• No results found

Denna avhandling utgörs av tre studier (I-III) av barns sociala perception.

Alla studier har använt mätning av ögonrörelser som metod. I Studie I och Studie II undersöktes perception av handlingar hos barn med normal utveckling och hos barn med autismspektrumstörning. I Studie III undersöktes ansiktsperception hos barn med autismspektrumstörning.

Enligt en inflytelserik teori ligger hjärnans Spegelneuronsystem bakom vår förståelse av andras handlingar. Spegelneuronsystemet låter oss förstå andras handlingar genom att det knyter sensorisk information från handlingar vi ser andra göra till vår egen motoriska erfarenhet med liknande handlingar. I Studie I mätte vi ögonrörelser hos sex och tolv månader gamla barn och hos vuxna när de tittade på andras manuella handlingar (flytta leksaker till en hink). Vuxna och tolv månader gamla barn flyttade blicken till hinken innan handen hade nått dit. Sex månader gamla barn flyttade däremot blicken till hinken efter handen hade nått fram. Reaktiva ögonrörelser observerades även hos vuxna och tolv månader gamla barn när de tittade på leksaker som förflyttas av sig själva till hinken, vilket antyder att prediktiva ögonrörelser kräver att man ser en interaktion mellan en hand och ett föremål. Utvecklingen av prediktiva ögonrörelser vid observation av andras handlingar sammanfaller med spädbarns egna motoriska utveckling (tolv månader gamla barn kan flytta saker till hinkar, sex månader gamla barn kan det inte). Dessa fynd stämmer väl överens med synen att Spegelneuronsystemet – tränad genom egen motorisk utveckling – utgör ett fundament för barns prediktion av andras handlingar.

Enligt en relaterad teori kan sociala symptom vid autismspektrumstörning härledas till störningar i Spegelneuronsystemet. I Studie II fick förskolebarn med autismspektrumstörning samt barn och vuxna med normal utveckling titta på de samma filmerna som användes i Studie I. Om teorin är korrekt, förväntas man inte observera samma titt-beteende vid observation av handlingar hos barn med autismspektrumstörning som hos barn med normal utveckling. Vi fann emellertid at grupperna inte skilde sig åt. Alla använde prediktiva ögonrörelser till målområden vid observation av andras manuella handlingar. Detta fynd stödjer inte synen att störningar i Spegelneuronsystemet är förklaringen till sociala symptom vid autismspektrumstörning.

tyder på att barn med autismspektrumstörning fokuserar på delarna i ett ansikte, medan barn med normal utveckling tenderar att se ansiktet som en helhet. I Studie III visade vi ansikten för barn med autismspektrumstörningar, och analyserade titt-beteende i relation till deras kliniska symptom. Resultaten stödde synen att barn med autismspektrumstörning bearbetar information från ansiktets olika delar utan att integrera dem till en helhet. Det fanns stor variation i titt-beteende inom gruppen med autismspektrumstörning, variation som kunde förklaras av individuella skillnader i sociala och kommunikativa färdigheter.

Acknowledgements

First of all, I would like to thank my first supervisor Claes von Hofsten for his invaluable support and guidance in all stages of the research. I am also very thankful to my second supervisor Kerstin Rosander. Her enthusiasm has inspired me and her hands-on experience with various techniques has helped me overcome numerous problems. Due to Claes and Kerstin’s hospitality and engagement, it has been a pleasure to work at the Uppsala Babylab, both scientific-wise and social-wise.

Although not my supervisor in a formal sense, Gustaf Gredebäck has been as much a supervisor as one can imagine. Especially during the first years (before he moved to Oslo), he helped me on a day-to-day basis. Gustaf is a great scientist, but also a great teacher that listens to others’ ideas and gives freedom to develop at one’s own pace.

I would also like to thank all the people at the Babylab, in particular Pär Nyström and Ben Kenward who have helped me with technical as well as scientific questions. I would also like to thank Andrea Handl, Gunilla Bohlin and Peter Juslin for helping me improve the quality of this thesis. Thanks also to my co-authors Elisabeth Fernell and Christopher Gillberg for support and fruitful discussions.

