• No results found

Effect of daytime and age on the avoidance and approach behaviour of commercial Danish broiler chicken

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "Effect of daytime and age on the avoidance and approach behaviour of commercial Danish broiler chicken"

Copied!
31
0
0

Loading.... (view fulltext now)

Full text

(1)

Department of Physics, Chemistry and Biology

Master Thesis

Effect of daytime and age on the avoidance and

approach behaviour of commercial Danish

broiler chicken

Franziska Hakansson

LiTH-IFM- Ex--15/2999--SE

Supervisor: Per Jensen, Linköping University & Helle H. Kristensen, Copenhagen University Examiner: Jordi Altimiras, Linköping University

Department of Physics, Chemistry and Biology Linköpings universitet

(2)

Rapporttyp Report category Examensarbete D-uppsats Språk/Language Engelska/English Titel/Title:

Effect of daytime and age on the avoidance and approach behaviour of commercial Danish broiler chicken.

Författare/Author:

Franziska Hakansson

Sammanfattning/Abstract:

As activity levels of intensively managed broiler chicken vary over time, detailed knowledge on their influence is potentially useful to further adjust welfare assessment schemes. Therefore, this study investigated the influence of daytime and age on the performance of broiler chicken in two applied fear tests.

On-farm studies were carried out in 14 flocks of intensively managed broiler chicken. A forced and a voluntary approach test were conducted during morning and evening hours and at three different ages (1: 6-12d; 2: 21-24d; 3: prior to slaughter). At each observation, avoidance distances (AD) and the number of animals voluntarily approaching (VA) an observer were collected.

No significant correlation was found between daytime and AD or VA. When tested at different ages, daytime and VA were significantly correlated (1: r= 0.56*; 2: r= 0.40*; 3: r= 0.64*), but the results were not consistent. At three weeks the VA was found to be higher in the morning but at younger age and prior to slaughter, more birds approach an observer in the evening. Both, approach and avoidance peaked at three weeks and decreased prior to slaughter. Further, the effect of differing observer locations in the chicken house on the behavioural response of broiler chicken was studied. A higher proportion of birds voluntarily approached an unknown human in the area close to the main entrance door, but the avoidance behaviour was not affected.

From the results of this study I conclude, that the time of assessment has no effect on avoidance or approach behaviour of commercial slow-growing broiler chicken. However, fear related behaviour changed depending on age. This makes the applied tests potentially applicable independent of daytime restrictions when performed with birds of the same age.

ISBN

LITH-IFM-A-EX—15/2999—SE

__________________________________________________ ISRN

__________________________________________________ Serietitel och serienummer ISSN

Title of series, numbering

Handledare/Supervisor Per Jensen

Ort/Location: Linköping

Nyckelord/Keyword:

Welfare Quality®, welfare assessment, broiler chicken (Gallus gallus domesticus), fear (avoidance/ approach), human-animal-relationship, diurnality of behaviour, daytime dependence

Datum/Date

2015-05-30

URL för elektronisk version

Institutionen för fysik, kemi och biologi Department of Physics, Chemistry and Biology

(3)

CONTENT

List of abbreviations ... 2

1 Abstract ... 3

2 Introduction ... 4

3 Material & Methods ... 8

3.1 Animals and management ... 8

3.1.1 Experiment 1 ... 8

3.1.2 Experiment 2 ... 8

3.2 Tests ... 9

3.2.1 Avoidance distance test ... 9

3.2.2 Stationary person test ... 10

3.3 Experimental set-up ... 11 3.3.1 Experiment 1 ... 11 3.3.2 Experiment 2 ... 12 3.4 Data analyses ... 12 4 Results ... 14 4.1 Experiment 1 ... 14 4.2 Experiment 2 ... 16 5 Discussion... 19

5.1 Overall conclusion and perspective ... 22

5.2 Societal and ethical considerations... 23

6 Acknowledgement ... 24

(4)

2

List of abbreviations

HAR Human-animal-relationship

ADT Avoidance distance test

AD Avoidance distance

VA Voluntary approach

SPT Stationary person test

FoH Fear of humans

ToD Time of Day

SE Standard error

(5)

3

1 Abstract

As activity levels of intensively managed broiler chicken vary over time, detailed knowledge on their influence is potentially useful to further adjust welfare assessment schemes. Therefore, this study investigated the influence of daytime and age on the performance of broiler chicken in two applied fear tests.

On-farm studies were carried out in 14 flocks of intensively managed broiler chicken. A forced and a voluntary approach test were conducted during morning and evening hours and at three different ages (1: 6-12d; 2: 21-24d; 3: prior to slaughter). At each observation, avoidance distances (AD) and the number of animals voluntarily approaching (VA) an observer were collected.

No significant correlation was found between daytime and AD or VA. When tested at different ages, daytime and VA were significantly correlated (1: r= 0.56*; 2: r= 0.40*; 3: r= 0.64*), but the results were not consistent. At three weeks the VA was found to be higher in the morning but at younger age and prior to slaughter, more birds approach an observer in the evening. Both, approach and avoidance peaked at three weeks and decreased prior to slaughter. Further, the effect of differing observer locations in the chicken house on the behavioural response of broiler chicken was studied. A higher proportion of birds voluntarily approached an unknown human in the area close to the main entrance door, but the avoidance behaviour was not affected.

From the results of this study I conclude, that the time of assessment has no effect on avoidance or approach behaviour of commercial slow-growing broiler chicken. However, fear related behaviour changed depending on age. This makes the applied tests potentially applicable independent of daytime restrictions when performed with birds of the same age.

