). Psychometric properties for sMFQ are satisfactory, with adequate in- ternal consistency [22, 24–26], validity for parent-rated scores [27, 28], and a strong overlap between sMFQ and the full-length MFQ [25, 26]. Based on a previous study with a similar study population, we chose to use a score of 7 as a cut-off [29]. The sMFQ revealed satisfactory scale reliability (Coefficient H = .84 for all participants as well as for boys and for girls [21].

Assessment at age 15

The assessment at age 15 encompassed self-and parent- rated versions of the Strength and Difficulties Question- naire (SDQ), a 25-item screening measure developed to assess externalizing and internalizing symptoms in chil- dren and adolescents 4–16 years old [30]. SDQ consists of five subscales (i.e., conduct problems, hyperactivity, emotional symptoms, peer problems, and prosocial be- havior) with five items in each subscale. The current study only included the emotional subscale (i.e., SDQ-

Emotional). Each item is rated on a 3-point Likert scale, from 0 (= not true), to 2 (= certainly true). SDQ is exten- sively used in different settings (i.e., clinical and commu- nity) to screen for internalizing symptoms [31], and it has been incorporated into national surveys of mental health in Swedish children [11]. We chose to use a score of 5 as cut-off, based on official recommendations for a Swedish context [32].

The SDQ-Emotional parent version revealed satisfac- tory scale reliability for all participants Coefficient H = .81 for all participants, .80 for boys and .82 for girls) [21]. Coefficient H values for the SDQ-Emotional self-re- port version were .73 for all participants, .66 for boys and .71 for girls. There was a moderate correlation be- tween the SDQ-Emotional parent version and the SDQ- Emotional self-report version (Pearson’s r = .41).

Statistical analyses

Internalizing symptoms were investigated using descrip- tive statistics (i.e., means, standard deviations, range, fre- quencies). Sex differences between completers and non- completers of the scales were investigated through chi- square tests whereas sex differences in scale scores were computed using t-test for independent samples. Add- itionally, Cohen’s d was calculated (by dividing the mean difference by the pooled standard deviation) as a meas- ure of effect size. To explore the relation between birth cohorts and internalizing symptoms, linear regressions were calculated. In these analyses, birth cohort served as the independent variable and individual total scale scores (on SCARED, sMFQ, SDQ parent-report and SDQ self- report) served as dependent variables. Prior to the calcu- lations, we checked the distributions of the scores, and considered them approximately normally distributed.

To explore whether the percentage of participants ex- ceeding cut-off scores of the scales changed over time, Table 1 Descriptive statistics for all scales used total group and divided by sex

M (SD)

total M (SD)

boys M (SD)

girls Cohens’d boys vs

girls p boys vs

girls

Range (min-

max) n (%) boys/girls

Age 9

SCARED

a

n = 14979 5.25 (6.57) 4.94 (6.47) 5.56 (6.66) .09 < .001 0 –74 7493 (50.0)/ 7486

(50.0)

sMFQ

a

n = 17562 1.33 (2.96) 1.40 (3.01) 1.27 (2.91) .04 .004 0 –26 8824 (50.2)/ 8738

(49.8) Age 15

b

SDQ-Emotional pr n = 9821 1.15 (1.60) .84 (1.35) 1.44 (1.76) .38 < .001 0 –10 4722 (48.1)/ 5099

(51.9) SDQ-Emotional sr

c

n =

10821

2.93 (2.28) 2.00 (1.80) 3.70 (2.35) .81 < .001 0 –10 4873 (45.0) 5946

(54.9)

Note. SCARED Screen for Child Anxiety Related Emotional Disorders, sMFQ Short Mood and Feelings Questionnaire, SDQ Strength and Difficulties Questionnaire, sr self-report, pr parent-report

a

Cohorts 1998 –2008

b

Cohorts 1993 –2001

c

Data on sex missing for two participants

Durbeej et al. BMC Psychology (2019) 7:50 Page 3 of 10

(5)

Table 2 Descriptive statistics for individual birth cohorts and results from linear regressions for SCARED, sMFQ, SDQ parent-report and SDQ self-report

Birth cohorts Total Boys Girls

M (SD) M (SD) M (SD)

