• No results found

Using SMS Text Message Reminders as a Way to Decrease Short Notice Cancellations of Surgeries

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "Using SMS Text Message Reminders as a Way to Decrease Short Notice Cancellations of Surgeries"

Copied!
14
0
0

Loading.... (view fulltext now)

Full text

(1)

IN

DEGREE PROJECT COMPUTER SCIENCE AND ENGINEERING, SECOND CYCLE, 30 CREDITS

STOCKHOLM SWEDEN 2016,

Using SMS Text Message

Reminders as a Way to Decrease Short Notice Cancellations of

Surgeries

A Semi-randomized Controlled Trial DAN CARIÑO

KTH ROYAL INSTITUTE OF TECHNOLOGY

SCHOOL OF COMPUTER SCIENCE AND COMMUNICATION

(2)

Using SMS Text Message Reminders as a Way to  Decrease Short Notice Cancellations of Surgeries: 

A Semi­randomized Controlled Trial 

 

Användning av SMS­påminnelser som ett sätt att minska  antalet operationer som avbokas med kort varsel: 

En semirandomiserad kontrollerad studie 

                     

DAN CARIÑO 

dcarino@kth.se   

                             

 

Master of Science in Engineering in Media Technology  Master's programme in Human­Computer Interaction 

Supervisor:​ Helena Tobiasson  Examinator:​ Jan Gulliksen 

Principal:​ Centre for Technology in Medicine and Health   

Published: 2016­08­25 

(3)

Sammanfattning 

Sjukvårdsgivare möts av stora kostnader då operationssalar inte används på grund av att  patienter (1) inte avbokar i tid för att tillåta att en annan patient tar deras platser eller (2) inte är  i tillräckligt bra tillstånd för att bli opererad på grund av ett misslyckande att följa de 

preoperativa instruktionerna ordentligt. Detta examensarbete har som mål att undersöka om  SMS­påminnelser kan användas för att minska antalet operationer som avbokas på detta vis. 

Patienter från de kirurgiska­, urologiska och ortopediska klinikerna på ett sjukhus i Stockholm  placerades i en av tre grupper; (1) en grupp som inte mottog SMS­påminnelser, (2) en grupp  som mottog en ”kort påminnelse”, (3) och en grupp som mottog en ”lång” påminnelse. En  enkätstudie utfördes för att kunna avgöra om patienter ansåg att påminnelserna påverkade  dem eller inte. Studien antyder att påminnelser inte fungerar som ett sätt att minska antalet  avbokningar med kort varsel eftersom det avbokningar eventuellt inte beror på minnet. Istället  är det möjligt att ångest har en större påverkan än förväntat. Fler studier borde utföras för att  kunna bekräfta denna hypotes. 

   

Abstract 

Health care providers face large costs in terms of operating rooms not being utilized due to  patients (1) not canceling enough in advance to allow a different patient to be scheduled or (2)  not being in a good enough condition to be operated on due to failure to follow preoperative  instructions properly. This degree project aims to see if SMS text reminders can be used to  lower the number of surgeries canceled in this manner. Patients of the surgical­, urological­ 

and orthodontic clinics at a hospital in Stockholm were placed into one of three groups; (1) a  group that did not receive a text message reminder, (2) a group that received a "short" 

reminder, (3) and a group that received a "long" reminder. A survey was also conducted to  determine what the patients thought of the reminders and whether they believed it influenced  them or not. The study suggests that reminders do not work as a method to decrease the  short notice cancellation rate as it may not be a poor memory that causes the patient to  cancel. Instead, anxiety might have a greater impact than expected. Further studies should be  conducted in order to confirm this hypothesis. 

   

(4)

Using SMS Text Message Reminders as a Way to Decrease Short Notice Cancellations of Surgeries:

a Semi-randomized Controlled Trial

Dan Cariño

KTH Royal Institute of Technology Stockholm, Sweden

dcarino@kth.se

ABSTRACT

Health care providers face large costs in terms of oper- ating rooms not being utilized due to patients (1) not canceling enough in advance to allow a different patient to be scheduled or (2) not being in a good enough condition to be operated on due to failure to follow preoperative instructions properly. This degree project aims to see if SMS text reminders can be used to lower the number of surgeries canceled in this manner. Patients of the surgical-, urological- and orthodontic clinics at a hospital in Stockholm were placed into one of three groups; (1) a group that did not receive a text message reminder, (2) a group that received a "short" reminder, (3) and a

group that received a "long" reminder. A survey was also conducted to determine what the patients thought of the reminders and whether they believed it influenced them or not.

The study suggests that reminders do not work as a method to decrease the short notice cancellation rate as it may not be a poor memory that causes the patient to cancel. Instead, anxiety might have a greater impact than expected. Further studies should be conducted in order to confirm this hypothesis.

Author Keywords

Cancellation, Health care, Reminder, Short notice, SMS, Surgery, Text message

INTRODUCTION

Whenever a patient cancels on short notice it hinders the hospital personnel’s ability to find another patient as a replacement. This leads to wasted resources in the form of unused operating rooms, equipment and personnel [1]

which lengthens the wait for other patients, and in turn results in a considerable cost to the health care system [2].

For example, it has been estimated that a surgery room costs 170 SEK (approximately 21 USD) per minute in Sweden, which in the Swedish region Skåne is estimated to cause a 15.7 million SEK (approximately 1.90 million USD) loss every year through canceled surgeries [3]. It is, therefore, alarming that such huge costs exist due to patients failing to cancel their surgeries in time.

In 2014, it was recorded that 12.4% of all planned surgery in Sweden were canceled, for various reasons (not only due to the patients’ actions). Other reasons include;

other patients being prioritized, the patient’s needs hav- ing changed since assessment, and personnel missing [4].