(6)

4

2 Introduction

On-farm animal welfare assessment has become more important in livestock production within the last years as it considers animal health, productivity and product quality of farm animals. This is not only because animal (welfare) scientists call for more regulations in animal production to prevent the animals from unnecessary suffering (Farm Animal Welfare Council, 1992) but also, because consumers are more concerned about food safety and quality as well as the environmental impact of intensively produced farm animals (Harper and Makatouni, 2002).

There are many definitions of the term ‘animal welfare’ which range from ‘a state as regards its attempt to cope with its environment’ (Broom, 1988) to a more animal based point in which an animal should be healthy and have ‘everything it needs’ (Dawkins, 2006). Today agreement exists on the statement that animal welfare is multidimensional (Botreau et al., 2007) and hence, considers not only the management system in which an individual lives and its physical health, but also its mental wellbeing.

Welfare related research has led to a five year EU project called the Welfare Quality®, which aimed at developing scientifically based methods to perform an overall welfare assessment of farm animals (Welfare Quality®, 2014). The collected information makes it possible to assign farms and slaughterhouses to one of four categories (poor to good animal welfare). This not only for the authorities to register farms with inadequate welfare, but also aims at helping individual farmers to improve their on-farm situation.

Measuring welfare of farm animals requires consideration of a variety of parameters and methods. The classical approach to use physiological parameters to assess welfare is considered more objective as they measure the real-time health status of an animal. Nevertheless, good welfare does not only include physical health of an individual but also the mental well-being (Dawkins, 2004). Next to physiological indices measures of behaviour play an important role in assessing welfare (Broom, 1991). Not only can behaviour be studied non-intrusively, it also gives an impression about the situation from an animal's perspective (Dawkins, 2006) as altering behaviour is used by animals to respond to environmental changes. Compared to prior approaches to assess animal welfare that mainly focused on the animal's available resources, the Welfare Quality® assessment (Barnett et al., 1992) system considers, next to that, more animal based measures.

(7)

5

Thus, body condition, health aspects, injuries and behaviour play a superior role in the assessment protocols, as all together gives information about the 'outcome of the interaction between the animal and its environment' (Welfare Quality®, 2009). The available protocols for poultry, pigs and cattle cover different aspects of animal welfare using four main principles: 'good health, good housing, good feeding and appropriate behaviour'. These principles are divided into 12 criteria, which are assessed with one or more measures (Welfare Quality®, 2009). To apply the protocol on-farm, assessors have to attend training sessions to learn how to use the protocol correctly.

Broiler chicken (Gallus gallus domesticus), birds kept for meat production, are the fastest growing farm species (Meluzzi and Sirri, 2009) and are therefore highly prone to various welfare problems. A strong consumer demand for chicken meat stimulated the growth of the broiler chicken industry within the last decades. Due to substantial breeding, selection for fast growth and intensive management broiler chicken face several problems such as severe health issues, decreased walking ability and limited possibilities to exhibit natural behaviours (Bessei, 2006; Meluzzi and Sirri, 2009). Intensively managed birds are restraint in barren environments with minimal environmental stimulation what decreases their ability to cope with altering situations (Jones and Hocking, 1999a). With the implementation of mechanically provision of food and other resources the daily interaction between broiler chicken and humans decreased. However, although intensive broiler farming is largely automated, contact between birds and humans exists (Zulkifli, 2013) as a stockperson normally investigates the animals and the equipment on daily control walks. Even though broiler chicken are domesticated for many generations and managing of commercial flocks mainly consists of daily routines, these might be fear evoking due to their intensity, duration and suddenness (Forkman et al., 2007; Jones, 1996). Consequently, sudden changes in their environment and exposure to humans are considered the most fearful events broiler chicken can experience (Jones, 1996), to which they react with freezing, escape or flight behaviour (Meluzzi and Sirri, 2009).

Fear is generally defined as an aversive, emotional state an individual might experiences due to the perception of actual danger (Forkman et al., 2007). This undesirable mental state motivates an individual to avoid harmful situations (Hemsworth et al., 1994; Rushen et al., 1999) and thus, protects it from injury and promotes its fitness.

(8)

6

However, an extreme fear response, such as panic, can lead to increased damage and higher mortality rates during management, catching and transport of broiler chicken. Prolonged periods of fearful events are further considered a major source of stress (Rushen et al., 1999) which can lead to the development of health problems and thus, reduce welfare and limited productivity (Hemsworth and Coleman, 2010; Hemsworth, 2003; Jones, 1996; Waiblinger et al., 2006).

The stockperson’s behaviour is one of the major variables that determine the human-animal-relationship (HAR) (Hemsworth and Coleman, 2010) and following, the level of fear broiler chicken might develop. A large numbers of studies examined the relationship between fear of humans, assessed due to behavioural observations, and the productivity of farm animals. Subsequent, a high level of fear has often been negatively correlated to productivity parameters of farm animals (Barnett et al., 1992; Breuer et al., 2000; Edwards et al., 2007; Hemsworth et al., 1989; Jones, 1996). In poultry, results of fear tests have shown to be negatively correlated with peak hen day production (Barnett et al., 1992), egg production and egg shell quality (Barnett et al., 1993) in laying hens and feed conversion (Hemsworth et al., 1994), first week mortality and meat quality (Cransberg et al., 2000) in broiler chicken. Consequently, as 'the quality of the HAR can have profound impact on the welfare and productivity of farm animals' (Zulkifli, 2013), a measure to investigate this relationship is included in the welfare assessment protocol for broiler chicken (Welfare Quality®, 2009). Nevertheless, the need to further develop and improve assessment methods, especially for fear, remains (Appleby et al., 2011).