SCARED parent-report age 9

1998 5.2 (6.7), n = 865 5.1 (6.9), n = 425 5.4 (6.5), n = 440

1999 4.7 (5.7), n = 1785 4.5 (5.8), n = 890 4.9 (5.5), n = 895

2000 4.8 (5.8), n = 1593 4.3 (5.5), n = 786 5.2 (6.1), n = 807

2001 4.4 (5.7), n = 1579 3.8 (5.1), n = 799 4.9 (6.2), n = 780

2002 5.0 (6.2), n = 1547 4.7 (5.9), n = 773 5.4 (6.5), n = 774

2003 5.1 (6.8), n = 1647 4.6 (6.2), n = 841 5.7 (7.4), n = 806

2004 5.2 (6.9), n = 1189 5.0 (7.0), n = 580 5.3 (6.9), n = 609

2005 5.5 (6.8), n = 1300 5.6 (7.2), n = 662 5.5 (6.3), n = 638

2006 6.0 (6.9), n = 1545 5.9 (7.3), n = 784 6.2 (6.6), n = 761

2007 6.4 (7.4), n = 1310 6.0 (6.9), n = 653 6.9 (7.9), n = 657

2008 6.6 (8.0), n = 619 6.2 (8.5), n = 300 6.9 (7.6), n = 319

Linear regression β = .187, p < .001 (95% CI: .151–.223) β = .260, p < .001 (95% CI: .145–.245) β = .260, p < .001 (95% CI: .128–.230) sMFQ parent-report age 9

1998 1.00 (2.4), n = 925 1.0 (2.5), n = 458 1.0 (2.4), n = 467

1999 1.1 (2.8), n = 1981 1.1 (2.7), n = 990 1.1 (2.8), n = 991

2000 1.1 (2.7), n = 1824 1.0 (2.6), n = 908 1.1 (2.7), n = 916

2001 1.3 (2.9), n = 1836 1.3 (2.8), n = 938 1.3 (3.0), n = 898

2002 1.6 (3.1), n = 1903 1.6 (3.2), n = 952 1.5 (3.2), n = 951

2003 1.5 (3.2), n = 1960 1.6 (3.3), n = 994 1.4 (3.1), n = 966

2004 1.3 (2.7), n = 1504 1.4 (2.8), n = 751 1.2 (2.6), n = 753

2005 1.2 (2.7), n = 1596 1.3 (2.8), n = 811 1.1 (2.6), n = 785

2006 1.5 (3.2), n = 1832 1.7 (3.1), n = 941 1.4 (3.0), n = 891

2007 1.6 (3.3), n = 1513 1.7 (3.4), n = 748 1.4 (3.2), n = 765

2008 1.5 (3.1), n = 686 1.6 (3.3), n = 332 1.5 (2.9), n = 354

Linear regression β = .005, p = .024 (95% CI: .001–.009) β = .003, p = .063 (95% CI: .000–.011) β = .003, p = .184 (95% CI: −.002–.009) SDQ-Emotional parent-report age 15

1994 1.1 (1.5), n = 1422 .83 (1.4), n = 644 1.3 (1.6), n = 778

1995 1.1 (1.6), n = 1309 .95 (1.5), n = 674 1.3 (1.7), n = 635

1996 1.1 (1.6), n = 1381 .80 (1.3), n = 681 1.4 (1.6), n = 700

1997 1.2 (1.6), n = 1186 .81 (1.3), n = 623 1.5 (1.7), n = 563

1998 1.2 (1.6), n = 1220 .82 (1.4), n = 623 1.4 (1.9), n = 575

1999 1.2 (1.6), n = 1128 .88 (1.3), n = 553 1.5 (1.9), n = 575

2000 1.2 (1.7), n = 1109 .77 (1.3), n = 495 1.6 (1.9), n = 571

2001 1.3 (1.7), n = 1109 .81 (1.3), n = 503 1.7 (1.89), n = 606

Linear regression β = .220, p = .002 (95% CI: −.080--.360) β = −.009, p = .299 (95% CI: −.026–.008) β = .046, p = .001 (95% CI: .025–.066) SDQ-Emotional self-report age 15

1994 2.6 (2.2), n = 1372 1.7 (1.7), n = 578 3.3 (2.3), n = 794

1995 2.5 (2.0), n = 1251 1.7 (1.8), n = 611 2.9 (2.1), n = 640

1996 2.9 (2.2), n = 1608 2.0 (1.8), n = 759 3.6 (2.2), n = 849

1997 3.0 (2.2), n = 1334 2.1 (1.8), n = 636 3.8 (2.3), n = 835

1998 3.0 (2.3), n = 1443 2.1 (1.8), n = 608 3.7 (2.4), n = 835

(6)

Cochrane Armitage Trend Tests (two-sided) were com- puted. Both sets of twins were used in all analyses. A cluster robust sandwich estimator was applied to adjust the standard errors for the nested twin data when com- puting the regression models. Analyses were computed in the total sample, and with boys and girls separately.