However, over 20% of the canceled surgeries were short notice cancellations (SNC), which is defined as cancella- tions where the patient cancels or reschedules the surgery later than 5 pm the day before the day of operation [4].

Although there has not been extensive research on the reason why patients cancel their surgeries later than desired nor on how to prevent it from occurring, there has been previous research on patients not attending their doctor’s appointments. Studies have shown that the main reason patients do not attend (DNA) is due to them being unable to remember it [5]. Some studies found that the percentage of canceled surgical procedures that stem from this reason lies at about 30-50% [2][5].

A large percentage of canceled appointments, therefore, seem to be preventable by reminding the patient of it.

To combat this issue, several hospitals have introduced the option of receiving text message reminders regarding their appointment, which has shown to be successful [6].

However, such text message reminders do not currently exist for surgeries. Another issue faced by the health care system is patients not executing preoperative (before a surgical operation) instructions properly. If the patient has not followed these instructions, it is up to the doctor performing the surgery to determine whether the risks are sufficiently low to allow for surgery or not. For these reasons SMS text message reminders should be introduced in order to determine if they not only decrease the DNA rate of doctor’s appointments but if they can solve the aforementioned issues as well.

(5)

THEORY

This section introduces concepts regarding statistical test theory which are important to understand in order to interpret the results.

Statistical test theory Type I and type II errors

When performing hypothesis testing it is important to consider type I and type II errors, also known as “false positives” and “false negatives”, respectively. To be exact, a type I error is the incorrect rejection of the “null hy- pothesis”, the hypothesis that is attempted to be rejected.

The opposite of this is the “alternative hypothesis”, the hypothesis that is attempted to be proven. A type II error is the false acceptance of the null hypothesis. In- creasing the sample size makes it less likely that these errors will occur by making the sample more similar to the population [7].

Significance test

A significance test determines whether there is a corre- lation between the variables in the test, or if the results were caused by chance. This, in turn, makes it possible to correctly reject the null hypothesis. It is also common to set the confidence level of the rejection alternatively as 95% certainty that the null hypothesis is correctly rejected, or as a 5% chance that the null hypothesis is incorrectly rejected. The latter is more commonly re- ferred to as alpha and is written as a decimal instead of a percentage (e.g. 0.05). In order to determine if the results are statistically significant, the significance test determines P, “the probability of obtaining the study results by chance if the null hypothesis is true”. The convention is to reject the null hypothesis when the con- fidence level P is less than 0.05. If P is higher than 0.05, the null hypothesis should not be rejected due to the greater possibility that the results are the outcome of chance [7].

BACKGROUND

This section aims to provide information regarding the health care industry that will provide a greater under- standing of this degree project.

Evolution of health care

The trend in the health care industry today is to shift patients from being inpatients (patients who stay at the hospital after their procedure) to becoming outpatients (patients who leave the hospital after the procedure on the same day), also known as day surgery patients [8].

This is caused by new advances in surgical technology, analgesia, and anesthetics which allow procedures to be done less invasive than before, allowing a quicker recovery [9]. The reasoning behind this change is due to the fact that outpatient surgeries are less expensive and makes operating room usage more efficient without impacting the quality [10][11][12].

Preoperative instructions

Two sets of instructions that are given to all patients before surgery (through the summon) were identified by the personnel as the most critical to ensure a safe procedure: full body disinfection and fasting. Full body disinfection is performed to reduce the risk for infection caused by the patient’s skin bacteria. The bacteria is removed by the use of a liquid soap or shower gel with a disinfecting effect. The patient must shower twice the day prior to the day of operation, and twice on the day of the operation. It is also important that the patient sleeps with a clean set of clothes, on a bed with newly washed bed linen, after showering [13].

Fasting is performed in order to avoid pulmonary aspi- ration; entry of material from the gastrointestinal tract into the larynx and lower respiratory tract, also known as having something “go down the wrong pipe” [14].

Anxiety in health care

10% to 30% of patients who are not scheduled for surgery experience anxiety, which is defined as “fear or nervous- ness about what might happen” [15][16]. For patients who are scheduled for surgery, the percentage increases to between 60% and 80%. The anxiety that stems from surgery usually begins to be experienced when planning for it has started. However, the intensity can vary based on age, gender, previous hospital experience and the pa- tient’s general ability to cope with stress [16]. Surgery causes anxiety primarily due to the patient’s lack of knowledge of what will transpire. Most commonly, it is the inadequate knowledge of the fasting period and surgical waiting period, as well as, isolation from a social environment, fears concerning the inability to awaken after surgery, loss of control, intraoperative complications and postoperative fear of pain that cause anxiety [16][17].

The primary reason patients are not aware of what will take place appears to be that their expectations of the information they receive from the hospital do not match the actually received information [18]. Furthermore, stud- ies show that patients forget between 40% and 80% of medical information immediately after hearing it [19]. It also appears that the lack of understanding causes pa- tients to comply with preoperative instructions as it has been implied that patients who do not know the purpose of the preparation are less likely to commit to complete them [20].

Different methods have been proven to decrease anxi- ety before surgery. Creating a preoperative education program, in which a member of the personnel identifies the patient’s knowledge level, perceptions of surgery, and ability to understand the offered information have been helpful in pinpointing which areas to touch upon, for individual patients, to bring their knowledge of the surgi- cal process and expectations of the surgical area to an adequate level. Music therapy has also been found to help in managing anxiety. Patients who were allowed to listen to music of their choice for 30 minutes before their surgery experienced a decrease in anxiety, while a control

(6)

group that rested in a quiet room for the same period experienced an increase in anxiety [17].