It is known that broiler chickens activity varies over time, with a main peak at approximately 21 to 26 days of age (Nielsen et al., 2003), followed by a continuous decrease due to an increase in temperature, metabolic rate, and stocking density and consequently, a decrease in the walking ability (Newberry et al., 1988). The relationship between age and activity in broiler chicken has been reported elsewhere (Bessei, 1992; Newberry et al., 1988; Nielsen et al., 2003), however, information is rare about diurnal activity patterns that might influence the outcome of behavioural tests applied to intensively managed broiler chicken. In 1988, Newberry et al. reported that broiler chicken raised under experimental conditions displayed a clear diurnal rhythm with a peak activity in the morning (8-9am) and evening (4-6 pm) which was explained to be correlated to the stockpersons regular control walk.

(9)

7

This was also found by other studies reporting that broiler chickens activity follows a diurnal rhythm (Bessei, 1992; Nielsen et al., 2003). However, this was only true for birds that were feed with a restricted diet delivered twice a day (morning/ evening) but not for birds fed ad libitum. Finally, Schwean-Lardner et al. concluded in 2012, that intensive management conditions might prevent birds from maintaining a diurnal rhythm.

As not much is known about daytime dependent behaviour pattern of intensively managed broiler chicken, this study aimed at investigating the outcome of two applied fear tests in regard to time of day of assessment and age.

Thus, the approach and avoidance behaviour of commercial broiler chicken towards an unknown human was investigated at morning and evening observations as well as at three different ages. Further, the influence of the observer location in the chicken house on the outcome of the applied tests was observed. Finally, results of the fear tests were correlated to the applied lighting scheme of each farm and the respective first week mortality.

(10)

8

3 Material & Methods

Within this study, two experiments were performed. A first experiment tested, if the observer location in the chicken house or the time-of-day of observation affected the behavioural response of Dutch broiler chicken towards humans. This part of the study will be referred to ‘experiment one’. However, due to the outbreak of the bird-flue in Holland in the end of 2014, the age dependent behavioural response of Dutch broiler chicken could not be investigated.

Experiment two investigated the repeatability of approach and avoidance behaviour of Danish broiler chicken within days and at different times in the rearing cycle and additionally correlated the results to 1st week mortality and lighting.

3.1 Animals and management 3.1.1 Experiment 1

In a first experiment Dutch broiler chicken of the one-star 'Beter-Leven' label were tested. The mentioned label requires that every house is provided with a roofed outdoor run (winter-garden) that is attached to the chicken house, enrichment devices (here straw bales) and scattered whole wheat in the litter area. The birds, a slow growing strain (JA-757, Hubbard), are kept at a maximum stocking density of 25 kg/m² (commercial Danish broiler production: max. 42 kg/m²) and usually go to slaughter at the age of 55 to 57 days (commercial Dutch broiler production: around 42 days). Natural light is provided due to the implementation of windows and the attached roofed winter-garden in all chicken houses. A semi-natural lighting scheme is required with a continuous dark-period of at least eight hours. Two times a day a stockperson walks through each house to control the animals and the equipment.

3.1.2 Experiment 2

This experiment was performed with commercially housed Danish broiler chickens. Tests were performed with birds from different producers of the 'Majs Plus' label (Rose Poultry), located in Denmark. Broiler chicken reared under the 'Majs Plus' label have to be provided with a diet consisting of 50% maize, 20% more space compared to commercial Danish broiler production (maximum stocking density 32 kg/ m2) and enrichment devices (bales of straw, hay, silage). Additionally, a short transportation time of maximal 1.5 hours from farm to slaughter is implemented.

(11)

9

The birds, a fast growing strain (Ross 308-broilers), are usually slaughtered at 33 to 35 days of age. No natural light is provided to the birds and lighting schemes differed between producers.

Two times a day a stockperson enters the house for a control walk. All producers provided this study with data about the 1st week mortality of each flock.

3.2 Tests

In order to assess the bird's fear related behaviour towards humans an avoidance distance test (ADT) and a stationary person test (SPT) were conducted.

Both tests are classified as approach tests, with the difference that in the ADT an experimenter applies a forced approach while in the SPT the voluntary approach of the tested animals is investigated by observing the number of birds in a certain proximity to an unknown human. Both tests were described in detail for the use with laying hens (Raubek et al., 2007) and were experimentally validated for the use in commercial settings (Graml et al., 2008).

Nevertheless, no validation study has been performed for the use of the assessment methods in commercial broiler chicken flocks and to the best of my knowledge, there have not been any published studies on broiler chickens fear dependency on time of observation described in the literature. The tests were conducted by the same person who was prior to testing unfamiliar to the birds. The female experimenter was wearing the clothes and shoes provided by each farm.

3.2.1 Avoidance distance test

While walking through the chicken house, every 6th step the experimenter turned alternative to the left or to the right and approached a randomly selected close individual from a distance of 1.5 m. During the approach, one hand was held in front of the body and the other was hanging loose on the side. The experimenter approached with an approximate speed of one step per second, continuously bending in the knees and lowering the upper body until the selected individual withdrawed (lifting the 2nd feed from the ground) or was touched.

The distance from the experimenter's hand to the position of the broilers feet prior to withdrawal was estimated (AD). Results were rounded to the nearest round number, giving an estimation of the AD in 10 cm steps (AD-range: 0 to 150 cm, in 10 cm steps).

(12)

10

Randomness was performed by selecting the first animal that appeared in the experimenters view after turning. Birds that seemed seriously ill have not been approached, or if so, the approach was stopped and repeated with another individual. The test was repeated until 40 birds' were approached. If a bird withdraws due to another reason than the approach, the test is stopped and another bird is chosen to perform the test.