Results

Descriptive statistics

The number of participants completing each scale ranged between 9821 and 17562, with an approximately equal distribution of boys and girls (see Table 1 for de- scriptive statistics of all scales used, for the total group and divided by sex). There were no differences in the proportions of boys and girls between completers and non completers of the SCARED and sMFQ respectively (χ 2 (1, 17816) = 3.06, p = .080, χ 2 (1, 17816) = 2.01,

p = .156). However, a larger proportion of boys were found among the completers (48.2%) than among the non-completers (43.2%) of the SDQ-Emotional parent report (χ 2 (1,12643) = 20.94, p < .001). In contrast, a lar- ger proportion of girls were found among the com- pleters (54.7%) of the SDQ-Emotional self-report than among the non-completers of this scale (39.4%) (χ 2 (1, 12643) = 131.83, p < .001). Moreover, among the com- pleters, girls had significantly higher mean scores than boys on all scales except for the sMFQ where boys had slightly higher mean scores than girls. Effect sizes for the sex differences were negligible for the SCARED and sMFQ (Cohen’s d = .09 and .04 respectively), small for the SDQ Emotional parent-report (Cohen’s d = .38) and large for the SDQ Emotional self-report (Cohen’s d = .81).

Table 2 Descriptive statistics for individual birth cohorts and results from linear regressions for SCARED, sMFQ, SDQ parent-report and SDQ self-report (Continued)

Birth cohorts Total Boys Girls

M (SD) M (SD) M (SD)

1999 3.1 (2.4), n = 1278 2.1 (1.9), n = 583 3.9 (2.4), n = 695

2000 3.2 (2.4), n = 1266 2.1, (1.8), n = 533 4.0 (2.3), n = 722

2001 3.3 (2.4), n = 1269 2.0 (1.8), n = 554 4.3 (2.4), n = 713

Linear regression β = .116, p < .001 (95% CI: .097–.135) β = .054, p < .001 (95% CI: .032–.077) β = .155, p < .001 (95% CI: .129–.181)

Table 3 Percentage of participants with scores above cut-offs, total sample and individual birth cohorts Birth cohorts SCARED parent-report age 9 sMFQ parent-report age 9 SDQ-Emotional

parent-report age 15

SDQ-Emotional self-report age 15

Total % n Boys % n Girls % n Total % n Boys % n Girls % n Total % n Boys % n Girls % n Total % n Boys % n Girls % n All birth cohorts 2.2 (324) 2.1 (154) 2.3 (170) 5.9 (1044) 6.3 (553) 5.6 (491) 5.0 (495) 2.9 (135) 7.1 (360) 23.6 (2551) 10.0 (489) 34.7 (2061)

1994 n.a n.a n.a n.a n.a n.a 4.9 (69) 2.6 (17) 6.7 (52) 19.5 (267) 7.4 (43) 28.2 (224)

1995 n.a n.a n.a n.a n.a n.a 5.4 (71) 4.3 (29) 6.6 (42) 15.8 (198) 8.3 (51) 23.0 (147)

1996 n.a n.a n.a n.a n.a n.a 3.8 (53) 2.6 (18) 5.0 (35) 23.0 (369) 10.0 (76) 34.5 (293)

1997 n.a n.a n.a n.a n.a n.a 4.7 (56) 2.1 (13) 7.6 (43) 23.5 (314) 10.1 (64) 35.8 (250)

1998 2.3 (20) 2.4 (10) 2.3 (10) 3.5 (32) 2.8 (13) 4.1 (19) 5.2 (64) 3.6 (20) 6.6 (44) 24.3 (350) 11.0 (67) 33.9 (283) 1999 1.3 (23) 1.3 (12) 1.2 (11) 5.3 (105) 5.6 (55) 5.0 (50) 5.0 (56) 2.4 (13) 7.5 (43) 26.4 (337) 11.3 (66) 39.0 (271) 2000 1.4 (22) 0.8 (6) 2.0 (16) 4.3 (79) 3.8 (35) 4.8 (44) 5.8 (62) 2.8 (14) 8.4 (48) 26.8 (339) 11.9 (65) 38.0 (274) 2001 1.1 (18) 0.9 (7) 1.4 (11) 6.2 (114) 6.4 (60) 6.0 (54) 5.8 (64) 2.2 (11) 8.7 (53) 29.7 (377) 10.3 (57) 44.7 (319)

2002 1.9 (29) 1.4 (11) 2.3 (18) 7.0 (134) 7.6 (72) 6.5 (62) n.a n.a n.a n.a n.a n.a

2003 2.2 (36) 1.8 (15) 2.6 (21) 7.0 (137) 7.6 (76) 6.3 (61) n.a n.a n.a n.a n.a n.a

2004 2.8 (33) 2.4 (14) 3.1 (19) 4.9 (73) 5.3 (40) 4.4 (33) n.a n.a n.a n.a n.a n.a

2005 2.2 (29) 2.7 (18) 1.7 (11) 5.3 (85) 5.8 (47) 4.8 (38) n.a n.a n.a n.a n.a n.a

2006 2.8 (43) 3.2 (25) 2.4 (18) 6.8 (124) 7.4 (70) 6.1 (54) n.a n.a n.a n.a n.a n.a

