• No results found

What happens in the shadows? C-Thesis

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "What happens in the shadows? C-Thesis"

Copied!
47
0
0

Loading.... (view fulltext now)

Full text

(1)

What happens in the shadows?

A quantitative analysis on the effect of shadows in

baseball.

C-Thesis

Author: Ström, Martin Supervisor: Pojskic, Haris Examiner: Carlsson, Bo

(2)
(3)

Abstract

Baseball is one of the most statistically documented sports in the world. Every statistical outcome in baseball starts with the pitcher and the plate appearance.

In baseball, it is believed that when shadows are present between the pitcher’s mound and the batter’s box, the pitcher is at an advantage. Therefore, the aim of the study was to identify if there is an advantage for pitchers pitching with shadows separating the pitcher’s mound from the batter’s box. Only games from Major League Baseball in which the shadows were present between the pitcher’s mound and batter’s box were analyzed. Analyzed variables were comprised of traditional statistical outcomes categorized as good or bad outcomes. Furthermore, good and bad outcomes were analyzed using their ordinal subcategories rated from 1 to 4. Differences between good and bad outcomes of plate appearances, when shadows were and were not present, was analyzed using a Mann-Whitney U Test.

The results of the study indicate that shadows do not have a significant effect on the outcome of plate appearances. Moreover, pitchers do not have an advantage pitching while shadows are present between them and the batter’s box. Frequencies of outcomes with shadows present was much the same to outcomes without the shadows present. In conclusion, it does not appear that shadows influence the outcome of plate appearances. However, further research on statistical metrics and their effect on plate appearances is necessary.

Key words

Statistics; outcome; plate appearance; pitcher; batter;

(4)

Acknowledgments

To Haris,

Whose unrelenting support is the only reason this was possible. I could not have asked for a better supervisor. Thank you.

To my mom and dad,

Who has always supported me, believed in me, and pushed me to go as far as possible.

You introduced me to this beautiful game, and for that, I am forever grateful.

I love you both.

(5)

Table of contents

1 Introduction 1

1.1 Background 2

1.1.1 An introduction to statistics 2

1.2 Objective and research questions 4

1.2.1 Objective 4

1.2.2 Research questions 5

2 Previous research 6

2.1 General research on baseball statistics 6

2.2 Research on advantages 7

2.3 Research on the pitcher and batter subgame 7

2.4 Research on shadows in baseball 8

2.5 Summary of previous research 9

3 Method 10

3.1 Study design 10

3.2 Sampling of data 11

3.2.1 Alignment of Major League Ballparks 12

3.2.2 Angles of sunlight 13

3.3 Processing of data 14

3.3.1 Definition of good outcomes 15

3.3.2 Definition of bad outcomes 16

3.4 Analysis of data 17

3.5 Research ethics considerations 17

4 Results 18

5 Discussion 20

5.1 Discussion of results 20

5.2 Discussion of method 23

5.2.1 Discussion of variables 24

5.2.2 Discussion of sources 25

5.2.3 Delimitations and limitations 25

6 Conclusions 27

6.1 Implications for further research 28

7 References 30

8 Appendix 36

8.1 Appendix 1 – Result of Shapiro-Wilk Test for normality in sampled data 36 8.2 Appendix 2 - Result of Mann-Whitney U test for difference in rating of

pitcher’s outcome variables 37

8.3 Appendix 3 - Result of Mann-Whitney U test for difference in good versus

bad outcomes. 38

8.4 Appendix 4 - Result of Mann-Whitney U test for difference in rating

between all good outcomes. 39

8.5 Appendix 5 - Result of Mann-Whitney U test for difference in rating

between all bad outcomes. 40

(6)
(7)

Appendices

Appendix 1 – Result of Shapiro-Wilk Test for normality in sampled data

Appendix 2 - Result of Mann-Whitney U test for difference in rating of pitcher’s outcome variables

Appendix 3 - Result of Mann-Whitney U test for difference in good versus bad outcomes.

Appendix 4 - Result of Mann-Whitney U test for difference in rating between all good outcomes.

Appendix 5 - Result of Mann-Whitney U test for difference in rating between all bad outcomes.

(8)

1 Introduction

This section presents a background to baseball, as well as the objective and research questions of the study.

Known as America’s national pastime, baseball has a long-standing historical and sociological connection to the country. Walt Whitman famously claimed that baseball fits as much into America’s constitutions and is as important to its total historical life, as any other institution in the country (Rader, 2008). It has shaped and been shaped by the collective minds of America since the first modern game in 1846, reaching every household and every heart of its citizens.

It is and has always been, a mirror of society, thus portraying the joy and hardships of America in every way imaginable (Hoffmann et al., 2003). From Jackie Robinson breaking the color barrier to the 1919 Black Sox Scandal; to the steroid era of the early 2000s and the Cubs finally ending their curse, baseball has had its fair share of legends and drama (Rader, 2008).

Although baseball is “America’s game” (Rader, 2008), it has not only crossed barriers domestically but internationally as well. During and after the Civil War, while the game spread across the states, it also reached the Caribbean and Central America in the 1860s. It reached China in 1863, while Japan and South Korea got their first taste of the sport in the early 1870s (Kelly, 2007).

The international spread of the sport predates soccer but is severely outclassed in the number of global players and interest. It lacks both the attendance and recognition to compete with a sport such as soccer on the world stage, perhaps due to the nationalization of the sport as America’s pastime, rather than the world’s (Kelly, 2007).

Baseball is played between two teams, each consisting of nine players, taking turns to play offense and defense. A game consists of nine innings and the winner is decided by whichever team has scored the most runs after said nine innings. To score runs, the offensive team’s batters attempt to hit the ball

(9)

thrown by the defensive team’s pitcher. If a player from the offensive team can hit the ball and safely make his way across all four bases, they score a run.

The defense attempts to get the offense out by throwing, catching, tagging, or striking the offensive team’s players out. When the defensive team gets three outs, the teams swap sides and defense becomes offense and vice versa. When both teams have had their three outs playing defense, one inning has passed.

The teams continue to swap between offense and defense for nine innings, or until a winner can be decided. If the teams are tied after nine innings, the game goes into extra innings and each team gets one more inning to try and score. If the teams are still tied after each extra inning, the game simply continues one inning at a time, until a winner is decided (Albert et al., 2005).

Within the game, what is known as the pitcher and batter subgame becomes the starting point of any play and the only part of baseball where the two teams truly “face” each other (Alamar et al., 2006). Due to this, baseball is the most individual of all team sports (Kelly, 2007). Players on the same team only interact through throwing the ball to each other or helping each other along the bases through their batting. Because of its nature, statistical analysis has been a key part of the game since as early as the 19th century (Wikipedia, 2021a).

Unlike any other sport, statistics guide managers, players, and fans alike. From broadcasts to stadiums, from the Hall of Fame to Little League, not a single part of baseball is unaffected by statistics.