Figure 1. Avoidance distance test sequence; approach of a randomly selected individual from a distance of 1.5 m with one hand in front and the other hanging loose on the side of the body. While approaching, the experimenter continuously bends in the knees and lowers the upper body until the animal withdraws or can be touched.

3.2.2 Stationary person test

To assess the reaction of the birds to a stationary person the experimenter stopped in between the assessment walk and placed herself with the back towards the wall or the feeding line in order to remain motionless. Following, she filmed a defined area (80x 100 cm) in front of her feet, including the tips of her shoes, for two minutes. The video camera (GoPro Hero 3+, silver edition, continuous mode: 1 photo/10s) was fixed to the experimenters head via a head-mount. The test was performed four times per house at different locations. To measure the number of birds in range and their reaction towards a stationary person the videotapes were later analysed by scan sampling (two minutes, every 10 seconds) counting birds that are with at least 30 % of their body in the defined area.

(13)

11

Figure 2. Stationary person test procedure. Left: The experimenter stands motionless for a period of two minutes and films a defined range (80x 100 cm) in front of her. Right: Picture from video recording (scan sampling, every 10 seconds) showing the range and the animals to be counted (at least 30% of the body in the area).

3.3 Experimental set-up

The experimental setup consisted of a defined assessment walk through the house with both a stationary and an approach phases (as done in STROLL test: Cransberg et al., 2000). After entering the house, the experimenter walked down one side parallel to the drinking/ feeding equipment at a distance of 1 m, with a speed of approximately one step per second. An ADT was performed at every 6th to 8th step, depending on the size of the house (approach of a total number of 40 birds per assessment walk). Every 30th step a SPT was performed and repeated four times per house at different locations.

3.3.1 Experiment 1

Within this part, four flocks of three weeks old and two flocks of five weeks old mixed gender JA-757 broilers from one producer located in the south of Holland were tested. Tests were performed in the morning and evening hours and each time at different predefined locations in the house. Location one (I) and two (II) were in the front part of the house, with the former being close to the main entrance door and the latter being at the winter-garden side. Location three (III) and four (IV) were in the back of the house, with the former being at the winter-garden side and the latter at the wall side (Fig. 3).

(14)

12

Figure 3: Non-scaled figure of a Dutch chicken house, with the roofed winter-garden, the main entrance doors and feeder/ drinker lines. For experiment one, the chicken house was divided into four test locations (I: Entrance area/ close to the main door, II: Entrance area / close to the winter-garden, III: Back area/ close to the winter-garden, IV: Back area/ at the wall side) in which both, the ADT and the STP were performed.

3.3.2 Experiment 2

Nine flocks of female and male Ross 308-broilers from three producers were investigated (flock size average: 25.500). Each flock was tested twice a day, in the morning (before 11:00) and the evening (after 17:00). Additionally, each flock was investigated at three different ages which are a young (day 6-12), a middle age (day 20-24) and prior to slaughter (day 33- 35). Additionally, data about the lighting schedule (Producer 1: 6h of light divided into 2 phases am/ pm; Producer 2: 4h of light at night, continuously; Producer 3: 8h of light, continuously) and the first week mortality was obtained from each farmer.

3.4 Data analyses

Statistical analyses were done in SPSS version 22. A Kolmogorov- Smirnov test was used to test the data for normal distribution, giving that the data was not normally distributed.

For the avoidance distance test data is presented as medians plus interquartile range (due to non-normally distribution of the data, lower sensitivity to outliers). For the stationary person test results are given as means ± SE. Mann-Whitney-U test of independent samples was used to test the difference in the distribution of the individual data sets.

(15)

13

Spearman rank order correlation was used to investigate the effect of the observer location on avoidance and approach behaviour as well the correlation between results of the applied tests, lighting and first week mortality. The significance level for all tests was 0.05.

(16)

14

4 Results

4.1 Experiment 1

When testing the approach and avoidance behaviour in different observer locations in Dutch chicken houses, an influencing effect of the chosen location on the behaviour of Dutch broiler chicken was observed (Fig. 4). The observer location in the chicken house influenced the number of animals that voluntary approached an observer. Significantly more birds approached an observer in location one, which was the area close to the main entrance door, compared to location two (U= 6012, p< 0.001), three (U= 5720, p< 0.001) and four (U= 6004, p< 0.001). However, this could not be confirmed for the avoidance distance, which did not differ significantly between different observer locations.

Figure 4: Overall fear behaviour of Dutch broiler chicken at four different observer locations (I: Entrance area/ close to the main door, II: Entrance area / close to the winter-garden, III: Back area/ close to the winter-garden, IV: Back area/ at the wall side). Voluntary approach behaviour given as mean number of animals in defined range (error bars: 95% confidence interval) and avoidance behaviour given as median avoidance distance (box: IQR/ 25th to 75th percentile, error bars: smallest and largest non-extreme value). * indicates significance at the 5% level.

Results of the observations performed in the morning and the evening hours produced evidence for the influencing effect of time-of-day on the behavioural response of Dutch broiler chicken towards humans.

Both, approach and avoidance were found to be significantly correlated to time of day (AD: r= -0.21, p< 0.001; VA: r= 0.48, p< 0.001; Fig. 5).

(17)

15

The median avoidance distance was found to be longer in the morning (60cm, IQR= 45- 100 cm) than in the evening (50 cm, IQR= 30- 80 cm). The mean voluntary approach was found to be significantly higher in the evening (50.5 1.65) than in the morning (23.82 1.19). Following, at times were the birds displayed a longer avoidance distance, the approach behaviour towards an unknown human was low.

However, the statistical significant difference from the data set of the avoidance distance measured in the morning and in the evening is not considered biological relevant, as the distances were estimated and rounded to the next full number (in 10 cm steps).