2007 3.7 (48) 3.4 (22) 4.0 (26) 7.6 (115) 8.3 (62) 6.9 (53) n.a n.a n.a n.a n.a n.a

2008 3.7 (23) 4.7 (14) 2.8 (9) 6.7 (46) 6.9 (23) 6.5 (23) n.a n.a n.a n.a n.a n.a

Cochrane-Armitage Trend Test Two-sided

p < .001 p < .001 p = .003 p = .168 p = .218 p = .483 p = .144 p = .261 p = .003 p = < .001 p = .008 p = < .001

Note. SCARED Screen for Child Anxiety Related Emotional Disorders, sMFQ Short Mood and Feelings Questionnaire, SDQ Strength and Difficulties Questionnaire

a

Data on sex missing for two participants

Durbeej et al. BMC Psychology (2019) 7:50 Page 5 of 10

(7)

Parent-rated internalizing symptoms in nine-year old children born 1998 –2008

The regression analyses with the SCARED total score as the dependent variable were statistically significant for the total sample, girls and boys respectively (p < .001).

Additionally, the regression analyses with the sMFQ as the dependent variable were statistically significant for the total sample (p = .024) but not for boys and girls sep- arately. The positive beta-coefficients in the regressions reaching significance suggested positive relations be- tween birth cohorts and the SCARED and sMFQ re- spectively (see Table 2).

The Cochrane Armitage Trend Test revealed statisti- cally significant trends for a change in the percentage of participants with scores above cut-off on the SCARED for the total sample (p < 0.001), and for both boys (p <

0.001), and girls (p = 0.003) when their results were ana- lyzed separately. There were no such trends for the sMFQ (see Table 3 and Fig. 1).

Self-rated internalizing symptoms in 15 year old adolescents, born 1994 –2001

The regression analyses with the SDQ-Emotional parent- report version as the dependent variable were statistically significant for the total sample (p = .002) and for girls (p = .001) but not for boys. The analyses with the SDQ- Emotional self-report version 1as dependent variable yielded significant models for the total sample and for both boys and girls (p < .001). The positive beta-coeffi- cients in the regressions that reached statistical signifi- cance suggested positive relations between birth cohorts and outcomes (see Table 2). The Cochrane Armitage Tests also demonstrated statistically significant trends for a change in the percentage of participants with scores above cut-off on the SDQ-Emotional self-report version for the total sample (p = < .001), boys (p = .008) and girls (p < .001) respectively. For girls, there was a corresponding trend of scores above cut-off on the SDQ-Emotional par- ent version (p = .003). There were however, no such trends for the total sample, or for boys (see Table 3 and Fig. 2).

Fig. 1 Percentage of participants with scores above cut-offs for the parent-reported Screen for Child Anxiety Related Emotional Disorders (SCARED) (n = 324) and the parent-reported Short Mood and Feelings Questionnaire (sMFQ), (n = 1044), assessed at age 9

Fig. 2 Percentage of participants with scores above cut-offs for the emotional subscale of the Strength and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ)

parent- (n = 495) and self-report (n = 2551) versions assessed at age 15

(8)

Discussion

This study aimed to examine whether there are time trends in internalizing symptoms, in children and youth.

Epidemiological data from high-income countries has demonstrated increases in clinical parameters including diagnosis and treatment of anxiety and depression dur- ing the past decades [1]. The current study examines whether such clinical trends correspond to changing levels of internalizing symptoms in population-based samples, using cross-cohort comparisons. The results in- dicate some trends in levels of internalizing symptoms across birth cohorts. At age 9, the Cochrane Armitage Tests demonstrated an increasing percentage of partici- pants, both boys and girls, with scores above cut-off on anxiety symptoms. There was no such trend for the de- pressive symptoms. Considering the large sample size of this study, the latter must be reckoned a relatively sound finding. At age 15, there were trends in percentage of participants scoring above cut-off of self-reported intern- alizing symptoms, for the total sample as well as for boys and girls, respectively. There was a corresponding trend for parent-reported internalizing symptoms for girls but not for boys or for the total sample. Overall, the differ- ence between boys and girls in internalizing symptoms was negligible with regard to scores on the SCARED and sMFQ at age 9. However, there were sex differences with regard to scores on the SDQ parent and self-report scales at age 15 (small and large, respectively).

Levels of internalizing symptoms

At age 9, levels of parent-rated internalizing symptoms (i.e., symptoms of anxiety and depression assessed with SCARED and sMFQ, respectively; see mean values in Table 1) were very low. Overall, this is to be expected in a non-clinical sample of young children. Comparable levels (including prevalence of participants scoring above cut-off ) have been observed in another recent study on a non-clinical sample [33]. It is worth highlighting that our data is based on parent-reports, which could differ significantly from self-ratings by the children (e.g., par- ents potentially over- or under-estimating the distress of their child).