Between all aspects of the game, statistics are what truly made me fall in love with baseball, and it is on that love that this thesis is based.

1.1 Background

1.1.1 An introduction to statistics

One fundamental aspect of baseball that separates it from many other sports is the sheer amount of numerical data recorded about the game. Unlike most

(10)

sports, the outcomes of the most common baseball event, a plate appearance, is easy to evaluate as there are not a great number of differentiating outcomes.

The plate appearance is the start and finish of any game, where the batter steps up to the plate to face the pitcher. Almost the entirety of the game depends on the outcome of these plate appearances, thus making them highly relevant to analyze from a statistical standpoint.

Furthermore, statistics in baseball are often used to decide the game’s best hitter or pitcher of a certain season as well as serving as a tool for salary arbitration and negotiating new contracts. The best hitter of a single season, for example, is decided by the amount of hits accumulated divided by the number of at-bats, or opportunities, the player had to get those hits. The result of this equation is the metric known as batting average. Similarly, the best pitcher is often decided by utilizing several metrics such as earned-run average, strikeouts, or wins. These examples are just the tip of the iceberg of baseball analysis and presently, most professional teams have hired analysts to provide statistical assistance to the team’s strategy during the season (Albert et al., 2005).

Recently, the rise of Sabermetrics, a term first introduced by baseball writer Bill James in 1977, has shown that advanced mathematical formulas can find underappreciated and undervalued players where traditional statistics fail to do so (Caporale & Collier, 2012). The impact of Sabermetrics and advanced statistical evaluation of players have even made it to the big screens, with the 2011 film adaptation of Michael Lewis’ book Moneyball (IMDB, 2011).

Moneyball depicts the 2002 Oakland Athletics’ attempt to build a playoff winning team on a small-market budget, using Bill James’ Sabermetrics, to compete with big-market teams. Since 2002, Sabermetrics is widely utilized and accepted as common practice for all professional baseball teams. As the sport continues to develop, so too will the statistical elements continue to proliferate and evolve into new, advanced metrics.

(11)

There are elements of statistics in baseball, which sometimes fail to tell the full story. As will be discussed in this thesis, the outcome of events is sometimes difficult to credit any one player. In baseball, for example, pitchers can achieve the feat of a no-hitter or a perfect game, by not giving up a single hit or getting all 27 batters out in a row, respectively. The pitcher gets credited with the achievement, but the feat itself depends on many other factors. The opposing team’s offensive prowess, the defensive fielder’s abilities, or, most importantly, the catcher’s ability to call the game, all play a part in the pitcher earning his achievement (Grosshandler, N.D). For the sake of this thesis, which examines the outcome between pitcher and batter, the discussion of where credit is due, is highly relevant.

Baseball’s many facets cannot be fully comprehended or appreciated in a single study. From the statistics to the traditions to the wonderful smell of green grass on opening day, each aspect of the game has a unique place amongst the hearts of Americans and fans from all over the world. The evolution of baseball statistics and its many aspects is the foundation for this study, which is focused on a microscopical part of baseball statistics to shed light on one of the game’s great mysteries: shadows.

1.2 Objective and research questions

1.2.1 Objective

The shadows affecting pitchers and batters during the early days of spring and the late afternoons of October seem to be a popular, yet an under-researched area of the sport (Branch, 2019; Schifman, 2019; Swartz, 2009). It seems, however, to be a topic of discussion by broadcasters and fans alike, whenever the weather phenomenon occurs (Major League Baseball, 2021a). The consensus appears to be that whenever the batter’s box is in the shadows, while the pitcher’s mound remains in the sun, the batter will be at a disadvantage.

As one study shows (Schifman, 2019) there does not seem to be a correlation

(12)

between strike outs, walks, and the positioning of the sun. This study, however, generalized plate appearances over a full season based on the position of the sun.

What the study fails to take into consideration is the difference in single games, when the shadows both are and are not present over the course of the game. In any single game, where the same pitcher and same batters face the issue of shadows, can the outcome of plate appearances be because of the shadows?

The objective of this study is to analyze Major League Baseball games, where the weather phenomenon both is and is not present. Using quantitative methods, the outcome of each plate appearance will be analyzed by dividing them into good outcomes and bad outcomes, from the pitcher’s perspective.

By comparing plate appearances with and without shadows in play, I will attempt to prove if shadows truly are an advantage to any team on the field.

1.2.2 Research questions

The research questions for this study are based on this objective and are as follows:

• Is there an advantage for pitchers when the shadows separate the batter’s box from the pitcher’s mound?

• Should Major League teams alter their in-game strategy based on the position of shadows?

(13)

2 Previous research

In this section, previous research on baseball statistics relevant to the objective and research questions are presented.

2.1 General research on baseball statistics

Statistical research in baseball goes back to the 19th century. This data, however, was not widely spread amongst the public until the 1950s, when Hy Turkin published his book The Complete Encyclopedia of Baseball (Wikipedia, 2021a). Since then, statistics have been an instrument for all aspects of the game. From recording any event in a game to computing advanced models for predicting outcomes and developing methods to analyze traditional statistics in modern systems. Any fan watching a ballgame will be exposed to statistics, either at the ballpark itself or through the broadcast with updating panels and graphs. While serving a vital role in the player’s careers (Anstrom & Eisenstein, 2011), statistics are quite literally an everyday phenomenon as 2,430 games are played every year, not including pre- or postseason play.

Arth and Billings (2020) claim that no other sport has such a downright difference between traditional and modern statistics as baseball. This is further established by Davis (N.D.) who defines the best baseball players the past 69 years based on advanced metrics such as Wins Above Replacement (WAR), On Base Percantage plus Slugging Percantage+ (OPS+), and Adjusted Earned Run Advantage+ (ERA+). Rather than applying traditional statistics to his weighting, Davis, who has been a baseball fan since 1952, is using metrics not widely established until the early 2000s as a basis for his rankings. As also stated by Butterworth (2010) baseball is perhaps the most traditionally rooted of all sports. The differentiation of old and new, combined with the immense amount of data collected since as early as 1876, further exploration of modern angles to analyze the game is a highly regarded and common practice amongst scholars and baseball experts.

(14)

2.2 Research

on

advantages

As presented by Jones (2015), the study of advantages in baseball is a common element of the game, much like any other sport. In his article, he applies a quantitative method to describe the difference in home-field advantage between baseball and other popular team sports. Further examples of looking to decipher value in advantages are studies by Fagan et al., (2018) and Winter et al. (2009), who examine left-handed advantages in individual sports and advantages based on time lost due to circadian synchronization, respectively.

Similar quantitative methods are used by all the authors above, which argues the fact that quantitative methods are optimal in studies measuring advantages, such as this one. The articles also illustrate how both specificity and broadness are common for statistical baseball research. Examining the advantages between pitcher and batter based on the position of shadows could therefore be argued to be in line with previous research in this area.