I am also very thankful to my parents, Cecilie and Alf Henrik, who never grew tired of my endless childhood inquiries (“who created the universe?”

etc). I am also very thankful to my grandfather Nils Martin for showing continuous support and interest in my work, and to my sister and brother, Kaja and Martin.

My two daughters Elsa and Hedda have also been extremely supportive in their own, mainly implicit, ways. Finally, I would like to thank Karin for her patience, her advice, her caring and for her firm conviction that there are more important things in life than science.

References

Abrahams, B. S., & Geschwind, D. H. (2008). Advances in autism genetics:

On the threshold of a new neurobiology. Nature Reviews Genetics, 9(5), 341-355.

Adolphs, R., Gosselin, F., Buchanan, T. W., Tranel, D., Schyns, P., &

Damasio, A. R. (2005). A mechanism for impaired fear recognition after amygdala damage. Nature, 433(7021), 68-72.

Aglioti, S. M., Cesari, P., Romani, M., & Urgesi, C. (2008). Action

anticipation and motor resonance in elite basketball players. Nature Neuroscience, 11(9), 1109-1116.

Amaral, D. G., Schumann, C. M., & Nordahl, C. W. (2008). Neuroanatomy of autism. Trends In Neurosciences, 31(3), 137-145.

American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4 ed.). Washington DC: American Psychiatric Association.

Aslin, R. N., McMurray, B. (2004). Automated Corneal-Reflection Eye Tracking in Infancy: Methodological Developments and Applications to Cognition. Infancy, 6(2), 155-163.

Avikainen, S., Kulomaki, T., & Hari, R. (1999). Normal movement reading in asperger subjects. Neuroreport, 10(17), 3467-3470.

Baron-Cohen, S., Knickmeyer, R. C., & Belmonte, M. K. (2005). Sex differences in the brain: Implications for explaining autism. Science, 310(5749), 819-823.

Bentin, S., Allison, T., Puce, A., Perez, E., & McCarthy, G. (1996).

Electrophysiological studies of face perception in humans. Journal Of Cognitive Neuroscience, 8(6), 551-565.

Berument, S. K., Rutter, M., Lord, C., Pickles, A., & Bailey, A. (1999).

Autism screening questionnaire: Diagnostic validity. British Journal Of Psychiatry, 175, 444-451.

Boucher, J., & Lewis, V. (1992). Unfamiliar face recognition in relatively able autistic-children. Journal Of Child Psychology And Psychiatry And Allied Disciplines, 33(5), 843-859.

Brambring, M. (2007). Divergent development of verbal skills in children who are blind or sighted. Journal Of Visual Impairment &

Blindness, 101(12), 749-762.

Brass, M., Schmitt, R. M., Spengler, S., & Gergely, G. (2007). Investigating action understanding: Inferential processes versus action simulation.

Current Biology, 17(24), 2117-2121.

Bruner, J. S. (1970). The growth and structure of skill. In K. J. Connolly (Ed.), Mechanisms of motor skill development (pp. 63-91). London:

Academic Press.

Buccino, G., Lui, F., Canessa, N., Patteri, I., Lagravinese, G., Benuzzi, F., et al. (2004). Neural circuits involved in the recognition of actions performed by nonconspecifics: An fmri study. Journal Of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16(1), 114-126.

Calvo-Merino, B., Grezes, J., Glaser, D. E., Passingham, R. E., & Haggard, P. (2006). Seeing or doing? Influence of visual and motor familiarity in action observation. Current Biology, 16(19), 1905-1910.

Cannon, E., & Woodward, A. L. (2008). Action anticipation and interference: A test of prospective gaze. Paper presented at the Proceedings of the Meetings of the Cognitive Science Society, Austin, TX.

Cannon, E., & Woodward, A. L. (in press). Action anticipation and

interference: A test of prospective gaze. Proceedings of the Meetings of the Cognitive Science Society.

Carey, S., & Diamond, R. (1977). From piecemeal to configurational representation of faces. Science, 195(4275), 312-314.

Carr, L., Iacoboni, M., Dubeau, M. C., Mazziotta, J. C., & Lenzi, G. L.

(2003). Neural mechanisms of empathy in humans: A relay from neural systems for imitation to limbic areas. Proceedings Of The National Academy Of Sciences Of The United States Of America, 100(9), 5497-5502.