Figure 5: Overall and age dependent approach and avoidance behaviour of Dutch broiler chicken assessed twice a day. Results are given as mean number of animals in defined range (error bars: 95% confidence interval) and avoidance behaviour given as median avoidance distance (box: IQR/ 25th to 75th percentile with median; error bars: smallest and largest non-extreme value). * indicates significance at the 5% level.

(18)

16 4.2 Experiment 2

The results of the applied tests produced by this study are listed in Table 2. For Danish broiler chicken assessed in this study, no correlation between daytime and the outcome of the ADT (r= 0.23, p= 0.35) or the SPT (r= 0.01, p= 0.56) was found.

TABLE 1. Overall and age dependent approach and avoidance behaviour of Danish broiler chicken assessed twice a day. Results are given as median (and interquartile range) for the avoidance distance and as mean (± SE) for the approach behaviour. Non-significant results are labelled ‘ns’.

Time of day

Applied fear test Age Morning Evening p

Median avoidance distance [cm] Overall 40 (20- 60) 40 (20- 70) ns 1 2 3 50 (30- 70) 50 (20- 70) 30 (10- 50) 40 (30- 60) 50 (20- 80) 40 (10- 60) ns ns ns Mean number of birds

in range Overall 10.3 ± 0.4 9.5 ± 0.3 ns 1 2 3 4.7 ± 0.4 15.5 ± 0.7 10.1 ± 0.4 4.9 ± 0.3 12.5 ± 0.5 11.9 ± 0.4 0.03 0.01 0.00

Overall, the birds displayed the same median avoidance distance towards an unknown observer in the morning as in the evening. When tested at different ages in the rearing cycle, no differences between the median avoidance distances of morning and evening observations was found. When investigating data from different age classes of broiler chicken, no differences in the distribution of the distances were found. However, a lower avoidance distance was displayed by the broiler chicken prior to slaughter, as expected.

(19)

17

The mean number of birds voluntarily approaching an observer differed not between morning and evening testing. Nevertheless, significant differences between morning and evening observations were found when looking at the different age classes. However, the results are not consistent. Significantly more birds approached an observer in the evening at a young age and prior to slaughter but at a middle age, voluntary approach was higher in the morning than in the evening.

As only a low number of animals approached at all ages in a total of four trails per house the obtained statistical significant differences are not considered biological relevant.

Figure 6: Age dependent approach and avoidance behaviour of Dutch broiler chicken assessed twice a day. Results are given as mean number of animals in defined range (error bars: 95% confidence interval) for the voluntary approach and avoidance behaviour was given as median avoidance distance (box: IQR/ 25th to 75th percentile with median; error bars: smallest and largest non-extreme value; dots: extreme outliers). * indicates significance at the 5% level.

Data about the lighting schedule and first week mortality was obtained from each farm and was investigated in regard to fear related behaviour of broiler chicken towards humans.

No correlation between first week mortality and avoidance distance was found (r= 0.04, p= 0.11). However, first week mortality was significantly correlated to the voluntary approach of the birds, but only at the middle age (r= -0.17, p< 0.001) and prior to slaughter (r= -0.17, p< 0.001). The avoidance distance displayed by broiler chicken of different producers was not correlated to their applied lighting scheme (r= 0.001, p= 0.8) but on the contrary, the voluntary approach was (r= -0.4, p< 0.001).

(20)

18

Consequently, at farms with a lower amount of darkness hours provided to the birds a lower number of approaches toward an unknown human were observed.

(21)

19

5 Discussion

In 2014, approximately 110 million broiler chicken were produced by about 185 Danish producers (Landbrug og Fødevarer, 2015). Fear, social stress and injurious pecking are the main behavioural problems the modern broiler industry is facing (Jones and Hocking, 1999b). Increased and permanent fear is considered an undesirable state of suffering (Jones and Waddington, 1992) and moreover, fear of humans is regarded a serious welfare issue (De Jong et al., 2012). Already in 1965, the so called Brambell Committee (Farm Animal Welfare Council, 1992) proposed in their ‘Five Freedoms’ that intensively-housed livestock should be free from fear (Brambell, 1965). Although fear protects an animal from harmful situations and promotes its fitness through motivating it to avoid potentially harmful situations (Hemsworth et al., 1994; Rushen et al., 1999), intense and persistent fear is known to be a major source of stress (Rushen et al., 1999) leading to health problems and limited productivity (Jones, 1996). Even though farm animals have been domesticated for a long time, humans and sudden changes in their environment are still the most frightening events they might encounter (Boissy, 1995; Jones, 1996). The stockpersons behaviour is a major variable in determining fear in livestock (Hemsworth and Coleman, 2010) and thus, the assessment of the human-animal-relationship is included in welfare assessment protocols. However, not much is known about influencing effects, such as diurnal and age dependent behaviour patterns or location dependent habituation, on the behaviour and performance of broiler chicken in fear assessment tests. Therefore, this study investigated the influence of daytime, age and the observer location on avoidance and approach behaviour of intensively managed broiler chicken.

A first experiment, conducted with Dutch broiler chicken of the ‘Beter-Leven’ label, produced evidence for the influencing effect of the observer location in the chicken house on the voluntary approach behaviour, but not on the avoidance of birds towards an unknown human. This might be because the applied tests investigate different perceptions of the birds towards humans. Forceful approach, as performed in the avoidance distance test, gives information about the fear of humans the animals might experience. In contrary, the stationary person test measures the approach the birds do voluntarily towards a human and thus, their underlying motivation to perform an action.