RELATED RESEARCH

This section discusses research on messages in the health care industry that are of relevance of the degree project.

Reminders in health care

Previous solutions that have been found to be successful in health care include letters [21], post-cards [22] and both automated [23] and personalized phone calls [24].

However, these methods require a considerable amount of effort and capital and are, therefore, challenging to implement as an operational solution by the hospital after the completion of this degree project. It is, therefore, desirable to find an inexpensive and easily implementable solution.

A study that analyzed missed appointments at a sexual health and HIV clinic before and after the implementa- tion of SMS text reminders of appointments found that they were effective in decreasing the number of missed appointments. They recorded an overall decrease in DNA rates by 4 percentage units (28% to 24%) and an increase in cancellation by 4 percentage units (62% to 66%) over- all [25]. Other studies have proven to be more successful, such as at follow-ups of primary care, where the atten- dance increased by 11 percentage units (48% to 59%) [26].

Another study examined the differences between the us- age of SMS messages and telephone calls, as reminders for appointments, and found that they produced similar results in improving the attendance rate, the main dif- ference being that SMS messages are more cost effective [6].

The importance of the message content

One study carried out in the UK examined how different statements could affect the attendance rate [27]. The study discovered that if the text message explicitly stated how much a missed appointment costs for the health care system, it performed better than the standard reminder, having a DNA that was 2.7 percentage points lower (8.4%

compared to 11.1%), which would lead to 5,800 fewer can- celed appointments yearly. Other messages tested were

"easy call", where the telephone number for canceling was included in the message, "social norms", which stated that a large majority attend the appointments, "general costs", where only the concept of costs (due to nonattendance of the appointment) was presented, "recording", in which the patient was informed that the hospital keeps record of the missed appointments, and "empathy", in which the patient is made to consider that being absent from an appointment would be burdensome for others [27].

Tailored interventions

Tailored interventions are performed by “developing a specific, behavior-focused communication based on an assessment of individual characteristics” [28]. Tailoring

can take the form of mentioning personal information such as the patient’s name, children or workplace, or by changing the information based on their demographics, for example, gender, ethnicity and age [29]. They have also been found to work in health care, making it easier for patients to read the information provided with interest, though it is not as well established as in other practices, such as marketing. The effects of tailored interventions have been found to be more effective among lower income and minorities. It is suggested that the aforementioned groups benefit more due to a general lack of access to information compared to others [29].

Determining the research scope

Currently, there are several articles on the attendance rate of appointments, however, there has not been any ex- tensive research on the attendance rate, nor cancellation, of surgeries. Similarly, there has not been any extensive research on text message reminders for surgeries, nor how the content can affect the execution of preoperative in- structions. Due to the degree project examining surgeries instead of appointments, it is not feasible to state specific costs in the reminder due to there being a wide range of surgeries with varying lengths and costs. Due to this, the reminder will rely on stating the importance of the desired actions to motivate the patients to act in order to decrease the amount of short notice cancellations.

The research question is, therefore, “How effective are SMS text message reminders, that state the importance of actions, at decreasing short notice cancellations?”.

METHOD

This section describes how the problem was encountered, the methods used to conduct the intervention and how the results were analyzed.

Encountering the problem

This thesis was part of a project of an organization with the aim to improve health care through the use of tech- nology. It was conceived through a project idea in which the hospital hosting the thesis sought to improve the patient information they distribute. The aforementioned hospital has chosen to be anonymous, as a precaution, in order to avoid the possibility of sensitive information being linked to it.

Initially, the gathering of information took place at the day surgery ward, as it was smaller than the other wards and was relatively calm, making it a suitable starting point. An ethnographic study was performed in order to gain knowledge of the workings of the ward as well as the hospital in general. However, while interviewing the patients, it seemed that they, for the most part, did not have any issues and were content with the information and treatment they received. The second primary loca- tion of the ethnographic study was at the office of the surgical operation planners who are tasked to schedule the patients for surgery and having contact with them regarding the matter.

(7)

During the study, several conversations were held with different members of the personnel; these were contex- tual conversations regarding the patients (which will be extended upon in the discussion section). After having these dialogs with them and listening to their reflections on the topic of patient information, it was decided that the focus of the study would be shifted to the problems identified by the personnel, as opposed to the patients, as they were more vocal about what could be improved.

Through their narratives, it was discovered that the prob- lem they experienced as most troublesome was canceled surgeries due to either patients not attending or not following preoperative instructions.

Additionally, during the literature study, a study was found in which the researchers examined if the content of text message reminders affected the attendance rate of doctor’s appointments. As this solution was intriguing it was decided that a similar solution should be tested to ascertain whether or not it could solve the problem of patient cancellations.

Reminders

The reminders were created together with the hospital’s surgical operation planners, head nurses, directors of the department and the unit for communication. The mes- sages were written in Swedish, but have been translated into English below:

Short reminder: "Hello and welcome to [name of hospital]

for your planned surgery [date]. If you wish to cancel or reschedule, contact us as soon as possible at num.

[number]."

Long reminder: "Hello and welcome to [name of hospital]

for your planned surgery [date]. Note: In order for your surgery to be executed you have to follow the instructions in the sent out summon.

If you wish to cancel or reschedule, contact us as soon as possible at num. [number] so we are able to schedule another patient."