Catmur, C., Walsh, V., & Heyes, C. (2007). Sensorimotor learning

configures the human mirror system. Current Biology, 17(17), 1527-1531.

Cattaneo, L., Fabbri-Destro, M., Boria, S., Pieraccini, C., Monti, A., Cossu, G., et al. (2007). Impairment of actions chains in autism and its possible role in intention understanding. Proceedings Of The National Academy Of Sciences Of The United States Of America, 104(45), 17825-17830.

Chong, T. T. J., Cunnington, R., Williams, M. A., Kanwisher, N., &

Mattingley, J. B. (2008). Fmri adaptation reveals mirror neurons in human inferior parietal cortex. Current Biology, 18(20), 1576-1580.

Csibra, G. (2007). Action mirroring and action interpretation: An alternative account. In P. Haggard, Y. Rosetti & M. Kawato (Eds.),

Sensorimotor foundations of higher cognition. Attention and performance xxii (pp. 435-459). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Csibra, G., Biro, S., Koos, O., & Gergely, G. (2003). One-year-old infants use teleological representations of actions productively. Cognitive Science, 27(1), 111-133.

Csibra, G., & Gergely, G. (2007). 'obsessed with goals': Functions and mechanisms of teleological interpretation of actions in humans. Acta Psychologica, 124(1), 60-78.

Csibra, G., Gergely, G., Biro, S., Koos, O., & Brockbank, M. (1999). Goal attribution without agency cues: The perception of 'pure reason' in infancy. Cognition, 72(3), 237-267.

D'Ausillio, A., Pulvermuller, F., Salmas, P., Bufalari, I., Begliomini, C., &

Fadiga, L. (2009). The motor somatotopy of speech perception.

Current Biology, 381-385.

Dalton, K. M., Nacewicz, B. M., Johnstone, T., Schaefer, H. S.,

Gernsbacher, M. A., Goldsmith, H. H., et al. (2005). Gaze fixation and the neural circuitry of face processing in autism. Nature Neuroscience, 8(4), 519-526.

Damasio, A. R., & Maurer, R. G. (1978). Neurological model for childhood autism. Archives Of Neurology, 35(12), 777-786.

Dapretto, M., Davies, M. S., Pfeifer, J. H., Scott, A. A., Sigman, M., Bookheimer, S. Y., et al. (2006). Understanding emotions in others:

Mirror neuron dysfunction in children with autism spectrum disorders. Nature Neuroscience, 9(1), 28-30.

Darwin, C. (1872). The expression of the emotions in man and animals.

Dawson, G., Webb, S. J., Wijsman, E., Schellenberg, G., Estes, A., Munson, J., et al. (2005). Neurocognitive and electrophysiological evidence of altered face processing in parents of children with autism:

Implications for a model of abnormal development of social brain circuitry in autism. Development And Psychopathology, 17(3), 679-697.

de Gelder, B. C., Frissen, I., Barton, J., & Hadjikhani, N. (2003). A modulatory role for facial expressions in prosopagnosia.

Proceedings Of The National Academy Of Sciences Of The United States Of America, 100(22), 13105-13110.

de Heering, A., Houthuys, S., & Rossion, B. (2007). Holistic face processing is mature at 4 years of age: Evidence from the composite face effect.

Journal Of Experimental Child Psychology, 96(1), 57-70.

De Lange, F. P., Spronk, M., Willems, R. M., Toni, I., & Bekkering, H.

(2008). Complementary systems for understanding action intentions.

Current Biology, 18(6), 454-457.

Dinstein, I., Hasson, U., Rubin, N., & Heeger, D. J. (2007). Brain areas selective for both observed and executed movements. Journal Of Neurophysiology, 98(3), 1415-1427.

Dinstein, I., Thomas, C., Behrmann, M., & Heeger, D. J. (2008). A mirror up to nature. Current Biology, 18(3), R13-R18.

Dinstein, I., Thomas, C., Humphreys, K., Minshew, N., Behrmann, M., &

Heeger, D. J. (2010). Normal movement selectivity in autism.

Neuron, 66(3), 461-469.