2.3 Research on the pitcher and batter subgame

Like advantages are a part of this thesis’ objective, so is the aspect of pitcher versus batter. The showdown between pitcher and batter is the foundation of baseball, as all plays are set in motion by the pitcher. Furthermore, it is the most researched and statistically documented event of baseball (Baseball Reference, N.D.a) as it is the basis of the sport. As with advantages, the research on pitchers and batters ranges from traditional, statistical documentation of the outcome of plate appearances to predicting the path of a pitch (Bahill et al., 2005), or isolating the pitcher and batter subgame completely (Alamar et al., 2006). The relevance of research on the pitcher and batter matchup not only derives from its importance on the outcome of games, but it is also the base of all other statistics of the game, such as baserunning or defense.

Alamar et al. (2006) explain how the outcome of the pitcher and batter subgame influences a statistic known as Net Expected Run Value or NERV.

(15)

NERV is used to explain the value of any plate appearance. What is problematic according to the author, is whether an outcome of a plate appearance can be fully credited to either pitcher or batter. If, for example, a fielder makes an exceptional defensive play the credit to change in NERV of that plate appearance would be because of the fielder, rather than the pitcher.

Previous research on NERV, however, only takes the batter and pitcher into consideration in calculating value, which means additional metrics are needed to understand why plate appearances have certain outcomes.

If a defensive play is exceptional or not, can be measured using statistics such as Batting Average on Balls In Play, or BABIP for short (Slowinski, 2010a).

The combination of statistics such as NERV and BABIP would explain whether a pitcher is credible for the outcome of a plate appearance, or if aspects such as defensive talent or luck played a part. Examples such as this, with multiple levels of statistical metrics being applied, illustrate the difficulty of crediting pitchers or batters with the outcome of plate appearances. For this thesis, which will specifically analyze the outcome of plate appearances between pitcher and batter, acknowledging that other statistical metrics may influence such outcomes will be a central point of discussion.

2.4 Research on shadows in baseball

As previously stated, although baseball statistics is a thoroughly researched topic, not much has been done on the topic of shadows. The weather, however, is a prevalent factor in baseball, as rainouts are a common occurrence over the course of a season. For this reason, 8 out of 30 teams have opted to build domes, rather than open-air stadiums (Wikipedia, 2021b). Other environmental factors such as wind, sunlight, and shadows are also more controllable in domes, creating a more consistent environment for players (Goodman & McAndrew, 1993). Therefore, games that are played in domes are never affected by shadows, meaning that they are exempt from this study.

(16)

Only one study (Schifman, 2019) has examined similar weather phenomena to this thesis. Schifman analyzed the position of the sun to see if it influenced strikeouts and walks. By using generalized boosted regression models, Schifman summarized over 60’000 randomly-sorted plate appearances over the course of one season. With the article, the author concluded that the sun’s angles did not have a significant effect on strikeouts or walks. Schifman also claims that this in turn means that shadows do not affect the outcome of plate appearances.

What the author fails to take into consideration are games where shadows both are and are not in play. The author also limits his research to only strikeouts and walks, not acknowledging any other outcomes of the pitcher and batter subgame. Both aspects will be highly relevant for this thesis, as they are key to answering the research questions.

2.5 Summary of previous research

Studies regarding advantages, pitcher evaluation, and modeling of outcomes have been thoroughly published throughout the years, with each new study assessing a different aspect of the game (Anstrom & Eisenstein, 2011;

Bradbury & Forman, 2012; Jensen et al., 2009; Jones, 2015; Huber &

Sturdivant, 2010). In different ways, these articles illustrate both the relevance of research regarding topics related to this thesis, as well as emphasize the purpose of further examining minuscule aspects of the game, such as the shadows. While previous research on baseball statistics is extensive, there is a gap specifically regarding the objective of this thesis. By analyzing how pitchers perform in games where shadows vary, this thesis will attempt to fill the existing gap in research.

(17)

3 Method

The method in this study is presented through a broad study design, as well as the sampling, processing, and analysis of data.

3.1 Study design

With regards to the statistical nature of the objective presented in this thesis, quantitative method was deemed the most appropriate approach to answer the research questions. By using content analysis (Gratton & Jones, 2010), the aim was to investigate the outcome of plate appearances based on the position of shadows.

In the first phase of the study, statistical data was collected from past games in Major League Baseball. The collected data was retrieved from the website Baseball-Reference, which advertises itself as “The complete source for current and historical baseball players, teams, scores and leaders.” (Baseball Reference, N.D.a). The website collects and presents all numerical outcomes and variables from Major League Baseball games since as early as 1876.

Another website used was MLB Film Room, as it provided necessary videos and clips from as early as 2017. These videos were used to confirm whether the conditions for the data collected were met, in the games that were analyzed.

These conditions will be further presented in the sampling section of this chapter.

In the second phase, the collected data was processed to define the relevant variables for this study. Since the objective of the study was to analyze the outcome of plate appearances, the data was divided into good and bad outcomes from the pitcher’s perspective, as well as outcome ratings of these outcomes. In addition, shadows were defined as an independent variable, as it is a dichotomous phenomenon: present, or not present.

In the third and final phase, the processed data was analyzed in SPSS, 25.0 (IBM SPSS Statistics, New York, USA). The Shapiro-Wilk Test was used to

(18)

determine the normality of the outcome variables in the sampled data. The Mann-Whitney U-test was also applied to illustrate any possible difference in the pitcher’s outcome variables when the shadow was and was not present.

These phases are explained in further detail below.

3.2 Sampling of data

As stated by Gratton and Jones (2010), data collected for quantitative studies need to be sampled to properly represent the population of all available data.

Since the total sum of all baseball games was too large to analyze, data for this study were sampled according to certain conditions. Firstly, the observed sample was defined as any Major League Baseball game, in which a pitcher threw at minimum three innings, and the shadows had to be both in and out of play. Shadows being in play was defined as when the shadows are present at any point between the pitcher’s mound and the batter’s box, as seen in figure 1. This definition is in line with previous research on the shadows affecting play, as stated by Schifman (2019). Other sources, such as broadcasters or general news articles that discuss the phenomenon also concur on this definition (The New York Times, 2009; Major League Baseball, 2021).

Figure 1 - Shadows in play (Redbirdrants, 2009)

(19)

If the pitcher remains in the sun, and the batter’s box in the shadow, the shadows are in play. Any other weather condition would deem the shadows not being in play. If the weather suddenly got cloudy, for example, the shadows would cover the whole field, thus not separating the pitcher and batter resulting in the shadows not being in play.

After the inclusion criteria had been established, Baseball Reference in combination with MLB Film Room were utilized to find games that matched the established conditions. Due to the conditions, the sampling method used for the study was systematic, to find any specific game that matched the population. A systematic sampling method is a process of defining specific inclusion criteria by which all samples used in the study must abide (Gratton

& Jones, 2010). However, since MLB Film Rooms database only records pitches as far back as 2017, all data collected in this study are from games 2017 and onwards.