(22)

20

When testing the single observer locations against each other, more birds approach an observer in the location close to the main entrance door. As human pass by this location more often to enter or exit the house, the birds in this location might be more habituated towards humans. However, the avoidance distance was not different in this location. This might be because all birds in a flock might have the same overall level of fear of humans but due to habituation and regular visual contact (Zulkifli et al., 2002), the birds close to the entrance might be less fearful and thus, more motivated to approach a human than birds in the back of the house.

When analysing the effect of the daytime of assessment on the fear related behaviour responses towards humans, the two experiments produced different results. In the Dutch system, a significant higher avoidance of an observer has been seen at morning observations, while at the same time voluntary approach towards a stationary person was found to be lower compared to evening observations. This follows the suggestion that when the level of fear is high birds will avoid an unknown person by displaying a higher avoidance distance and simultaneously show less voluntary approach.

However, this relationship could not been confirmed in experiment two, when testing broiler chicken reared under Danish conditions. No correlations were found between daytime of assessment and approach or avoidance behaviour of Danish broiler chicken reared after the ‘Majs-Plus label. Although significant differences between morning and evening observations were found for the approach behaviour, these were not constant as at three weeks of age more animals approached an observer in the morning while prior to slaughter approach was higher in the evening. When comparing the results of the two experiments, this study produced some evidence for a diurnality of fear related behaviour patterns. However, the existence of these patterns might be depending on the management system and especially, the lighting regime (Nielsen et al., 2003), as light is a major factor regarding broiler chickens performance and activity (Newberry et al., 1988). Many regulatory systems within a bird are affected by light (Kristensen et al., 2007) and hence, the applied lighting scheme may have an impact on the bird’s behaviour. While the Dutch system uses a semi-natural lighting scheme due to the implementation of windows and eight hours of continuous darkness at night, the systems in Denmark is fully artificial. The chicken houses do not have windows and darkness is provided four to maximum eight hours per day, sometimes divided into two periods, depending on the producer.

(23)

21

In this study the avoidance distance was not correlated to the applied lighting schemes, but the voluntary approach was. Consequently, at farms with a lower amount of darkness hours provided to the birds, lower numbers of approaches towards an unknown human were observed. It is known that long day-lengths not only disturb diurnal patterns of broiler chicken (Sanotra et al., 2002) but also prevent them from establishing a diurnal rhythm (Schwean-Lardner et al., 2012). As Alvino et al. suggested in 2009, this might be due to the lack of light differences between day and night which prevents the birds from synchronizing diurnal rhythms. Longer day-lengths do not only negatively affect the sleep patterns of broiler chicken (Blokhuis, 1983) but also feeding, and are thus considered a welfare concern (De Jong et al., 2012). Additionally, Bayram and Özkan found in 2010 that birds reared with longer days are more fearful which goes along with earlier findings demonstrating that shorter days (due to longer dark-periods) can reduce fearfulness (Sanotra et al., 2002).

Generally, Danish birds showed an overall lower avoidance but also a lower number of voluntary approaches towards an unknown human compared to Dutch birds. The overall voluntary approach of Dutch animals towards an observer was double to five times higher in morning and evening observations respectively, compared to Danish birds. This might be due to strain differences, with the Dutch broilers being a slow growing and the Danish birds a fast growing strain. It is well-known that slow growing strains are overall more active than fast-growing strains (Nielsen et al., 2003) because of a better walking ability and increased health, but also because of a slower increasing stocking density.

When investigating the fear related behaviour displayed by Danish broiler chicken at different ages, a clear pattern was found. Avoidance behaviour was low at one week of age, peaked at three weeks of age, which was found to be the age of main activity in poultry (Cornetto and Estevez, 2001), and decreased to a minimum prior to slaughter. This was also true for the results of the stationary person test, although prior to slaughter voluntary approach declined only little and was lowest at one week of age. The lower level of fear displayed by Danish broiler chicken prior to slaughter may be explained by a reduced mobility due to an increased stocking density at that age. Thus, avoidance and approach are correlated to the age of the birds as they get more immobile at the end of the rearing cycle (Bassler et al., 2013).

However, an appreciable number of birds voluntary approached an observer prior to slaughter.

(24)

22

This is in accordance to results from Bokkers and Koene (2003), who found that the ability to perform various activities but not the motivation of fast growing broilers decreased with age because of an increased live weight. Further, the different tests applied are ‘not alternative ways of measuring fear, but rather measures of different aspects of the relationship between people and animals’ (de Passillé and Rushen, 2005). Hence, the avoidance distance test gives an overview about the general fear of the birds towards humans as the approach is forceful and independent from the motivation of the animals (Forkman and Keeling, 2009). In contrary, the stationary person test measures the voluntary approach, depending on feelings, motivation and emotions, hence, the reaction will not be as strong as when performing an ADT (Waiblinger et al., 2006).

5.1 Overall conclusion and perspective

From the results of this study I conclude, that the time of assessment has no effect on avoidance or approach behaviour of commercial broiler chicken, that are managed under artificial light conditions. Nevertheless, lighting and enrichment practices might have a significant effect on the level of fear broiler chicken experience and display.

In both studies, broiler chickens avoidance and approach behaviour was only assessed two times a day, in the morning and evening hours. For a better understanding of daytime depending behaviour, both tests should be performed more often. As activity and behaviour of chicken changes depending on darkness periods and feeding time, it might be worth investigating fear related behaviour prior to and after darkness hours.

Under intensive management conditions, the applied tests appear to be potentially applicable independent of daytime restrictions. However, age and the observer location in the chicken house can possible influence the outcome of applied fear assessments. Hence, data collection spots chosen should always be evenly distributed in the whole house and tests applied to birds of different age should not be compared.