Eshuis, R., Coventry, K. R., & Vulchanova, M. (2009). Predictive eye movements are driven by goals, not by the mirror neuron system.

Psychological Science, 20(4), 438-440.

Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., Pavesi, G., & Rizzolatti, G. (1995). Motor

facilitation during action observation - a magnetic stimulation study.

Journal Of Neurophysiology, 73(6), 2608-2611.

Fagan, J. F. (1972). Infants recognition memory for faces. Journal Of Experimental Child Psychology, 14(3), 453-476.

Falck-Ytter, T. (2008). Face inversion effects in autism: A combined looking time and pupillometric study. Autism Research, 1(5), 297-306.

Falck-Ytter, T. (2010). Young children with autism spectrum disorder use predictive eye movements in action observation. Biology Letters, 6(3), 375-378.

Falck-Ytter, T., Gredebäck, G., & von Hofsten, C. (2006). Infants predict other people's action goals. Nature Neuroscience, 9(7), 878-879.

Falck-Ytter, T., & Grip, L. (manuscript in preparation).

Flanagan, J. R., & Johansson, R. S. (2003). Action plans used in action observation. Nature, 424(6950), 769-771.

Fogassi, L., Ferrari, P. F., Gesierich, B., Rozzi, S., Chersi, F., & Rizzolatti, G. (2005). Parietal lobe: From action organization to intention understanding. Science, 308(5722), 662-667.

Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., Fadiga, L., & Rizzolatti, G. (1998). Neurons responding to the sight of goal-directed hand/arm actions in the parietal area pf (7b) of the macaque monkey. Social Neuroscience Abstracts, 24, 257.255.

Fombonne, E. (2005). Epidemiology of autistic disorder and other pervasive developmental disorders. Journal Of Clinical Psychiatry, 66, 3-8.

Frith, C. D., & Frith, U. (2006). The neural basis of mentalizing. Neuron, 50(4), 531-534.

Frith, U. (1989). Autism: Explaining the enigma. Oxford: Blackwell.

Gallese, V., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., & Rizzolatti, G. (1996). Action recognition in the premotor cortex. Brain, 119, 593-609.

Gallese, V., & Goldman, A. (1998). Mirror neurons and the simulation theory of mind-reading. Trends In Cognitive Sciences, 2(12), 493-501.

Gallese, V., Keysers, C., & Rizzolatti, G. (2004). A unifying view of the basis of social cognition. Trends In Cognitive Sciences, 8(9), 396-403.

Gazzola, V., Rizzolatti, G., Wicker, B., & Keysers, C. (2007a). The anthropomorphic brain: The mirror neuron system responds to human and robotic actions. Neuroimage, 35(4), 1674-1684.

Gazzola, V., van der Worp, H., Mulder, T., Wicker, B., Rizzolatti, G., &

Keysers, C. (2007b). Aplasics born without hands mirror the goal of hand actions with their feet. Current Biology, 17(14), 1235-1240.

Gergely, G., & Csibra, G. (2003). Teleological reasoning in infancy: The naive theory of rational action. Trends In Cognitive Sciences, 7(7), 287-292.

Gergely, G., Zoltán, N., Csibra, G., & Szilvia, B. (1995). Taking the intentional stance at 12 months of age. Cognition, 56, 165-193.

Gobbini, M. I., Koralek, A. C., Bryan, R. E., Montgomery, K. J., & Haxby, J. V. (2007). Two takes on the social brain: A comparison of theory

Goffaux, V., & Rossion, B. (2006). Faces are "spatial" - holistic face perception is supported by low spatial frequencies. Journal Of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception And Performance, 32(4), 1023.

Gredebäck, G., Stasiewicz, D., Falck-Ytter, T., Rosander, K., & von Hofsten, C. (2009). Action type and goal type modulate goal-directed gaze shifts in 14-month-old infants. Developmental Psychology, 45(4), 1190-1194.

Gredebäck, G., Theuring, C., Hauf, P., & Kenward, B. (2008). The microstructure of infants' gaze as they view adult shifts in overt attention. Infancy, 13(5), 533-543.

Hamilton, A. F. D., Brindley, R. M., & Frith, U. (2007). Imitation and action understanding in autistic spectrum disorders: How valid is the hypothesis of a deficit in the mirror neuron system?