Since there are over 2400 games played every year in the Major Leagues, several aspects affecting the weather conditions were identified to further define which games matched the inclusion criteria. In total, 8 games were found matching the inclusion criteria for this study. Within these 8 games, 13 different pitching performances and a total of 319 plate appearances were analyzed.

3.2.1 Alignment of Major League Ballparks

In an article by Kagan (2014) the author explains how baseball stadiums are built in accordance to rule 1.04 of the official rules of Major League Baseball (2019) which states:

“It is desirable that the line from home base through the pitchers plate to second base shall run East-Northeast.” (Major League Baseball, 2019)

Rule 1.04 determines at what angle the sun hits the field at certain times of the day. Using data from Google Maps, Kagan established how each of the 30

(20)

ballparks aligned to the rule, as seen in figure 2. As most baseball games start between 1:00 P.M. and 10:00 P.M. Eastern Time, ballparks that closely match the rule would have the sun directly behind home plate between 3.00 and 4.00 P.M. Eastern Time. Thusly, games that start too early, or too late for the shadows to be in play were disregarded for the study.

In figure 2, the red arrows represent teams whose ballparks are called domes, which are built with a permanent or retractable roof, meaning that the weather has almost no effect on the games (Goodman &

McAndrew, 1993). Games that were played in these ballparks were disregarded for the study.

Additionally, Baseball Reference provides information on any game’s weather condition since at least 2017.

Games that were not defined as

“sunny” could be disregarded before consulting MLB Film Room to confirm if the shadows are in play at any point of the game since no sun means no shadows.

3.2.2 Angles of sunlight

Another factor that proved important in sampling specific games was the time of year at which the games are played. As the Major League Baseball season stretches from late March to late October, sometimes even as late as early November, the angle of the sun changes throughout the season. Schroeder (2011) explains in his article how the sunlight hits the ground at different angles at different times of the year. Late March and late September, for

Figure 2 - Alignment of MLB Ballparks (Kagan, 2014)

(21)

example, are close to the equinoxes, meaning that sunlight hits the ground at a larger area than they do in June, which is closer to the solstice.

The relevance of considering the sun’s angles is because it is the ballparks themselves that cast the shadows on the field. If the sun is at too high of an angle when baseball is played, fewer shadows will be created on the ground.

Games that are played closer to the equinoxes, however, will cause ballparks to create more shadows on the ground, meaning that such games are more probable to have shadows in play than those played in the middle of summer.

By utilizing the starting time of games, type of ballpark, time of year, and weather conditions as metrics to find games matching the inclusion criteria, the efficiency of the method was improved.

3.3 Processing of data

When the sampling process was completed, the processing of data could commence. To establish a method for analyzing the data, plate appearances in the games selected for the study had to be coded. The coding process was based on the research objective and questions, meaning that plate appearances were sorted by several aspects. These were shadows in play, outcome of plate appearances, home or away, and rating of the outcome.

For every game selected, one pitcher on either or both teams had to pitch at minimum three innings, and the shadows had to be both in and out of play during these innings. For every game, each batter the pitcher faced was numbered from 1 going up to as many total batters he faced in that game. This was established through Baseball Reference and its record of the games chosen for the study. To establish which of these plate appearances the shadows were in play for, MLB Film Room was utilized. By searching the database for every single plate appearance in each specific game, clips and videos showed whether the shadows were in play. If the shadows were in play, that plate appearance was marked as 1. If not, it was marked as 0. Based on this, a

(22)

categorical, dichotomous variable was created, due to shadows only having two levels of variance.

As seen in Table 1, the outcome of each plate appearance was rated as good or bad, from the pitcher’s and defensive team’s perspective. A good outcome was marked as 1, meaning the batter was out. If the batter got on base, the outcome was bad and marked as 0. Every outcome was then rated, based on how good or bad the specific outcome was. These ratings ranged from 1, being the worst, and 8 being the best. In addition, the ratings within good and bad outcomes were rated based on how good or bad they were, as explained further below.

3.3.1 Definition of good outcomes

The rating of good outcomes was based on several aspects. Firstly, double plays were ranked as the best outcome as the plate appearance resulted in two outs. Triple plays were not included in this study, as they did not occur in the games analyzed. They are, however, better than double plays as the plate appearance results in three outs. Strike outs were rated slightly worse than double plays, but better than both fly outs and ground outs. This is simply because a strike out holds any possible baserunners to their base, meaning they cannot advance or score based on the outcome of the plate appearance. A strike out, however, only records one out so by definition a double play is better.

Outcome Type of play Rating

1 & 0 1 - 8 1-4

Good

Double play 1 8 4

Strike out 1 7 3

Ground out 1 6 2

Fly out 1 5 1

Bad

Single 0 4 4

Walk 0 4 4

Hit-By-Pitch 0 4 4

Double 0 3 3

Triple 0 2 2

Home run 0 1 1

Table 1– Result of frequency tests between all outcome variables.

(23)

Ground outs were rated above fly outs, since a groundout is most likely hit to an infielder, meaning a runner on second or third has a hard time advancing or scoring. Fly outs, on the other hand, are more likely to be hit to the outfielders.

If a runner on base tags up before the ball is caught, he could advance or score, meaning fly outs are slightly worse than ground outs. Even if a runner on base scored a run due to the batter getting out, the plate appearance would still count as good, since the plate appearance itself resulted in an out.

3.3.2 Definition of bad outcomes

Bad outcomes were also rated on several aspects. As Anstrom and Eisenstein (2011) presents:

It seems intuitive that a triple is better than a double, which is better than a single — observations that the batting average ignores. Other baseball statistics, such as the slugging percentage, assign differing values to different types of hits, with a double having twice, a triple having 3 times, and a home run having 4 times the value of a single (Anstrom &

Eisenstein, 2011).

The value of bad outcomes is, as the authors present, based on slugging percentage, with home runs being valued the highest, and triples, doubles, singles, walks, and hit by pitches all following suit.

It could be argued that walks and hit by pitches are worse than singles, as singles are more likely to score runs than walks and hit by pitches. However, unlike walks or hit by pitches, any base hit can result in outs as the ball is put into play. A base runner on any base, or the batter himself, can be thrown out when the ball is put into play via a base hit. When the batter is walked or hit by a pitch, there is no chance for the defensive team to record an out. There is also the argument presented by Themanson (2019) who explains how singles are harder to attribute to the batter’s skill than a walk. Singles can sneak, fall, squeeze or find a hole in the defense due to many aspects, whilst walks are

(24)

more easily attributable to the batter’s skill. Meanwhile, doubles, triples, and home runs all require the batter to hit the ball hard, far, or be extremely fast.

Since a walk or hit by pitch also has the possibility of advancing or scoring base runners, and the batter reaches the same base as he does with a single, all three were valued the same in the outcome ratings.