(25)

23 5.2 Societal and ethical considerations

The welfare of intensively managed animals is of great importance in modern society, and to improve welfare of farm animals, the behaviour must first be studied in every way possible. This study provided information about restrictions and influencing effects that may alter the performance of broiler chicken in applied fear assessment tests performed after the Welfare Quality® protocol (Welfare Quality®, 2009). Identification of those effects may enable further adjustments of such protocols to guarantee the accuracy of the applied assessment on-farm. This study was performed with the consent of each farmer. The observer never visited a farm alone and was most of the time under supervision of a stockperson. As the observed birds were kept for commercial use the observer followed the regulations regarding bio-security and the rules of each farm precisely. This study was an ethological approach and thus, there was no need to touch or pick up birds for closer observation. Additionally, the performed tests were conducted in a way similar to the daily routine walks of the stockperson and by this, only induced a low level of stress to the animals.

(26)

24

6 Acknowledgement

Hereby, I would like to thank Helle Halkjær Kristensen and Per Jensen for the excellent supervision. Helle, thanks for giving me so much input to start a project on my own.

Furthermore, I would like to thank Marlene K. Kirchner for helping me to step up for my career in always challenging and pushing me a bit further. Thanks to Benita H. Poulsen for the nice company in endless hours of driving through Denmark.

Finally, I would like to thank all the farmers that allowed me to collect data on their farms.

(27)

25

7 References

Alvino, G.M., Archer, G.S., Mench, J.A., 2009. Behavioural time budgets of broiler chickens reared in varying light intensities. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 118, 54-61.

Appleby, M.C., Hughes, B.O., Mench, J.A., Olsson, A., 2011. Animal Welfare.

Barnett, J.L., Hemsworth, P.H., Newman, E.A., 1992. Fear of humans and its relationships with productivity in laying hens at commercial farms. Br. Poult. Sci. 33, 699-710 doi: 10.1080/00071669208417510.

Barnett, J.L., Hemsworth, P.H., Jones, R.B., 1993. Behavioural responses of commercially farmed laying hens to humans: evidence of stimulus generalization. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 37, 139-146 doi:

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0168-1591(93)90106-Y.

Bassler, A.W., Arnould, C., Butterworth, A., Colin, L., De Jong, I.C., Ferrante, V., Ferrari, P., Haslam, S., Wemelsfelder, F., Blokhuis, H.J., 2013. Potential risk factors associated with contact dermatitis,

lameness, negative emotional state, and fear of humans in broiler chicken flocks. Poult. Sci. 92, 2811-2826 doi: 10.3382/ps.2013-03208 [doi].

Bayram, A., Özkan, S., 2010. Effects of a 16-hour light, 8-hour dark lighting schedule on behavioral traits and performance in male broiler chickens. The Journal of Applied Poultry Research 19, 263-273. Bessei, W., 2006. Welfare of broilers: a review. Worlds Poult. Sci. J. 62,

455-466.

Bessei, W., 1992. The behaviour of broilers under intensive management conditions. Archiv fuer Gefluegelkunde (Germany, FR).

Blokhuis, H., 1983. The relevance of sleep in poultry. Worlds Poult. Sci. J. 39, 33-37.

Bokkers, E.A., Koene, P., 2003. Behaviour of fast-and slow growing broilers to 12 weeks of age and the physical consequences. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 81, 59-72.

(28)

26

Botreau, R., Veissier, I., Butterworth, A., Bracke, M., Keeling, L., 2007. Definition of criteria for overall assessment of animal welfare.

ANIMAL WELFARE-POTTERS BAR THEN WHEATHAMPSTEAD- 16, 225.

Brambell, F.W.R., 1965. Report of the Technical Committee to Enquire into the Welfare of Animals Kept Under Intensive Livestock

Husbandry Systems: Presented to Parliament by the Secretary of State for Scotland and the Minister of Agriculture, Fisheries and Food by Command of Her Majesty December, 1965. HM Stationery Office. Breuer, K., Hemsworth, P.H., Barnett, J.L., Matthews, L.R., Coleman, G.J.,

2000. Behavioural response to humans and the productivity of commercial dairy cows. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 66, 273-288 doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0168-1591 (99)00097-0.

Broom, D.M., 1991. Animal welfare: concepts and measurement. J. Anim. Sci. 69, 4167-4175.

Broom, D.M., 1988. The scientific assessment of animal welfare. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 20, 5-19 doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0168-1591(88)90122-0.

Cornetto, T., Estevez, I., 2001. Influence of vertical panels on use of space by domestic fowl. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 71, 141-153.

Cransberg, P.H., Hemsworth, P.H., Coleman, G.J., 2000. Human factors affecting the behaviour and productivity of commercial broiler chickens. Br. Poult. Sci. 41, 272-279 doi: 10.1080/713654939.

Dawkins, M., 2004. Using behaviour to assess animal welfare. ANIMAL WELFARE-POTTERS BAR THEN WHEATHAMPSTEAD- 13, S3-S8.

Dawkins, M.S., 2006. A user's guide to animal welfare science. Trends in Ecology & Evolution 21, 77-82 doi:

http://dx.doi.org.e.bibl.liu.se/10.1016/j.tree.2005.10.017.

De Jong, I., Berg, C., Butterworth, A., Estevéz, I., 2012. Scientific Report Updating the EFSA Opinions on the Welfare of Broilers and Broiler Breeders. Supporting Publications.

(29)

27

De Passillé, A.M., Rushen, J., 2005. Can we measure human–animal interactions in on-farm animal welfare assessment?: Some unresolved issues. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 92, 193-209.