The purpose of the "short" type reminder is to isolate the effects of introducing a text message reminder as it otherwise would be difficult to ascertain if the addi- tional information had any effect. The purpose of the

"long" type reminder is to see if the patients are more likely to cancel earlier and/or comply with preoperative instructions if it is explicitly mentioned in the reminder that they are required to follow the instructions. The sections that are part of the "long" type reminder, but not the "short" type reminder are; "Note: In order for your surgery to be executed you have to follow the instructions in the sent out summon." and "[...] so we are able to schedule another patient." . The first section aims to mention the importance of the preoperative instructions, the second section aims to elicit the patients’ pro-social motivation; the willingness to exert energy for someone else’s benefit. This is typically triggered by recognizing that others are in need of help [30]. Eliciting this type of motivation will hopefully make patients cancel earlier,

rather than later, in order to increase the chances of the hospital being able to operate on another patient.

Participants of the study were patients from the surgical-, urological- and orthodontic clinics (including both out- patients and inpatients). Surgical procedures performed at these clinics include hernias, kidney stone removals, and stabilization of fractures. The study was conducted between April 29, 2016, and June 3, 2016. Patients who had not indicated that they wish to receive text messages from the hospital were placed in the group that would not be receiving any reminders, the patients who had indicated they wanted to receive text messages were ran- domly placed into either the "short" reminder group or the "long" reminder group.

The patients were sent reminders seven days before their procedure, as suggested by the surgical operation plan- ners, through the messaging service TextLocal which made it possible to send multiple text messages at once through a web interface. The list of patients was ac- cessed the same day as the reminders would be sent, to ensure that the list was as up-to-date as possible since it is common to change the day of operation for patients as patients who are in greater need are routinely placed before others on the waiting list for surgeries.

The dependent variables in this study are whether or not the patient canceled, if the cancellation was done "on short notice" (defined as a cancellation that occurs the day before or on the day of operation) and whether or not the patients followed the preoperative instructions.

The independent variable is the group the patients were placed in. After the patients’ surgical procedure had been performed the surgery planning system was accessed to record the status of the dependent variables.

As the study took place in relation to health care, the ethics of the study have been considered by reviewing the Declaration of Helsinki; a statement of “ethical principles for medical research involving human subjects, including research on identifiable human material and data” devel- oped by The World Medical Association (WMA). The declaration is primarily aimed at physicians, however, WMA encourages others who perform similar research to follow them as well. The intervention performed in this degree project follows the principles mainly by ensuring that the primary purpose of the study is to improve on the intervention, the risks, burdens, and benefits have been assessed, and the privacy and information of the subjects have been protected. However, the study does violate some of the principals, such as those on informed consent.

Although only participants who have given consent to receiving text messages were recruited, they were not directly asked for their permission to be included in this study. The reasoning behind this was similar to the one in the study done by Hallsworth et al (2015) who reasoned that “obtaining consent would introduce burdens to the patient (larger than the intervention itself); obtaining informed consent would cause serious practical problems that would undermine the trial results (particularly for

(8)

the control group); and the risk of harm was low, since the intervention merely consisted of small modifications to existing routine processes.”. The difference between this study and theirs being that there has not been a text message reminder system in place for surgeries, however, considering that the information provided is similar to the information given on paper or orally, it can be viewed as a lesser modification as well.

Telephone interviews

In order to also gather the patients’ opinions and, poten- tially, confirm that the text itself affected their actions, semi-structured interviews were held via telephone, as it was the most straightforward method to come into contact with the patient after their visit to the hospi- tal. Patients were chosen at random and were asked about how the written information (consisting of the text message, summon and any other information about their surgery) contributed to their actions regarding the preparations for surgery and their attendance or lack thereof.

Analysis

In order to discover the relationship, or lack of a rela- tionship, between the variables of the study, a logistic regression model was applied using R 3.3.0. Three anal- yses were done; both using the reminder group as the independent variable, with the no reminder group as the reference group, and gender and age as covariates. The first regression analysis examined the patient attendance as the binary dependent variable, the second regression analysis examined the cancellation, the third examined the short notice cancellation.

RESULTS

This section will present the main results from the inter- vention, the logistic regression analysis, as well as the opinions of the patients that were contacted.

Intervention results

Out of the 537 patients treated by the clinics during the period of the study, 263 had not expressed that they wanted to receive a text message, while 274 had indicated that they did. Figure 1 presents the manner in which participants were placed into their respective groups.

Table 1 displays the main results of the study along with descriptive results in raw data. It should be noted that cancellations are divided into two groups, Cancel (Pat.);

the patients who had great control over the cause of the cancellation and Cancel(Oth.); the patients who did not have great control over the cause of the cancellation. The encountered causes for the cancellations and the manner in which they were split is presented in table 2. Lastly, the results of the statistical analysis are displayed in table 3.

There was only one instance of a patient that did not attend on the day of operation without contacting the hospital in any manner, however, the patient did not speak Swedish and was therefore excluded from the study

together with all other patients who required a trans- lator during their hospital stay due to their difficulty in understanding the reminders which were written in Swedish.

In the group that did not receive a reminder, 2.7% (7/241) of the patients canceled their surgery. 42.9% (3/7) of these cancellations were on short notice. 2.3% (3/121) of the patients in the short reminder group canceled, with no short notice cancellations. Lastly, 3.5% (5/129) of the long reminder group canceled, where 40.0% (2/5) of the cancellations were on short notice. This data is visualized as bar charts in figure 2.

Analysis results

In table 2 the encountered reasons for cancellation are displayed. The three topmost causes are the ones that are represented in the Cancel (Pat.) results in table 1, as they are the reasons which the patients have the most control over and were susceptible to the effect of the intervention, namely, reasons concerning rescheduling, desire to go through with surgery, and follow-through of preoperative instructions. These cancellations comprised 32.6% (15/46) of all cancellations. The lower half com- prise the Cancel (Oth.) results, which are the causes the patients do not have considerable control over, namely, reasons concerning the patient’s health or decisions made by the hospital. These cancellations comprised 67.4%

(31/46) of all cancellations.