3.4 Analysis of data

Descriptive statistics, mean and standard deviation [SD], were calculated for the pitcher’s outcome variable. For all variables, frequencies and percentages were calculated in the analysis of data. The Shapiro Wilk was applied to assess the distribution of outcome variables. The test revealed that the outcome variables were not normally distributed, therefore the Mann-Whitney U test, a rank-based non-parametric test for independent samples, was used to illustrate any possible difference between the pitcher’s outcome when the shadow was and was not present. Statistical analyses were performed using SPSS, 25.0 (IBM SPSS Statistics, New York, USA) for Windows, and the alpha level was set at p <0.05.

3.5 Research ethics considerations

The research ethics considerations as presented by Vetenskapsrådet (2017) have been taken into consideration for this thesis. As this study did not use any interviewees, observation, or in any other way interacted with individuals whose rights need to be acknowledged, no further action has been taken regarding the ethical considerations. Since all data and videos used for the analysis of this thesis are available to the public, free of charge, there was no necessity to consider any other ethical consequences.

(25)

4 Results

The result of the study is based on the outcome of the different tests performed in SPSS. These are illustrated and explained below.

The analysis of frequencies of all outcomes revealed that there was no major difference in outcomes with or without the presence of shadows, as seen in table 2. Some outcomes increase or decrease slightly based on the presence or absence of shadows, which will be further discussed in the next chapter.

Overall, when high rated outcomes decrease, low rated outcomes increase, thus balancing the rating of all good or bad outcomes, respectively.

Outcome

NO Shadow

YES Shadow

Frequency Percent (%) Frequency Percent (%)

BAD 53 28.5 30 26.3

GOOD 133 71.5 84 73.7

Total 186 100.0 114 100.0

GOOD Outcome

NO Shadow

YES Shadow

Frequency Percent (%) Frequency Percent (%)

Fly out 32 24.1 26 31.0

Ground out 36 27.1 19 22.6

Strike out 60 45.1 35 41.7

Double play 5 3.8 4 4.8

Total 133 100.0 84 100.0

BAD Outcome

NO Shadow

YES Shadow

Frequency Percent (%) Frequency Percent (%) Hit-By-Pitch / Single / Walk 42 79.2 24 82.8

Double 4 7.5 3 10.3

Triple 2 3.8 - -

Home run 5 9.4 2 6.9

Total 53 100.0 29 100.0

The Mann-Whitney U-test was then used to determine if there were significant differences in the pitcher’s outcome when the shadow was and was not present, as seen in table 3. Outcome ratings when shadow was present (mean rank = 149.4) and when it was not present (mean rank = 151.2) were not statistically significantly different, U = 10478, z = -.175, p = 0.861. The same is the case

Table 2 – Result of frequency tests between all outcome variables.

(26)

for if the outcome was either good or bad (U = 10371, z = -.409, p = 0.683) when the shadows were and were not present. There were no differences neither in the pitchers good (U = 5318, z = -.634, p = 0.526), nor bad (U = 737, z = -.442, p =0.658) outcome when the shadow was and was not present.

SHADOW

Presence N Mean Rank Sum of Ranks Mann-

Whitney U Z Asymp. Sig.

(2-tailed) Good and Bad

Outcome (1-8)

Shadow NO 186 151.17 28117.00

10478.00 -.175 .861 Shadow YES 114 149.41 17033.00

Good and Bad Outcome (1 and 0)

Shadow NO 186 149.26 27762.00

10371.00 -.409 .683 Shadow YES 114 152.53 17388.00

GOOD

Outcomes (1-4)

Shadow NO 133 111.02 14765.00

5318.00 -.634 .526

Shadow YES 84 105.81 8888.00

BAD

Outcomes (1-4)

Shadow NO 53 42.09 2231.00

737.00 -.442 .658

Shadow YES 29 40.41 1172.00

Table 3 – Result of Mann-Whitney U test in four pairs of non-parametric, independent variables.

(27)

5 Discussion

In this section, the results, as well as methodological choices, are discussed.

5.1 Discussion of results

The main finding of this study is that there was no significant difference in the pitcher’s outcome when the shadow was and was not in play during the plate appearance. As opposed to the general assumption of how shadows affect the outcome of plate appearances (Major League Baseball, 2021; Redbirdrants, 2012), the results indicate that shadows do not affect the outcome of plate appearances. Based on the conducted study, it appears that shadows do not create an advantage for pitchers, neither do they pose a disadvantage to hitters.

This was seen in multiple aspects.

Firstly, the shadows do not have a significant effect on whether a plate appearance ends in a good or a bad outcome for the pitcher. This is the main opposing argument to the existing consensus in baseball, which assumes that the shadows are beneficial for the pitcher. As recent as April 26, 2021, a pitcher for the Arizona Diamondbacks by the name of Maddison Bumgarner threw a no-hitter, meaning he did not allow a single base-hit through the entire game. In his post-game interview, he specifically says:

“…I want to thank the shadows in Atlanta. They helped me out a good bit, that was pretty awesome…” (Bally Sports Arizona, 2021)

The quote above is an example of Major League Baseball’s attitude toward shadows. The pitcher’s opinions are part of a wider argument which not only broadcasters (Major League Baseball, 2021), but even players themselves seem to share, on the topic of shadows (Redbirdrants, 2009).

Secondly, the result of this study illustrates that the ratings of outcomes, whether good or bad, are not affected by shadows. For example, as seen in table 2 of the result chapter, the rate of strike outs decreases with the presence of shadows, further strengthening Schifman’s (2019) argument that batters do

(28)

not struggle to see the ball more in the shadows than out of it. In the analysis of ratings within good and bad outcomes, the results showed that there was no statistical significance in how good or bad the outcomes were, with or without the presence of shadows. As seen in table 2, even if the amount of fly outs increases with the presence of shadows, ground outs and strike outs decrease.

The same is seen for bad outcomes, in which singles, walks, hit-by-pitches, and doubles increases with the presence of shadows, while triples and home runs are less frequent.

Interestingly, the increase of fly outs would assumingly cause an increase of home runs, as they are correlated according to statistics such as HR/FB%, which calculates the number of home runs hit per every fly ball (Slowinski, 2010b). This, however, does not seem to be the case for games in this study, as the rate of home runs goes down, while the amount of fly balls goes up with the presence of shadows. According to the statistical metric of batted ball velocity, which correlates the rate of hard-hit fly balls with the number of home runs (Zola, 2020), we can assume that the decrease of home runs despite the increase of fly balls are due to the batters hitting the ball at a slower velocity in the presence of shadows. This could also explain the increase of doubles in the presence of shadows, since a lower exit velocity of the batted ball results in balls not traveling as far, thus resulting in either a fly out or a double.