Neuropsychologia, 45(8), 1859-1868.

Happe, F., & Frith, U. (2006). The weak coherence account: Detail-focused cognitive style in autism spectrum disorders. Journal Of Autism And Developmental Disorders, 36(1), 5-25.

Happe, F., Ronald, A., & Plomin, R. (2006). Time to give up on a single explanation for autism. Nature Neuroscience, 9(10), 1218-1220.

Hasselmo, M. E., Rolls, E. T., & Baylis, G. C. (1989). The role of expression and identity in the face-selective responses of neurons in the

temporal visual-cortex of the monkey. Behavioural Brain Research, 32(3), 203-218.

Haxby, J. V., Hoffman, E. A., & Gobbini, M. I. (2000). The distributed human neural system for face perception. Trends In Cognitive Sciences, 4(6), 223-233.

Heider, F., & Simmel, M. (1944). An experimental study of apparent behavior. The American Journal of Psychology, 57(2), 243-259.

Heyes, C. (2010). Where do mirror neurons come from? Neuroscience And Biobehavioral Reviews, 34(4), 575-583.

Hobson, R. P., Ouston, J., & Lee, A. (1988). Whats in a face - the case of autism. British Journal Of Psychology, 79, 441-453.

Hunnius, S., & Geuze, R. H. (2004). Developmental changes in visual scanning of dynamic faces and abstract stimuli in infants: A longitudinal study. Infancy, 6(2), 231-255.

Hurley, S., Clark, A., & Kiverstein, J. (2008). The shared circuits model (scm): How control, mirroring, and simulation can enable imitation, deliberation, and mindreading. Behavioral And Brain Sciences, 31(1), 1-22.

Iacoboni, M., & Dapretto, M. (2006). The mirror neuron system and the consequences of its dysfunction. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 7(12), 942-951.

Iacoboni, M., Woods, R. P., Brass, M., Bekkering, H., Mazziotta, J. C., &

Rizzolatti, G. (1999). Cortical mechanisms of human imitation.

Science, 286(5449), 2526-2528.

Inhoff, A. W., & Jian, W. (1992). Encoding of text, manual movement planning, and eye hand coordination during copytyping. Journal Of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception And Performance, 18(2), 437-448.

Itier, R. J., Alain, C., Sedore, K., & McIntosh, A. R. (2007). Early face processing specificity: It's in the eyes! Journal Of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19(11), 1815-1826.

Johansson, R. S., Westling, G. R., Backstrom, A., & Flanagan, J. R. (2001).

Eye-hand coordination in object manipulation. Journal Of Neuroscience, 21(17), 6917-6932.

Johnson, C. P., & Myers, S. M. (2007). Identification and evaluation of children with autism spectrum disorders. Pediatrics, 120(5), 1183-1215.

Johnson, M. H. (2005). Subcortical face processing. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6(10), 766-774.

Joseph, J. E., Gathers, A. D., Liu, X., Corbly, C. R., Whitaker, S. K., &

Bhatt, R. S. (2006). Neural developmental changes in processing inverted faces. Cognitive Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 6(3), 223-235.

Kenworthy, L., Yerys, B. E., Anthony, L. G., & Wallace, G. L. (2008).

Understanding executive control in autism spectrum disorders in the lab and in the real world. Neuropsychology Review, 18(4), 320-338.

Keysers, C., & Gazzola, V. (2010). Social neuroscience: Mirror neurons recorded in humans. Current Biology, 20(8), R353-R354.

Keysers, C., & Perrett, D. I. (2004). Demystifying social cognition: A hebbian perspective. Trends In Cognitive Sciences, 8(11), 501-507.

Kilner, J. M., & Frith, C. D. (2008). Action observation: Inferring intentions without mirror neurons. Current Biology, 18(1), R32-R33.

Kiraly, I., Jovanovic, B., Prinz, W., Aschersleben, G., & Gergely, G. (2003).

The early origins of goal attribution in infancy. Consciousness And Cognition, 12(4), 752-769.

Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R., Volkmar, F., & Cohen, D. (2002). Visual fixation patterns during viewing of naturalistic social situations as predictors of social competence in individuals with autism. Archives Of General Psychiatry, 59(9), 809-816.