To our knowledge, there are few studies investigating depression and anxiety symptoms separately for boys and girls [2]. At age 15, girls self-reported substantially higher levels of internalizing symptoms than boys did.

Several studies have demonstrated that adolescents have an elevated risk for psychiatric symptoms overall [34].

Specifically, depression and anxiety disorders are typic- ally more common in girls than in boys, especially during adolescent years. This finding is also in line with the Swedish study on levels of psychosomatic health in 15,000 adolescents, demonstrating successive increases in psychosomatic health problems specifically in girls

across the study period [12]. Possible explanations for this sex difference might include increased school per- formance pressure, earlier sexual debut, weight and ap- pearance pressure among girls [2, 12, 34]. The levels of SDQ-scores in the current study seem in line with corre- sponding figures from previous international studies [35–37]. Even though the levels of internalizing symp- toms differed between boys and girls, it is worth noting the increasing percentage of participants with self-re- ported SDQ-scores above cut-off across time for both boys and girls. These trends could challenge the repli- cated findings in previous research that increasing levels of anxiety and depression symptoms during the past de- cades have been relatively specific to girls [1]. Clinicians might be more aware of assessing internalizing symp- toms in girls, with the risk of overlooking this set of symptoms in boys.

Percentages of participants exceeding cut-off scores At age 9, there was an increasing number of both boys and girls with scores above cut-offs on anxiety symp- toms. At age 15, there were increasing numbers of indi- viduals of both sexes exceeding cut-off for self-rated internalizing symptoms. However, such a trend for par- ent-rated internalizing symptoms was only detected for girls. This trend of increasing percentages exceeding cut- off scores on parent-rated internalizing symptoms was lacking for boys despite the large sample studied. Again, parent-ratings could potentially be underestimating true levels of anxiety and depression symptoms in children.

At age 15, the increasing percentage of participants ex- ceeding cut-off is prominent, especially for girls. At the end of the study period, 44.7% of 15-year old girls rated symptoms above cut-off, which is quite remarkable. This result might challenge 5 as a valid cut-off in Sweden when screening for clinical cases with SDQ Emotional in girls at this age. Trends of increasing percentages of par- ticipants with scores above cut-offs are relevant from a clinical perspective, since it distinguishes individuals with pronounced symptomatology from participants with milder symptoms.

In contrast to our findings, a meta-analysis of 26 epi- demiological studies based on clinical interviews, did not find evidence of increasing rates of child and adolescent depression during a 30-year observation period begin- ning in the mid 60’s [38]. Also, in a population-based study from Canada, where symptoms of mental illness were assessed bi-annually during the years 1994/1995 to 2008/2009 in large cohorts of Canadian children (N >

9000 in each cycle), there were no significant changes in mean scores on depression and anxiety across time for children 10–11 years old and 12–13 years old, but a sig- nificant and small increase across time for youth 14–15 years old [10]. Of note, the assessments in the current

Durbeej et al. BMC Psychology (2019) 7:50 Page 7 of 10

(9)

study were conducted the following decade (between 2007 and 2017 for age 9, and between 2009 and 2016 for age 15). As a clinical reference, there was a certain in- crease in a main diagnosis of depression among adoles- cents enrolled in the Stockholm Child and Adolescent Psychiatry (CAP) services: 6.2% of 15-year-old boys were assigned a main diagnosis of depression in 2011, with a corresponding figure of 7.2% in 2016. For girls, the fig- ures were 13.6, and 15.3%, for 2011 and 2016, respect- ively. Rising levels of internalizing symptoms in youth could indicate an increase of future psychiatric problems, especially if this does not correspond with awareness amongst parents. Increasing percentages of youth scoring above cut-off for internalizing problems may imply psy- chiatric problems at a level with need for specialized men- tal health services. This finding might reflect the observed increasing numbers of patients within CAP units.

Strengths and limitations

There are several unique aspects of the current study.

To our knowledge, this is the first population-based study with cross-cohort comparisons, using well-vali- dated scales to assess symptoms of anxiety and depres- sion separately. The CATSS-study is one of the most comprehensive twin studies of childhood mental and somatic health problems ever performed. Given the rela- tively high response rate (> 60% overall) and the large study cohorts, the sample can be considered repre- sentative of the Swedish population. Additionally, this study adds to previous literature by assessing large cohorts and investigating boys and girls separately.

Moreover, it includes later cohorts than previously re- ported in research [2, 11].