If the ball is being hit at a lower velocity in the presence of shadows, it would be indicative of the pitchers having an advantage in the shadows, despite what the result of the study proves. As presented by Siegel (2015) the metric of exit velocity has been popular in recent years to explain underlying numbers to traditional statistics. A higher exit velocity increases the probability of getting a base hit, as well as the probability of hitting a home run. If the batters are hitting the ball at a lower velocity in the shadows, they would statistically have a lower chance of getting a base hit. As the result shows, however, neither

(29)

good nor bad outcomes change significantly with the presence or absence of shadows.

A similar statistical indifference is found in the frequencies of ground outs versus double plays, as seen in table 2. While double plays are not necessarily started on a ground ball hit to a fielder, a decrease of ground outs should suggest that double plays increase as well, which is not the case for this study.

Double plays, however, are the result of a previous batter reaching base before the current plate appearance, meaning that the decrease in double plays could simply be a coincidence and not correlated to the change in the rate of ground outs.

While the results prove that shadows do not influence the outcome of plate appearances, several other factors are excluded in this study. As previously stated, exit velocity is a statistical metric that correlates to the rate of base hits and home runs. Even if the rate of such outcomes does not increase with the presence of shadows, exit velocity could still be indicative to explain whether the pitcher is at an advantage in the shadows.

Another possible factor that could affect the result is the fatigue of the pitcher at different points in the game. For instance, the pitcher is more likely to be less fatigued at the start of the game than he is at the end of his appearance (Whiteside et al., 2016; Bradbury & Forman, 2012). Birfer et al. (2019) illustrate how fatigue levels have linear codependency with decreased performance, as pitch counts, and innings thrown increase.

Due to this, if shadows are present at the start of the game, they may not have the same effect on the outcome of plate appearances as they would if they were present at the end. This is simply because different pitchers in this study are most likely at different levels of fatigue when the shadows are present. Since the games selected for this study had to have shadows both present and absent

(30)

but could appear at different points of the game, the level of fatigue for pitchers could be a confounding variable for the outcome of plate appearances.

Furthermore, the defensive aspect of baseball is not taken into consideration when analyzing the significance of outcome variables. There is a multitude of defensive metrics in baseball statistics, each explaining their specific aspect of the game. These include, but are not limited to, fielding average, range factor, and zone rating (Baseball Almanac, N.D.). Each of these metrics measures different facets of the defensive fielder such as how good he is at catching the ball, how far he can throw and how good his positioning is in comparison to the balls hit towards him. What they all have in common, however, is they measure the value defensive plays have on the outcome of the game.

As previously discussed, outcomes of plate appearances are impossible to credit to only the pitcher or batter. A single can find a hole and drop for a base hit, while a double play just as easily could be a single if the fielder’s positioning or fielding ability was any different. Therefore, stating that the pitcher or batter is at an advantage in the shadows should be taken with caution, as the study conducted did not consider defensive factors. If statistical metrics such as Batting Average on Balls In Play or Net Expected Run Value, which were discussed in previous research, were also applied to the outcome of each outcome in this study, other results than the ones presented may have been evident.

Whilst the main finding of this study is that there was no significant difference in the outcome of plate appearances when the shadow was and was not present, it should be taken with caution. Further research on the topic of shadows is necessary to reach a statistical conclusion.

5.2 Discussion of method

Quantitative method is defined as the collection of numerical data based on an objective view of reality (Bryman, 2011). Since the empirical data collected

(31)

for this thesis is numerical and of a statistical nature, it seemed only logical to apply quantitative method to this study.

The purpose of this thesis was to determine value from and reach a conclusion based on pre-existing statistics, meaning that content analysis of statistics proved to be the optimal collection method (Gratton & Jones, 2011). For content analysis, the data sets, codes, and categories used in this thesis are identified according to previous research and theories on baseball statistics.

As with any research method, however, content analysis is disadvantageous in its own way. An inappropriate sampling of the data or failure to acknowledge confounding variables that may influence the outcome of the analysis may cause the result of the study to be illegitimate.

5.2.1 Discussion of variables

The most critical aspect of the method is the exclusion of other variables apart from the strict outcome of plate appearances. If more statistical aspects, such as exit velocity, defensive factors, or batting average on balls in play, were included in this study, different results may or may not have been revealed.

The variables included in this study were chosen based on their direct effect and correlation to good and bad outcomes. For the pitcher, a hit, walk or hit by pitch is bad, while an out is good. To include all possible variables that may or may not influence the outcome of plate appearances, however, is not as simple. While increased exit velocity, for example, increases the probability for the batter to get on base (Siegel, 2015), it does not always result in a good outcome for the batter. Therefore, such variables were excluded from this study, whilst remaining highly relevant for further research within this topic.

While the ratings of all outcomes analyzed in this study are based on the nature of baseball as well as previous research, not all outcomes are equal. For one, the difference between a good or a bad outcome for the pitcher is much bigger than the difference in ratings between the lowest-rated good outcome, fly outs

(32)

(5), versus the highest-rated bad outcome, singles, walks, or hit-by-pitches (4).

If a pitcher were to get 27 good outcomes in a row, he would have pitched a perfect game and led his team to the win. Such a feat has only been achieved 23 times in over 200’000 games played since the start of the modern era of 1901 (Baseball Reference, N.D.b). If a pitcher were to get 27 bad outcomes in a row, he would have given up at minimum 24 runs and the game would still be in the first inning without any outs recorded. Thus, the difference between a 4 and a 5 in the rating of all possible outcomes does not fully illustrate the significance of a plate appearance ending in a good or bad outcome. This, however, is also considered in the analysis of the data, since the frequency of good and bad outcomes also showed no statistical significance.

5.2.2 Discussion of sources

In the study conducted, popular culture references have been avoided as far as possible, especially when developing a foundation for the objective and research questions through previous research. While baseball statistics may be a thoroughly researched topic, most sources that explain specific statistical aspects are not scientifically reviewed. In this study, scientifically reviewed articles and books were used in combination with baseball statistics to argue or present established perspectives within the sport.

5.2.3 Delimitations and limitations

A delimitation for this study has been to analyze the outcome of plate appearances using only the independent variables of shadows. Thus, other factors such as wind, fatigue, opponent’s win percentage, or fielding quality have not been considered as variables that may affect the outcome of plate appearances. These factors could be relevant to analyze in a similar context of what affects the outcomes of plate appearances, but since this has not been the focus, as the aim of the thesis is to strictly analyze whether shadows affect the outcome of plate appearances, such factors were excluded from the research.

(33)

A limitation for this study was the availability of visual data only stretching as far back as 2017. MLB Film Room proved to be a valuable source for this study, as the data gathered from the analyzed games could be verified through MLB Film Room to determine whether the shadows are present. However, as MLB Film Room is the only source for these videos, and it began collecting the data in 2017, no game prior to 2017 was included in this study.

(34)

6 Conclusions

In this section, the conclusions based on the result and discussion are presented, as well as implications for further research.