Klin, A., Lin, D. J., Gorrindo, P., Ramsay, G., & Jones, W. (2009). Two-year-olds with autism orient to non-social contingencies rather than biological motion. Nature, 459(7244), 257-261.

Knapp, M., Romeo, R., & Beecham, J. (2009). Economic cost of autism in the uk. Autism, 13(3), 317-336.

Kohler, E., Keysers, C., Umilta, M. A., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., &

Rizzolatti, G. (2002). Hearing sounds, understanding actions: Action representation in mirror neurons. Science, 297(5582), 846-848.

Land, M. F. (1992). Predictable eye head coordination during driving.

Nature, 359(6393), 318-320.

Land, M. F., & Furneaux, S. (1997). The knowledge base of the oculomotor system. Philosophical Transactions Of The Royal Society Of London Series B-Biological Sciences, 352(1358), 1231-1239.

Land, M. F., Mennie, N., & Rusted, J. (1999). The role of vision and eye movements in the control of activities of daily living. Perception, 28, 1311-1328.

Lecavalier, L., Aman, M. G., Scahill, L., McDougle, C. J., McCracken, J. T., Vitiello, B., et al. (2006). Validity of the autism diagnostic

interview-revised. American Journal On Mental Retardation, 111(3), 199-215.

Leighton, J., Bird, G., Charman, T., & Heyes, C. (2008). Weak imitative performance is not due to a functional 'mirroring' deficit in adults with autism spectrum disorders. Neuropsychologia, 46(4), 1041-1049.

Lobmaier, J. S., & Mast, F. W. (2007). Perception of novel faces: The parts have it! Perception, 36(11), 1660-1673.

Lord, C., Rutter, M., & Lecouteur, A. (1994). Autism diagnostic interview-revised - a interview-revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders.

Journal Of Autism And Developmental Disorders, 24(5), 659-685.

Ludman, C. N., Summerfield, A. Q., Hall, D., Elliott, M., Foster, J., Hykin, J. L., et al. (2000). Lip-reading ability and patterns of cortical activation studied using fmri. British Journal Of Audiology, 34(4), 225-230.

Luo, Y., & Baillargeon, R. (2005). Can a self-propelled box have a goal?

Psychological reasoning in 5-month-old infants. Psychological Science, 16(8), 601-608.

Mascalzoni, E., Regolin, L., & Vallortigara, G. (2010). Innate sensitivity for self-propelled causal agency in newly hatched chicks. Proceedings Of The National Academy Of Sciences Of The United States Of America, 107(9), 4483-4585.

Maurer, D., Le Grand, R., & Mondloch, C. J. (2002). The many faces of configural processing. Trends In Cognitive Sciences, 6(6), 255-260.

McGurk, H., & Macdonald, J. (1976). Hearing lips and seeing voices.

Nature, 264(5588), 746-748.

McPartland, J., Dawson, G., Webb, S. J., Panagiotides, H., & Carver, L. J.

(2004). Event-related brain potentials reveal anomalies in temporal processing of faces in autism spectrum disorder. Journal Of Child Psychology And Psychiatry, 45(7), 1235-1245.

Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. (1977). Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science, 198, 75-78.

Miall, R. C. (2003). Connecting mirror neurons and forward models.

Neuroreport, 14(17), 2135-2137.

Mondloch, C. J., Le Grand, R., & Maurer, D. (2002). Configural face processing develops more slowly than featural face processing.

Perception, 31(5), 553-566.

Morris, J. S., DeGelder, B., Weiskrantz, L., & Dolan, R. J. (2001).

Differential extrageniculostriate and amygdala responses to presentation of emotional faces in a cortically blind field. Brain, 124, 1241.

Moscovitch, M., Winocur, G., & Behrmann, M. (1997). What is special about face recognition? Nineteen experiments on a person with visual object agnosia and dyslexia but normal face recognition.

Journal Of Cognitive Neuroscience, 9(5), 555-604.

Mottron, L., Dawson, M., Soulieres, I., Hubert, B., & Burack, J. (2006).

Enhanced perceptual functioning in autism: An update, and eight principles of autistic perception. Journal Of Autism And

Developmental Disorders, 36(1), 27-43.