The study also had several limitations. Firstly, we did not have separate measures for anxiety and depression at age 15. This is due to sacrifices in terms of extent to prioritize response rate at the follow-ups of the CATSS study [13]. Secondly, there were different raters for par- ticipants aged 9 (parent-report) and 15 (self-and parent report). Self-reports of internalizing symptoms could be higher than parent-rated symptoms [33, 39, 42]. There- fore, parent-rated levels of internalizing symptoms at age 9 might be an underestimation of actual symptoms among our participants. However, there is some evi- dence that parent-information regarding symptoms of anxiety and depression in youth could be as valid as self- rated symptoms (see Table 4 in [40]), and may even be more informative than self-rated information for chil- dren specifically (see Table 1 in [41]) In Sweden, mental health problems among youth are low in relation to cor- responding figures from other industrialized countries [11]. Therefore, the results are not easily generalized to other cultural contexts. Generalizability from a twin- study might be questioned, even though several studies

have suggested that twins are representative of the popu- lation at large [43, 44] and that monozygotic and dizyg- otic twins are similar in personality variation [45].

Previous research on the CATSS population has demon- strated that participants have a higher socioeconomic status compared to non-participants [13]. In population- based surveys, higher levels of psychological problems have been demonstrated in children whose parents have a lower, in contrast to a higher, educational degree [11].

Therefore, the levels of internalizing problems in the current study might not be generalizable to a socioeco- nomically more diverse population. However, a trend of decreasing response rates would rather underestimate internalizing problems in the present study.

Finally, it is not possible to delineate whether the aug- mented levels of internalizing symptoms in the current study reflect a real increase in symptoms, an increased tendency to recognize or report these symptoms, or a com- bination of both. Future longitudinal studies utilizing regis- ter-based data linked with self-report should explore whether augmented levels of internalizing symptoms cor- respond with increasing levels of health care consumption.

To our knowledge, this is the first study using well- validated measures to assess internalizing symptoms, i.e.

both anxiety and depressive symptoms, across consecu- tive birth cohorts of youth. The results demonstrated some trends in internalizing symptoms across birth co- horts, for both boys and girls.

Conclusions

In the current study including a large Swedish twin sam- ple, slight increases in parent-reported internalizing symptoms at age 9 were observed across the years 2007 –2017 for both boys and girls. At age 15, there was an increase in self-reported internalizing symptoms, dur- ing the years 2009 –2016 for both boys and girls. This study contributes to the field through its methodological strengths; separate assessments of symptoms of depres- sion and anxiety in representative community samples using repeated cross-sectional assessments.

Abbreviations

CATSS: Child and Adolescent Twin Study in Sweden; DSM: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders; MFQ: Mood and Feelings Questionnaire; SCARED: Screen for Child Anxiety Related Emotional Disorders; SDQ: Strength and Difficulties Questionnaire; sMFQ: Short Mood and Feelings Questionnaire; STR: Swedish Twin Registry

Acknowledgements

Not applicable. The study was presented at the NordCAP meeting, Turku, Finland, August 30-31, 2018.

Authors ’ contributions

LH, CH, PL, SL, ENS, KS and ND contributed to the design of the work. ND and SL performed the analyses. ND, KS, ENS and LH interpreted the results.

LH, CH, PL, SL, ENS, KS and ND drafted the manuscript. All authors provided

feedback on the manuscript and also read and approved the final version.

(10)

Funding

The Child and Adolescent Twin Study in Sweden study was supported by the Swedish Council for Working Life, funds under the ALF agreement, the Söderström-Königska Foundation, and the Swedish Research Council (Medicine, Humanities and Social Science, grant number 2017 –02552, and SIMSAM).

Availability of data and materials

Datasets are available on reasonable request from the corresponding author.

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The CATSS has ethical approval from the Regional Ethical Review Board of Stockholm (#03 –672, 2010/507–31/1; 2010/1356/31/1). All 15-year old study participants and participating parents provided written consents.

Consent for publication Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Author details

1

Department of Clinical Neuroscience, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden.

2

Department of Public Health and Caring Sciences, Uppsala University, Uppsala, Sweden.

3

Centre for Clinical Research, County of Västmanland, Uppsala University, Uppsala, Sweden.

4

Institute of Neuroscience and Physiology, Gillberg Neuropsychiatry Centre, Centre of Ethics Law and Mental Health, Gothenburg University, Gothenburg, Sweden.

5

Department of Medical Epidemiology and Biostatistics, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden.

6

Child and Adolescent Psychiatry Research center, BUP Klinisk forskningsenhet, Stockholm, Sweden.

7

Department of Clinical Science, Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Umeå University, SE-901 87 Umeå, Sweden.

Received: 9 November 2018 Accepted: 18 July 2019

References

1. Collishaw S. Annual research review: secular trends in child and adolescent mental health. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2015;56(3):370 –93.

2. Bor W, Dean AJ, Najman J, Hayatbakhsh R. Are child and adolescent mental health problems increasing in the 21st century? A systematic review. Aust NZ J Psychiat. 2014;48(7):606 –16.