The main finding of this study is that there was no significant difference in the outcome of plate appearances when the shadow was and was not present. This finding is not in line with current opinions of Major League Baseball and its players, coaches, or broadcasters (Bally Sports Arizona, 2021; Branch, 2009;

Major League Baseball, 2021; New York Times, Redbirdrant, 2009).

Many factors, on top of the variables taken into consideration for this study, have a clear effect on the outcome of plate appearances. This seems to be the case whether the shadows are present or not. Based on the study conducted, it is impossible to conclude that simply adding or subtracting the shadows from the equation of plate appearances, creates an advantage for either side of the field. Analysis of which factors may affect the outcome of plate appearances, and considering such factors, is required to create a more distinct result of how large the effect of shadows is.

The answer to the first research questions for this study is that too many other factors need to be taken into consideration to prove whether shadows does affect the outcome of plate appearances. With the variables selected for this study, however, no statistical significance was found with or without the presence of shadows.

To answer the second research question, further research needs to be made to reach a conclusion on the topic. If managers were to augment their strategy based on this study alone, it would be recommended that they explain to their batters, specifically, that the shadows do not affect their ability at the plate.

For pitchers, it might be a different problem. Even if further research were to prove that shadows do not affect the outcome of plate appearances, it is unclear if managers should continue to let their pitchers believe they are at advantage

(35)

in the shadows. Whether they perform better or not, thinking they are at an advantage, is uncertain.

6.1 Implications for further research

As far as the results of this study show, it seems to point in the opposite direction to the general assumption of Major League players and broadcasters.

If the shadows do not affect the outcome of plate appearances, it would disprove any argument which states that such shadows impact the game overall. Regardless of the shadows affecting the outcome of the game, the widespread claim that the weather phenomena act in the pitcher’s favor could be a placebo effect. Any bad outcome, for the batter, could be explained away by the presence of shadows, and a good outcome would be credited to the batter’s skill, rather than the pitcher making a mistake in such a situation. If such a placebo effect exists in Major League Baseball, regarding the effect of shadows, is unclear at this moment. It would, however, be an interesting topic for further research.

Exit velocity is a variable not accounted for in this study, as the data for every batted ball is not available publicly. However, it could be a deciding factor in whether the pitcher has the advantage in the shadows or not. Incorporating variables such as exit velocity, among others, could be of interest for research within the topic of shadows in the future.

It could be interesting for further research to interview Major League pitchers to see if they prefer pitching when the shadows are present. Such a study could comprehensively show whether a consensus exists within Major League Baseball. Based on such research, it could be further applied in combination with studies that prove or disprove the shadows effect on the outcome of plate appearances, to reveal whether a placebo effect exists surrounding the topic in question.

(36)

Further research on this specific topic should include other statistical metrics into the calculation, such as exit velocity and defensive factors. Currently, in addition to Schifman’s (2019) article on the phenomenon, this study can serve as a basis for further research on this topic. To determine what underlying aspects affect the outcome of plate appearances is not a small task, as there are plenty of factors other than statistics to take into consideration. I suggest that further research assume that shadows do not play a part in the outcome of plate appearance, but question that hypothesis, whilst adding more factors into the equation.

(37)

7 References

Alamar, B., Ma, J., Desjardins, G., Ruprecht, L. (2006) Who Controls the Plate? Isolating the Pitcher/Batter Subgame. Journal of Quantitative Analysis in Sports, 2 (3). https://doi-org.proxy.lnu.se/10.2202/1559-0410.1012

Albert, J., Bennett, J., Cochran J. J. (Ed.) (2005). Anthology of Statistics in Sports. American Statistical Association and the Society for Industrial and Applied Mathematics.

Anstrom J. K., Eisenstein L. E. (2011). From batting average to wins above replacement to composite end points – refining clinical research using baseball statistical methods. American Heart Journal, 161(5), 805-806.

https://doi-org.proxy.lnu.se/10.1016/j.ahj.2011.01.023

Arth W. Z., Billings C. A. (2020). Batting Average and Beyond: The Framing of Statistics Within Regional Major League Baseball Broadcasts.

International Journal of Sport Communication, Ahead of print. https://doi- org.proxy.lnu.se/10.1123/ijsc.2020-0112

Bahill, A., Baldwin, D., Venkateswaran, J. (2005). Predicting a Baseball's Path: A batter watches the pitcher's motion plus the spin on the ball to calculate when and where it will cross the plate. American Scientist, 93(3), 218-225. Retrieved May 10, 2021, from

http://www.jstor.org/stable/27858576

Bally Sports Arizona [BallySportSAZ]. (April 26, 2021). The perfect answer for the perfect day [Twitterpost].

https://twitter.com/BallySportSAZ/status/1386448705092472833

Baseball Almanac. (N.D.). Defensive Stats 101. Baseball Almanac.

https://www.baseball-almanac.com/stats3.shtml

Baseball Reference. (N.D.a). Baseball stats and history. Baseball Reference.

https://www.baseball-reference.com/

(38)

Baseball Reference. (N.D.b). No-Hitters and Perfect Games. Baseball Reference. https://www.baseball-reference.com/friv/no-hitters-and-perfect- games.shtml

Bradbury C. J., Forman L. S. (2012). The Impact of Pitch Counts and Days Rest on Performance Among Major-League Baseball Pitchers. Journal of Strength and Conditioning Research, 26(5), 1181-1187. DOI:

10.1519/JSC.0b013e31824e16fe

Branch, J. (October 9, 2009). Afternoon Starts Put Shadows in Plat. The New York Times.

https://www.nytimes.com/2009/10/07/sports/baseball/07shadows.html

Bryman, A. (2011). Samhällsvetenskapliga metoder (2nd ed.) Stockholm:

Liber.

Butterworth L. M. (2010). Baseball and Rhetorics of Purity.

https://books.google.se/books?hl=sv&lr=&id=Xv73owGa3IMC&oi=fnd&pg

=PR7&ots=N4LV3IfpDE&sig=kO2hE56oZrs3Os6nBgSHqANboY0&redir_

esc=y#v=onepage&q&f=false

Caporale, T., Collier C. T. (2012). Scouts versus Stats: the impact of

Moneyball on the Major League Baseball draft. Applied Economics, 45(15), 1983-1990. https://doi-org.proxy.lnu.se/10.1080/00036846.2011.641933 Davis, D. (N.D.). Top baseball players of past 69 years. Lehigh University.

https://www.lehigh.edu/~dmd1/baseball.html

Fagan, F., Haugh, M., Cooper, H. (2019). The advantage of lefties in one-on- one sports. Journal of Quantitative Analysis in Sports, 15(1), 1-25.

https://doi-org.proxy.lnu.se/10.1515/jqas-2017-0076

Goodman H. G., Mcandrew T. F. (1993). Domes and Astroturf. Environment and Behavior, 25(1), 121-125. DOI: 10.1177/0013916593251006

(39)