The most common reason for cancellation, in which the patient has greater control, was related to rescheduling, which was the cause of cancellation for 10 patients in total: 5 patients from the no reminder group, 2 from the short reminder group and 3 from the long reminder group. 4 patients did not want to go through with surgery whatsoever, i.e. did not schedule another surgery. 1 patient was from the no reminder group, 1 from the short reminder group, and 2 from the long reminder group.

Only one patient was not allowed to proceed with their surgery due to not following the preoperative instructions.

The chi-squared test for the first regression analysis re- sulted in a P value of 0.54. This indicates that the results are not statistically significant. The regression analyses with cancellations and SNC were not statistically signifi- cant either with a P value of 0.54 and 0.95 respectively.

Table 3 shows the odds ratios, how likely the intervention is to give an effect compared to the reference, for the independent variables, with the 95% confidence intervals written below. The confidence intervals show where the true effect lies with a 95% certainty.

The odds of a patient, who received the short reminder, attending is 21% higher compared to the reference, a pa- tient who did not receive a reminder. On the other hand, for the patients receiving the long reminder, the odds are 23% lower. The analysis also indicates that males seem to be slightly less prone to attending. Furthermore, the analysis shows that the chance of attendance increases

(9)

Figure 1. Figure displaying in which manner the participants of the trial were distributed between the groups.

Table 1. Table displaying in which percentages the patients of the trial groups attended, did not attend, canceled due to patients’ choice, canceled due to other reasons, as well as the percentage of the cancellations (due to patients) that were on short notice. The groupings of the cancellations are displayed in table 2. In addition, the descriptive makeup of the groups is also displayed.

Main results Descriptive results

Reminder Attend DNA Cancel (Pat.) Cancel (Oth.) SNC N Male Female Age (Mean)

None 91.6% 0.0% 2.7% 5.7% 1.1% 263 57.0% 43.0% 58.8

Short 93,1% 0.0% 2.3% 4.6% 0.0% 130 58.5% 12.5% 56.6

Long 89.6% 0.0% 3.5% 6.9% 1.4% 144 45.1% 54.9% 57.3

Table 2. Reported reasons for canceling a surgery. The three topmost reasons are the cancellations which are referred to as Cancel (Pat.) in table 1 due to them being supposedly susceptible to the intervention. The remaining reasons are referred to as Cancel (Oth.) in table 1 due to them supposedly not being susceptible to the intervention.

Reason for cancellation N No reminder Short reminder Long reminder

Patient wants to reschedule surgery 10 5 2 3

Patient does not want to have surgery 4 1 1 2

Patient did not follow preoperative instructions 1 1 0 0

Patient is deemed unfit for surgery 10 5 3 2

Other surgery is given priority 9 5 1 3

Patient’s condition has worsened (at hospital) 4 2 2 0

Patient is hospitalized 3 0 0 3

Patient became ill (at home) 2 0 0 2

Surgery ran out of time 2 2 0 0

Complication during preparation of surgery 1 1 0 0

Total 46 22 9 15

with age by 2 percentage points per year. Similar results are seen for the second regression analysis.

As for short notice cancellations, the odds of a patient in the short reminder group canceling is not applicable since there were no patients in the short reminder group

who canceled on short notice. On the other hand, it is 13% more likely for patients of the long reminder group, compared to the control, to cancel. Additionally, males are 40% less likely to leave a short notice cancellation, and as the patients grow older, they are 2% less likely to leave a short notice cancellation per year.

(10)

Figure 2. A bar chart displaying the percentage of patients, where the cancellation would be susceptible to the interven- tion, according to the group they were placed in. The blue section represents the cancellations that were not on short notice. The orange sections represent the cancellations that were on short notice.

Table 3. Results of the multivariate logistic regression analysis showing, in odds ratios, how likely patients are to attend, cancel and cancel on short notice based on their trial group (compared to the no reminder group), sex and age. 95%

confidence intervals are written within brackets.

Variable Regression 1 Attend

Regression 2 Cancel (Pat.)

Regression 3 SNC

Short reminder 1.21 0.82 1.00 · 10−7

[0.33, 5.70] [0.18, 3.04] [NA, 4 .14 · 1067]

Long reminder 0.77 1.30 1.13

[0.24, 2.65] [0.38, 4.21] [0.15, 7.01}

Male 0.99 1.01 0.60

[0.34. 2.83] [0.35, 2.94] [0.08, 3.68]

Age 1.02 0.98 0.98

[1.00, 1.05] [0.95, 1.00] [0.94, 1.04]

N 537 537 537

Telephone interview results

Out of the 18 patients that were called, 5 were willing to participate in the survey. One patient from the surgical clinic, who had received the long reminder, stated that she preferred the text message over the summon because she lives on the countryside and therefore does not check her mailbox frequently. For this reason, she received information regarding the day of operation through the text message reminder first, and the summon second.

Another patient, from the day surgery ward, who had received the long reminder as well, stated that the re- minder was very helpful in helping him remember the surgery date. However, when asked if it helped him in remembering to perform the preoperative preparations, he stated there was no such reminder in the text message.

A third patient, from the urological clinic, who had re- ceived the short reminder, felt it was unnecessary for him, but that it might be useful for others.

One patient who had canceled her surgery due to feeling ill was in the long reminder group but did not have any recollection of receiving a text message.

The last patient, who received the long reminder, did not find the text message necessary since the surgery was of great importance, seemingly difficult to forget, and was in no need of a reminder. When asked about the content of the text message, she did not remember the segments that separate it from the short reminder, instead, she only recalled that it contained the date of the surgery and a welcoming message.

(11)

DISCUSSION

In this section, the results will be discussed in relation to the theory and related research. Additionally, the research question will be attempted to be answered.