3. Klasen F, Otto C, Kriston L, et al. Risk and protective factors for the development of depressive symptoms in children and adolescents: results of the longitudinal BELLA study. Eur Child Adoles Psy. 2015;24(6):695 –703.

4. Schulte-Körne G. Mental health problems in a school setting in children and adolescents. Dtsch Arztebl Int. 2016;113(11):183 –90.

5. Weeks M, Ploubidis GB, Cairney J, et al. Developmental pathways linking childhood and adolescent internalizing, externalizing, academic competence, and adolescent depression. J Adoles. 2016;51:30 –40.

6. Herpertz-Dahlmann B, Bühren K, Remschmidt H. Growing up is hard: mental disorders in adolescence. Dtsch Arztebl Int. 2013;110(25):432 –9.

7. Lundervold AJ, Breivik K, Posserud MB, et al. Symptoms of depression as reported by Norwegian adolescents on the short mood and feelings questionnaire. Front Psychol. 2013;4:613.

8. Chaplin TM, Aldao O. Gender differences in emotion expression in children:

a meta-analytic review. Psychol Bull. 2013;139(4):735 –65.

9. Achenbach TM, Ivanova MY, Rescorla LA, Turner LV, Althoff RR. Internalizing/

externalizing problems: review and recommendations for clinical and research applications. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2016;55(8):647 –56.

10. McMartin SE, Kingsbury M, Dykxhoorn J, et al. Time trends in symptoms of mental illness in children and adolescents in Canada. CMAJ. 2014;186(18):

E672 –8.

11. Bremberg S, Dalman C. Begrepp, mätmetoder och förekomst av psykisk hälsa, psykisk ohälsa och psykiatriska tillstånd hos barn och unga. Formans, Forte, Vetenskapsrådet, Vinnova; 2015.

12. Hagquist C. Psychosomatic health problems among adolescents in Sweden--are the time trends gender related? Eur J Pub Health. 2009;19(3):

331 –6.

13. Anckarsäter H, Lundström S, Kollberg L, et al. The child and adolescent twin study in Sweden (CATSS). Twin Res Hum Genet. 2011;14(6):495 –508.

14. Birmaher B, Khetarpal S, Brent D, et al. The screen for child anxiety related emotional disorders (SCARED): scale construction and psychometric characteristics. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1997;36(4):545 –53.

15. Birmaher B, Brent DA, Chiapetta L, et al. Psychometric properties of the Screen for Child Anxiety Related Emotional Disorders (SCARED): a replication study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1999;38(10):1230 –6.

16. De Sousa DA, Zibetti MR, Trentini CM, et al. Screen for child anxiety related emotional disorders: are subscale scores reliable? A bifactor model analysis.

J Anxiety Disord. 2014;28(8):966 –70.

17. Chan SM, Leung CH. Factor structure of the screen for child anxiety-related emotional disorders (SCARED) in a community sample of Hong Kong Chinese adolescents. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. 2015;46(5):671 –82.

18. Hale WW 3rd, Crocetti E, Raaijmakers QA, et al. A meta-analysis of the cross- cultural psychometric properties of the screen for child anxiety related emotional disorders (SCARED). J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2011;52(1):80 –90.

19. Hariz N, Bawab S, Atwi M, et al. Reliability and validity of the Arabic screen for child anxiety related emotional disorders. Psychiatry Res. 2013;209(2):

222 –8.

20. Arab A, El Keshky M, Hadwin JA. Psychometric properties of the screen for child anxiety related emotional disorders (SCARED) in a non-clinical sample of children and adolescents in Saudi Arabia. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev.

2015;47(4):554 –62.

21. McNeish D. Thanks coefficient alpha, We ’ll take it from here. Psychol Methods. 2018;23(3):412 –33.

22. Angold A, Costello EJ, Messer SC, et al. Development of a short questionnaire for use in epidemiological studies of depression in children and adolescents. Int J Meth Psych Res. 1995;5:237 –49.

23. Angold A, Costello EJ, Pickles EJ, et al. The development of a questionnaire for use in epidemiological studies of depression in children and adolescents. London: Medical research council; 1987.

24. Angold A, Erkanli A, Silberg J, et al. Depression scale scores in 8-17-year olds: effects of age and gender. J Child Psychol Psych. 2002;43:1052 –63.

25. Kent L, Vostanis P, Feehan C. Detection of major and minor depression in children and adolescents: evaluation of the mood and feelings questionnaire. J Child Psychol Psych. 1997;38:565 –73.

26. Messer SC, Angold A, Costello E, et al. Development of a short questionnaire for use in epidemiological studies of depression in children and adolescents. Factor composition and structure across development. Int J Method Psychiatr Res. 1995;5:251 –562.