Gratton, C., Jones, I. (2009). Research methods for sports studies (2nd ed.).

https://ebookcentral-proquest-com.proxy.lnu.se

Grosshandler, S. (N.D.). Unsung Heroes: No-Hit Catchers. SABR. Retrieved May 10, 2021, from http://research.sabr.org/journals/unsung-heroes-no-hit- catchers

Hoffmann, F., Rielly J. E., Manning J. M. (2003). Baseball and American Culture. https://ebookcentral-proquest-com.proxy.lnu.se/lib/linne-

ebooks/reader.action?docID=1588497&ppg=16

Huber R. M.., Sturdivant. X. R. (2010). Building a Model for Scoring 20 or More Runs in a Baseball Game. The Annals of Applied Statistics, 4(2), 791- 804. Retrieved May 10, 2021, from https://www.jstor.org/stable/29765530 IMDB. (May 10, 2021). Moneyball. https://www.imdb.com/title/tt1210166/

Jensen T. S., Shirley E., K, Wyner J. A. (2009). Bayesball: A Bayesian Hierarchical Model for Evaluating Fielding in Major League Baseball. The Annals of Applied Statistics, 3(2), 491-520. Retrieved May 10, 2021, from http://www.jstor.org/stable/30244253

Jones B. M. (2015). The Home Advantage in Major League Baseball.

Perceptual & Motor Skills, 121(3), 791-804. DOI:

10.2466/26.PMS.121c25x1

Kagan, D. (July 30, 2014). Lost in the Sun: The Physics of Ballpark Orientation. The Hardball Times. https://tht.fangraphs.com/lost-in-the-sun- the-physics-of-ballpark-orientation/

Kelly W. W. (2007). Is baseball a global sport? America’s ‘national pastime’

as global field and international sport. Global Networks, 7(2), 187-201.

Retrieved May 10, 2021, from https://onlinelibrary-wiley- com.proxy.lnu.se/doi/epdf/10.1111/j.1471-0374.2007.00164.x

(40)

Major League Baseball. (2019). Official Baseball Rules. Major League Baseball. https://img.mlbstatic.com/mlb-

images/image/upload/mlb/ub08blsefk8wkkd2oemz.pdf

Major League Baseball. (April 4, 2021). Toronto Blue Jays vs New York Yankees [Video]. Major League Baseball.

https://www.mlb.com/tv/g634607/v12f108bf-db74-4d2d-97c8-

aead398791f9?clickOrigin=Gameday&media_type=video#game=634607,tfs

=20210404_170500,game_state=live

Rader G. B. (2008). Baseball: A History of America’s Game (3rd ed.). Illinois History of Sports.

Redbirdrants. (2009). Shadows Becoming Serious Problem at Busch Stadium. Redbirdrants. Retrieved May 10, 2021, from

https://redbirdrants.com/2011/09/08/shadows-becoming-serious-problem-at- busch-stadium/

Schifman, G. (2019). Expanding the Zone: Do Shadows Affect Postseason Play? Baseball Prospectus. Retrieved May 10, 2021, from

https://www.baseballprospectus.com/news/article/54487/expanding-the- zone-do-shadows-affect-postseason-play/

Schroeder V. D. (2011). The Sun and the Seasons. Understanding Astronomy. Retrieved May 10, 2021, from

https://physics.weber.edu/schroeder/ua/sunandseasons.html

Siegel, J. (February 9, 2021). The Importance of 90th Percentile Exit Velocity. Pitcherlist. https://www.pitcherlist.com/the-importance-of-90th- percentile-exit-velocity/

Slowinski, P. (February 16, 2010a). BABIP. Fangraphs.

https://library.fangraphs.com/pitching/babip/

(41)

Slowinski, P. (February 19, 2010b). HR/FB. Fangraphs.

https://library.fangraphs.com/pitching/hrs/

Swartz, M. (2009). Ahead in the Count: Home-Field Advantages, Part One.

Baseball Prospectus. Retrieved May 10, 2021, from

https://www.baseballprospectus.com/news/article/9372/ahead-in-the-count- home-field-advantages-part-one/

Themanson, J. (June 19, 2019). The Psychology of Walks and Singles. The Hardball Times. https://tht.fangraphs.com/the-psychology-of-walks-and- singles/

Vetenskapsrådet. (2017). God forskningssed. Vetenskapsrådet.

https://www.vr.se/download/18.2412c5311624176023d25b05/15553321120 63/God-forskningssed_VR_2017.pdf

Whiteside, D., Martini, D. N., Zernicke, R. F., Goulet, G. C. (2016). Changes in a Starting Pitcher's Performance Characteristics Across the Duration of a Major League Baseball Game. International journal of sports physiology and performance, 11(2), 247–254. Retrieved May 18, 2021, from

https://doi.org/10.1123/ijspp.2015-0121

Wikipedia. (April 29, 2021a). List of current Major League Baseball stadiums. Wikipedia.

https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/List_of_current_Major_League_Baseball_stadi ums

Wikipedia. (January 20, 2021b). Baseball statistics. Wikipedia.

https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Baseball_statistics

Winter W. C., Hammond R. W., Green H. N., Zhang, Z., Bliwise L. D.

(2009). Measuring Circadian Advantage in Major League Baseball: A 10- Year Retrospective Study. International Journal of Sports Physiology and Performance, 4(3), 394-401. Retrieved May 10, 2021, from

(42)

http://journals.humankinetics.com.proxy.lnu.se/view/journals/ijspp/4/3/articl e-p394.xml

Zola, T. (January 30, 2020). The Z Files: The Relationship Between HR and Flyball Average Exit Velocity. Rotowire.

https://www.rotowire.com/baseball/article.php?id=49468

References

Related documents

46 Konkreta exempel skulle kunna vara främjandeinsatser för affärsänglar/affärsängelnätverk, skapa arenor där aktörer från utbuds- och efterfrågesidan kan mötas eller

Generally, a transition from primary raw materials to recycled materials, along with a change to renewable energy, are the most important actions to reduce greenhouse gas emissions

Both Brazil and Sweden have made bilateral cooperation in areas of technology and innovation a top priority. It has been formalized in a series of agreements and made explicit

För att uppskatta den totala effekten av reformerna måste dock hänsyn tas till såväl samt- liga priseffekter som sammansättningseffekter, till följd av ökad försäljningsandel

The increasing availability of data and attention to services has increased the understanding of the contribution of services to innovation and productivity in

Generella styrmedel kan ha varit mindre verksamma än man har trott De generella styrmedlen, till skillnad från de specifika styrmedlen, har kommit att användas i större

Parallellmarknader innebär dock inte en drivkraft för en grön omställning Ökad andel direktförsäljning räddar många lokala producenter och kan tyckas utgöra en drivkraft

Närmare 90 procent av de statliga medlen (intäkter och utgifter) för näringslivets klimatomställning går till generella styrmedel, det vill säga styrmedel som påverkar