The aim of this study was to see if text message reminders and its content had an effect on the SNC rates and the compliance of the preoperative instructions. Percentage- wise, the group in the long reminder group had more cancellations and short notice cancellations (3.5% and 1.4%). The no reminder group was second in both can- cellations and short notice cancellations (2.7% and 1.2%).

The short message reminder group performed the best, having the lowest percentage of cancellations, with no short notice cancellations (2.3% and 0.0%).

Regarding the logistic regression, the results show that the short reminder decreases the chance of cancellation by 18% while a long reminder increases the chance by 30%.

A similar effect is seen for SNC as no patients of the short reminder group canceled on short notice, while the long reminder group was 13% more likely to cancel on short notice (compared to the control). These results suggest that the long reminder surprisingly increases the chance that the surgery will be canceled as well as the chance that it will be done so on short notice. The short reminder, on the other hand, decreases this chance. This in turn suggests that the extra information in the long reminder did affect the patients differently, namely; "Note: In order for your surgery to be executed you have to follow the instructions in the sent out summon." and "[...] so we are able to schedule another patient.", driving them to cancel to a greater extent and to do so closer to the surgery date.

It may be possible that the text message increased the anxiety the patients were experiencing, maybe by fueling the feeling that they lack adequate knowledge of what will transpire, with the section "In order for your surgery to be executed[...]".

It could also be possible that the additional information in the long reminder was perceived as noise, increasing the chance that the information (the day of the surgery) is not remembered which could lead to a situation where the patient realizes too late that they cannot go through with surgery on the planned date, ultimately, canceling later compared to others. This explanation is supported by the telephone interviews in which none of the patients recalled the additional information in the long reminder.

It should be noted that no patients failed to attend, in this case, as opposed to other studies on improving attendance rate. Therefore, it is unwanted to have an increased cancellation rate, since instead of there being a negative correlation with the non-attendance rate, there is, instead, a negative correlation with the attendance rate.

As for the effect of the reminders on following the pre- operative instructions, not enough data was gathered in order to make a deduction, as there was only one pa- tient who was reported to have been denied their surgery due to not following the instructions. However, through

the regression analysis, it was possible to see that the intervention outcome could be the result of chance. It is therefore not possible to confidently reject the null hypothesis; that the reminders did not have an effect on the cancellations. One reason for this might be due to the fact that there were fewer cancellations than ex- pected. It was expected that approximately 12.4% of the surgical procedures would be canceled (the percent- age of cancellation in Sweden), which would result in roughly 66 cancellations, as opposed to 8.6% with 46 cancellations. Furthermore, the percentage of cancella- tions, where the cause was due to the patient, was lower than expected. Seeing as how these cancellations only constituted 32.6% of all cancellations, with only 0.9% of all patients canceling on short notice, SNC does not seem to be a considerable problem while the hospital narrative seemed to imply that it was. On the contrary, most cancellations were done within a reasonable amount of time or was caused by something outside of the patient’s control.

Possible issues with the method that could have led to inconclusive results could be due to the amount of time before the surgery that the text message was sent, 7 days, was of inappropriate length. It is possible it was sent too early, mitigating its effect. Due to its exploratory nature, the study required several assumptions, such as the appropriate time to send the text message, but also the size required to be able to obtain a significant result.

The study would most certainly have benefited from commencing earlier and continuing for a longer period, as this would allow more patients to partake in the study, increasing the sample and therefore the reliability of the results. Using the numbers gathered from the study, there being at least 263 patients in the control group and the combined intervention groups, with a significance level of 5 percent and power of 80%, a 5.6 percentage points difference between the no reminder group and the combined reminder groups would be required in order for there to be a significant result. As for detecting a difference between the two different reminders, which had at least 130 patients, a 7.3 percentage point difference is required. If instead, the appropriate sample size is calculated from the observed smallest SNC difference, 1.1 percentage points between the control group and the short reminder group, the study would require 706 patients in order to call the results statistically significant.

Another possible reason for the statistically insignificant results is that sending reminders might not be an effec- tive method for making patients change their behavior when it comes to surgeries, as opposed to doctor’s appoint- ments. The reasons for cancellations do not communicate whether or not the cancellations stemmed from the fact that the patients forgot about their surgery. As it was presented in the interview results, most of the patients did not seem to have a problem with actually remember- ing when their surgery was. This could mean that the patients’ cancellations do not have anything to do with remembering, and cannot be improved by reminders of

(12)

this nature, as in this study, since the underlying reason for why the patients canceled might be different from the reason patients cancel their doctor’s appointment.

During the contextual conversations that were held with the personnel, it was mentioned that many of the pa- tients experienced anxiety while at the hospital and the personnel worked actively with ensuring that the patients would feel calm and at ease. Since, according to studies, up to 80% experience anxiety regarding their surgery, it is possible that it is, in fact, anxiety, not a lapse of mem- ory, that is the main reason patients cancel. The reason might be that the patients cannot cope with the anxiety they are experiencing, leading to a situation where they, in order to feel better, remove the source of the anxiety (the surgery) by either postponing it or canceling it all together. If the cancellations are due to this, then it is natural that the reminders of this study do not work as they are not designed to prevent this feeling. Therefore, it is suggested that in order to decrease the short notice cancellations and cancellations all together, work has to be done to remove the patients’ anxiety, the source being, ultimately, the fear of the unknown. The hospitals should focus on informing patients of the fasting period, surgi- cal waiting period, isolation from a social environment, concerns about not waking after surgery, loss of control, intraoperative complications and postoperative fear of pain, as these are the common sources of anxiety.