27. Thapar A, McGuffin P. Validity of the shortened mood and feelings ques- tionnaire in a community sample of children and adolescents: a preliminary research note. Psychiatry Res. 1998;81:259 –68.

28. Rhew IC, Simpson K, Tracy M, et al. Criterion validity of the short mood and feelings questionnaire and one- and two-item depression screens in young adolescents. Child Adolesc Psychiatry Ment Health. 2010;4(1):8.

29. Martinsen KD, Neumer SP, Holen S, et al. Self-reported quality of life and self-esteem in sad and anxious school children. BMC Psychol. 2016;4(1):45.

30. Goodman R. The strengths and difficulties questionnaire: a research note. J Child Psychol Psych. 1997;38(5):581 –6.

31. Gustafsson BM, Proczkowska-Björklund M, Gustafsson PA. Emotional and behavioural problems in Swedish preschool children rated by preschool teachers with the strengths and difficulties questionnaire (SDQ). BMC Pediatr. 2017;17(1):110.

32. Smedje H, Broman JE, Hetta J, et al. Psychometric properties of a Swedish version of the "Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire". Eur Child Adoles Psy. 1999;8(2):63 –70.

33. Goetz M, Sebela A, Mohaplova M, et al. Psychiatric disorders and quality of life in the offspring of parents with bipolar disorder. J Child Adolesc Psychopharmacol. 2017;27(6):483 –93.

34. Ullsperger JM, Nikolas MA. A meta-analytic review of the association between pubertal timing and psychopathology in adolescence: are there sex differences in risk? Psychol Bull. 2017;143(9):903 –38.

35. Meltzer H, Gatward R, Goodman R, et al. Mental health of children and adolescents in Great Britain. London: The Stationery Office; 2000.

36. Rahman SM, Kippler M, Tofail F, et al. Manganese in drinking water and cognitive abilities and behavior at 10 years of age: a prospective cohort study. Environ Health Perspect. 2017;125(5):057003.

37. De Vries PJ, Davids EL, Mathews C, et al. Measuring adolescent mental health around the globe: psychometric properties of the self-report strengths and difficulties questionnaire in South Africa, and comparison with UK, Australian and Chinese data. Epidemiol Psychiatr Sci. 2017;23:1 –12.

Durbeej et al. BMC Psychology (2019) 7:50 Page 9 of 10

(11)

38. Costello EJ, Erkanli A, Angold A. Is there an epidemic of child or adolescent depression? J Child Psychol Psych. 2006;47(12):1263 –71.

39. Dougherty ER, Smith VC, Bufferd SJ, et al. Preschool irritability predicts child psychopathology, functional impairment, and service use at age nine. J Child Psychol Psych. 2015;56(9):999 –1007.

40. Jensen PS, Rubio-Stipec M, Canino G, Bird HR, Dulcan MK, Schwab-Stone ME, Lahey BB. Parent and child contributions to diagnosis of mental disorder: are both informants always necessary? J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1999;38(12):1569 –79.

41. Smith SR. Making sense of multiple informants in child and adolescent psychopathology: a guide for clinicians. J Psychoed Assessment. 2007;25(2):

139 –49.

42. Cleridou K, Patalay P, Martin P. Does parent –child agreement vary based on presenting problems? Results from a UK clinical sample. Child Adolesc Psychiatry Ment Health. 2017;11:22.

43. Evans DM, Martin NG. The validity of twin studies. GeneScreen. 2000;1:77 –9.

44. Barnes JC, Boutwell BB. A demonstration of the generalizability of twin- based research on antisocial behavior. Behav Genet. 2013;43(2):120 –31.

45. Johnson W, Krueger R, Bouchard TJ Jr, et al. The personalities of twins: just ordinary folks. Twin Res. 2002;5(2):125 –31.

Publisher ’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in

published maps and institutional affiliations.

References

Related documents

This project focuses on the possible impact of (collaborative and non-collaborative) R&amp;D grants on technological and industrial diversification in regions, while controlling

Analysen visar också att FoU-bidrag med krav på samverkan i högre grad än när det inte är ett krav, ökar regioners benägenhet att diversifiera till nya branscher och

9 To begin with, in section 2.1, we use the sources of statistics above together with Statistics Sweden’s regular R&amp;D survey 10 to describe how R&amp;D and employment

Svenska Föreningen för Barn- och Ungdomspsykiatri Swedish Association for Child and Adolescent

[r]

Second only to death by accident, suicide is currently the most common cause of death among Swedish adolescents and young adults (males and females) in the 15–25 age group [1]..

Jämtland CAP patients registered for criminality needed general psychiatric care as adults due to diagnoses of sub- stance-related disorders and personality disorders, it seems

Comparison of the patients who were and were not given an ICD-10 diagnosis of schizophrenia and/or psychotic mood [affective] disorder in connection with CAP care