Mukamel, R., Ekstrom, A. D., Kaplan, J., Iacoboni, M., & Fried, I. (2010).

Single-neuron responses in humans during execution and observation of actions. Current Biology, 20(8), 750-756.

Myers, S. M., & Johnson, C. P. (2007). Management of children with autism spectrum disorders. Pediatrics, 120(5), 1162-1182.

Narumoto, J., Okada, T., Sadato, N., Fukui, K., & Yonekura, Y. (2001).

Attention to emotion modulates fmri activity in human right superior temporal sulcus. Cognitive Brain Research, 12(2), 225-231.

Nishitani, N., & Hari, R. (2002). Viewing lip forms: Cortical dynamics.

Neuron, 36(6), 1211-1220.

Norbury, C. F., Brock, J., Cragg, L., Einav, S., Griffiths, H., & Nation, K.

(2009). Eye-movement patterns are associated with communicative competence in autistic spectrum disorders. Journal Of Child Psychology And Psychiatry, 50(7), 834-842.

Nystrom, P. (2008). The infant mirror neuron system studied with high density eeg. Social Neuroscience, 3(3-4), 334-347.

O'Toole, A. J., Roark, D. A., & Abdi, H. (2002). Recognizing moving faces:

A psychological and neural synthesis. Trends In Cognitive Sciences, 6(6), 261-266.

Oberman, L. M., Hubbard, E. M., McCleery, J. P., Altschuler, E. L., Ramachandran, V. S., & Pineda, J. A. (2005). Eeg evidence for mirror neuron dysfunction in autism spectrum disorders. Cognitive Brain Research, 24(2), 190-198.

Onishi, K. H., & Bailargeon, R. (2005). Do 15-month-old infants understand false beliefs? Science, 308(5719), 255-258.

Passarotti, A. M., Smith, J., DeLano, M., & Huang, J. (2007).

Developmental differences in the neural bases of the face inversion effect show progressive tuning of face-selective regions to the upright orientation. Neuroimage, 34(4), 1708-1722.

Pazzaglia, M., Pizzamiglio, L., Pes, E., & Aglioti, S. M. (2008). The sound of actions in apraxia. Current Biology, 18(22), 1766-1772.

Pellicano, E., & Rhodes, G. (2003). Holistic processing of faces in preschool children and adults. Psychological Science, 14(6), 618-622.

Pfeifer, J. H., Iacoboni, M., Mazziotta, J. C., & Dapretto, M. (2008).

Mirroring others' emotions relates to empathy and interpersonal competence in children. Neuroimage, 39(4), 2076-2085.

Pierce, K., Muller, R. A., Ambrose, J., Allen, G., & Courchesne, E. (2001).

Face processing occurs outside the fusiform 'face area' in autism:

Evidence from functional mri. Brain, 124, 2059-2073.

Premack, D. (1990). The infants theory of self-propelled objects. Cognition, 36(1), 1-16.

Pulvermuller, F. (2005). Brain mechanisms linking language and action.

Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6(7), 576.

Pulvermuller, F., Hauk, O., Nikulin, V. V., & Ilmoniemi, R. J. (2005).

Functional links between motor and language systems. European Journal Of Neuroscience, 21(3), 793-797.

Rakover, S. S. (2002). Featural vs. Configurational information in faces: A conceptual and empirical analysis. British Journal Of Psychology, 93, 1-30.

Reithler, J., van Mier, H. I., Peters, J. C., & Goebel, R. (2007). Nonvisual motor learning influences abstract action observation. Current Biology, 17(14), 1201-1207.

Rizzolatti, G., & Arbib, M. A. (1998). Language within our grasp. Trends In Neurosciences, 21(5), 188-194.

Rizzolatti, G., & Craighero, L. (2004). The mirror-neuron system. Annual Review Of Neuroscience, 27, 169-192.

Rizzolatti, G., Fogassi, L., & Gallese, V. (2001). Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying the understanding and imitation of action.

Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2(9), 661-670.

Rizzolatti, G., & Sinigaglia, C. (2010). The functional role of the

Rizzolatti, G., & Sinigaglia, C. (2010). The functional role of the

Related documents