Given that there are several different causes for anxiety it might not optimal to send all information, regarding these sources, in the summon (nor as text messages) since, as stated earlier, tailored information works better due to the fact that only the relevant information for the patient is presented, making it easier to process. For this reason, it might be more appropriate for a member of the personnel, such as a nurse, to either meet with the patient, or call the patient (which should require less resources) before the surgery, in order to discuss with them, making it possible to tailor the information given to them by, for example, providing them with extra information based on their demographics, or answering the questions they have. As stated in the theory, these discussions have proven to be useful in combating anxiety and may, therefore, be more apt in reducing the amount of cancellations.

Given this new information, it would me more appro- priate to focus more on anxiety and its correlation to cancellations. Through administering anxiety tests it would be possible to see if the patients who cancel are more prone to have higher levels of anxiety. It would also be possible to compare the control and reminder groups to see if the reminders affect their anxiety level.

As sustainability is becoming increasingly important it is of value to see how also this would contribute towards a more sustainable world. When considering the sustain- ability aspect, the hospital is benefited by trying to lower the amount of canceled surgeries as this will prevent the waste of resources that otherwise would not have been

used.

For future research studies, it would be interesting to further experiment with sending text messages that are tailored individually to each patient to both see if the main reason for cancellations is in fact anxiety, and if so, if text messages that pinpoint the worries of the patient can alleviate it.

CONCLUSION

The study suggests that reminders do not work as a method to decrease the short notice cancellation rate of surgeries as it may not be forgetfulness that causes the patient to cancel in the first place. Anxiety might play a bigger role than expected. Further studies should be done to confirm this hypothesis.

ACKNOWLEDGMENT

I would first like to thank my academic supervisor Helena Tobiasson of the Media Technology and Interaction De- sign faculty at KTH Royal Institute of Technology for all the advice and support she has given me throughout the degree project, and my examiner Jan Gulliksen, from the same faculty, for his invaluable comments on this thesis.

I would also like to thank project supervisor Clara from the organization that set up this project for her advice on how to navigate through the unfamiliar hospital envi- ronment and hierarchy. I would also like to thank Anette and Kerstin from the hospital for their help with all my health care related questions. I am also grateful for Awa Seck, a fellow student at KTH, with whom I initially worked on this project on, but had to withdraw.

Finally, I would like to thank my parents and Somi for providing me with support and encouragement through- out my studies.

REFERENCES

1. Bech M. 2005. The economics of non-attendance and the expected effect of charging a fine on

non-attendees. Health Policy 74, 2 (2005), 181–191.

DOI:http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.healthpol.2005.01.001

2. Murdock A., Rodgers C., Lindsay H., and Tham T. C. K. 2002. Why do patients not keep their appointments? Prospective study in a

gastroenterology outpatient clinic. Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine 95, 6 (2002), 284–286.

DOI:http://dx.doi.org/10.1258/jrsm.95.6.284

3. KPMG. 2012. Inställda operationer, Revisionsrapport. Region Skåne (2012).

4. Svenskt perioperativt register. 2014. Årsrapport SPOR 2014. Technical Report. 1–31 pages.

5. Neal R. D., Hussain-Gambles M., Allgar V. L., Lawlor D. A., and Dempsey O. 2005. Reasons for and consequences of missed appointments in general practice in the UK: questionnaire survey and prospective review of medical records. BMC family

(13)

practice 6 (2005), 47. DOI:

http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/1471-2296-6-47

6. Chen Z., Fang L., Chen L., and Dai H. 2008.

Comparison of an SMS text messaging and phone reminder to improve attendance at a health promotion center: a randomized controlled trial.

Journal of Zhejiang University. Science. B 9, 1 (2008), 34–8. DOI:

http://dx.doi.org/10.1631/jzus.B071464

7. Banerjee A., Chitnis U.B., Jadhav S. L., Bhawalkar J. S., and Chaudhury S. 2009. Hypothesis testing, type I and type II errors. Industrial psychiatry journal 18, 2 (2009), 127. DOI:

http://dx.doi.org/10.4103/0972-6748.62274

8. Gilmartin J. and Wright K. 2008. Day surgery:

Patients’ felt abandoned during the preoperative wait. Journal of Clinical Nursing 17, 18 (2008), 2418–2425. DOI:

http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2702.2008.02374.x

9. Khavanin N., Mlodinow A., Milad M. P., Bilimoria K. Y., and Kim J. Y. S. 2013. Comparison of Perioperative Outcomes in Outpatient and Inpatient Laparoscopic Hysterectomy. Journal of Minimally Invasive Gynecology 20, 5 (2013), 604–610. DOI:

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jmig.2013.03.007

10. Lemos P., Regalado A., Marques D., Castanheira C., Malafaia F., Almeida M., Lanhoso M., and Salgado P. 2003. The economic benefits of ambulatory surgery relative to inpatient surgery for laparoscopic tubal ligation. Ambulatory Surgery 10, 2 (2003), 61–65. DOI:

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0966-6532(02)00040-9

11. J. Engbæk, J. Bartholdy, and N. C. Hjortsø. 2006.

Return hospital visits and morbidity within 60 days after day surgery: A retrospective study of 18,736 day surgical procedures. Acta Anaesthesiologica Scandinavica 50, 8 (2006), 911–919. DOI:

http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1399-6576.2006.01090.x

12. Kisic-Trope J., Qvigstad E., and Ballard K. 2011. A randomized trial of day-case vs inpatient

laparoscopic supracervical hysterectomy. American Journal of Obstetrics and Gynecology 204, 4 (2011).

DOI:http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ajog.2010.11.014

13. Söderström H. and Åkesdotter Gustafsson B. 2016.

Preoperativ vård. (2016).http://www.vardhandboken.

se/Texter/Operationsvard/Preoperativ-vard/

14. Buggy D. J. 2012. Oxford Handbook of Anaesthesia.

British Journal of Anaesthesia 108, 4 (2012), 707–707. DOI:http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/bja/aes076

15. Merriam-Webster.com. 2016. anxiety. (Retrieved 4 July, 2016).

http://www.merriam-webster.com/dictionary/anxiety

16. Gürsoy A., Candaş B., Güner S., and Yılmaz S.

2016. Preoperative Stress: An Operating Room Nurse Intervention Assessment. Journal of PeriAnesthesia Nursing (2016), 1–9. DOI:

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jopan.2015.08.011

17. Bailey L. 2010. Strategies for decreasing patient anxiety in the perioperative setting. AORN Journal 92, 4 (2010), 445–460. DOI:

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.aorn.2010.04.017

18. Ingadottir B., Johansson Stark Å., Leino-Kilpi H., Sigurdardottir A. K., Valkeapää K., and Unosson M.

2014. The fulfilment of knowledge expectations during the perioperative period of patients

undergoing knee arthroplasty - a Nordic perspective.

Journal of Clinical Nursing 23, 19-20 (2014), 2896–2908. DOI:http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/jocn.12552

19. Kessels R. P. C. 2003. Patients’ memory for medical information. J R Soc Med 96 (2003), 219–222.

20. Goodman H., Davison J., Preedy M., Peters E., Waters P., Persaud-Rai B., Shuldham C., Pepper J., and Cowie M. R. 2009. Patient and staff perspective of a nurse-led support programme for patients waiting for cardiac surgery:. Participant perspective of a cardiac support programme. European Journal of Cardiovascular Nursing 8, 1 (2009), 67–73. DOI:

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ejcnurse.2008.03.001

21. Jibaja-Weiss M. L., Volk R. J., Smith Q. W., Holcomb J. D., and Kingery P. 2005. Differential effects of messages for breast and cervical cancer screening. Journal of Health Care for the Poor &

Underserved 16, 1 (2005), 42–52.

http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true

22. Thomas D. 2004. Postal reminders can improve attendance at orthodontic clinics. Evidence-based dentistry 5, 1 (2004), 14. DOI:

http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/sj.ebd.6400244

23. Maxwell S., Maljanian R., Horowitz S., Pianka M. A., Cabrera Y., and Greene J. 2001.

Effectiveness of reminder systems on appointment adherence rates. Journal of health care for the poor and underserved 12, 4 (2001), 504–14. DOI:

http://dx.doi.org/10.1353/hpu.2010.0766

24. Shoffner J., Staudt M., Marcus S., and Kapp S.

2007. Using telephone reminders to increase attendance at psychiatric appointments: findings of a pilot study in rural Appalachia. Psychiatric services (Washington, D.C.) 58, 6 (2007), 872–5.

DOI:http://dx.doi.org/10.1176/appi.ps.58.6.872

25. Farmer T., Brook G., McSorley J., Murphy S., and Mohamed A. 2014. Using short message service text reminders to reduce ’did not attend’ rates in sexual health and HIV appointment clinics. International journal of STD & AIDS 25, 4 (2014), 289–93. DOI:

http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0956462413502325

(14)

26. Leong K. C., Chen W. S., Leong K. W., Mastura I., Mimi O., Sheikh M. A., Zailinawati A. H., Phua C. J., Ngand K. L., and Teng C. L. 2006. The use of text messaging to improve attendance in primary care: a randomized controlled trial. Family practice 23, 6 (2006), 699–705. DOI:

http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/fampra/cml044

27. Hallsworth M., Berry D., Sanders M., Sallis A., King D., Vlaev I., and Darzi A. 2015. Stating appointment costs in SMS reminders reduces missed hospital appointments: Findings from two

randomised controlled trials. PLoS ONE 10, 9 (2015). DOI:

http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0137306

28. Kreuter M. W., Strecher V. J., and Glassman B.

1999. One size does not fit all: the case for tailoring print materials. Annals of behavioral medicine : a publication of the Society of Behavioral Medicine 21, 4 (1999), 276–283. DOI:

http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF02895958

29. Skinner C. S., Strecher V. J., and Hospers H. 1994.

Physicians’ recommendations for mammography: Do tailored messages make a difference? American Journal of Public Health 84, 1 (1994), 43–49. DOI:

http://dx.doi.org/10.2105/AJPH.84.1.43

30. Grant A. M. 2008. Does intrinsic motivation fuel the prosocial fire? Motivational synergy in predicting persistence, performance, and productivity. The Journal of applied psychology 93, 1 (2008), 48–58.

DOI:http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.93.1.48

References

Related documents

The research questions investigated concerned teachers’ thoughts and experiences of students’ use of the target language both inside and outside the classroom,

Since public corporate scandals often come from the result of management not knowing about the misbehavior or unsuccessful internal whistleblowing, companies might be

The final report of the thesis work should be written in English or Swedish as a scientific report in your field but also taking into consideration the particular guidelines that

Fewer students (23%) watch English speaking shows and movies without subtitles on a weekly basis or more in this group than in the advanced group.. The opposite is true when it

Based upon this, one can argue that in order to enhance innovation during the time of a contract, it is crucial to have a systematic of how to handle and evaluate new ideas

In this thesis we investigated the Internet and social media usage for the truck drivers and owners in Bulgaria, Romania, Turkey and Ukraine, with a special focus on

However, employees’ reaction and behaviour is likely to be related to how they perceive organisational support (Eisenberger et al., 1986; Rousseau, 1989). Incentives are

In order to understand what the role of aesthetics in the road environment and especially along approach roads is, a literature study was conducted. Th e literature study yielded