Edwards, L., Hemsworth, P., Coleman, G., 2007. Human contact and fear responses in laying hens. 19, 33-36.

Farm Animal Welfare Council, 1992. Five freedoms. Vet. Rec. 131, 357. Forkman, B., Boissy, A., Meunier-Salaün, M., Canali, E., Jones, R., 2007.

A critical review of fear tests used on cattle, pigs, sheep, poultry and horses. Physiol. Behav. 92, 340-374.

Forkman, B., Keeling, L., 2009. Assessment of Animal Welfare Measures for Layers and Broilers. School of City and Regional Planning, Cardiff University, Cardiff.

Graml, C., Niebuhr, K., Waiblinger, S., 2008. Reaction of laying hens to humans in the home or a novel environment. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 113, 98-109 doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2007.10.004. Harper, G.C., Makatouni, A., 2002. Consumer perception of organic food

production and farm animal welfare. Br. Food J. 104, 287-299. Hemsworth, P.H., Coleman, G.J., 2010. Human-Livestock Interactions:

The Stockperson and the Productivity of Intensively Farmed Animals. CABI.

Hemsworth, P., Coleman, G., Barnett, J., Jones, R., 1994. Behavioural responses to humans and the productivity of commercial broiler chickens. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 41, 101-114.

Hemsworth, P.H., 2003. Human–animal interactions in livestock production. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 81, 185-198 doi:

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0168-1591(02)00280-0.

Hemsworth, P.H., Barnett, J.L., Coleman, G.J., Hansen, C., 1989. A study of the relationships between the attitudinal and behavioural profiles of stockpersons and the level of fear of humans and reproductive

performance of commercial pigs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 23, 301-314 doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0168-1591(89)90099-3.

(30)

28

Jones, R.B., Waddington, D., 1992. Modification of fear in domestic chicks, Gallus gallus domesticus, via regular handling and early environmental enrichment. Anim. Behav. 43, 1021-1033.

Jones, R., Hocking, P., 1999a. Genetic selection for poultry behaviour: big bad wolf or friend in need? ANIMAL WELFARE-POTTERS BAR- 8, 343-360.

Jones, R.B., Hocking, P.M., 1999b. Genetic selection for poultry

behaviour: Big bad wolf or friend in need? Anim. Welfare 8, 343-359. Jones, R.B., 1996. Fear and adaptability in poultry: insights, implications

and imperatives. Worlds Poult. Sci. J. 52, 131-174 doi: 10.1079/WPS19960013.

Kristensen, H.H., Prescott, N.B., Perry, G.C., Ladewig, J., Ersbøll, A.K., Overvad, K.C., Wathes, C.M., 2007. The behaviour of broiler chickens in different light sources and illuminances. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 103, 75-89 doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2006.04.017. Landbrug og Fødevarer, 2015. Erhvervsfjerkræsektionens

årsmøde, Aarsstatistikker/Fjerkrae/2013-2014. doi: http://www.lf.dk/. Meluzzi, A., Sirri, F., 2009. Welfare of broiler chickens. Italian Journal of

Animal Science 8, 161-173.

Newberry, R.C., Hunt, J.R., Gardiner, E.E., 1988. Influence of light intensity on behavior and performance of broiler chickens. Poult. Sci. 67, 1020-1025.

Nielsen, B.L., Litherland, M., Nøddegaard, F., 2003. Effects of qualitative and quantitative feed restriction on the activity of broiler chickens. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 83, 309-323 doi:

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0168-1591(03)00137-0.

Raubek, J., Niebuhr, K., Waiblinger, S., 2007. Development of on-farm methods to assess the animal-human relationship in laying hens kept in non-cage systems. Anim. Welfare 16, 173-175.

Rushen, J., Taylor, A.A., de Passillé, A.M., 1999. Domestic animals' fear of humans and its effect on their welfare. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 65, 285-303 doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0168-1591(99)00089-1.

(31)

29

Sanotra, G.S., Lund, J.D., Vestergaard, K.S., 2002. Influence of light-dark schedules and stocking density on behaviour, risk of leg problems and occurrence of chronic fear in broilers. Br. Poult. Sci. 43, 344-354 doi: 10.1080/000716601201036023611.

Schwean-Lardner, K., Fancher, B.I., Classen, H.L., 2012. Impact of daylength on behavioural output in commercial broilers. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 137, 43-52 doi:

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2012.01.015.

Waiblinger, S., Boivin, X., Pedersen, V., Tosi, M., Janczak, A.M., Visser, E.K., Jones, R.B., 2006. Assessing the human–animal relationship in farmed species: a critical review. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 101, 185-242.

Welfare Quality®, 2014. Welfare Quality network. 2015 doi: http://www.welfarequality.net/network.

Welfare Quality®, 2009. Assessment Protocol for Poultry (Broiler, Laying Hens). Welfare Quality® Consortium, Lelystad, Netherlands.

Zulkifli, I., 2013. Review of human-animal interactions and their impact on animal productivity and welfare. J. Anim. Sci. Biotechnol. 4, 25-1891-4-25 doi: 10.1186/2049-1891-25-1891-4-25 [doi].

Zulkifli, I., Gilbert, J., Liew, P.K., Ginsos, J., 2002. The effects of regular visual contact with human beings on fear, stress, antibody and growth responses in broiler chickens. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 79, 103-112 doi: http://dx.doi.org.lt.ltag.bibl.liu.se/10.1016/S0168-1591(02)00135-1.

References

Related documents

Stöden omfattar statliga lån och kreditgarantier; anstånd med skatter och avgifter; tillfälligt sänkta arbetsgivaravgifter under pandemins första fas; ökat statligt ansvar

46 Konkreta exempel skulle kunna vara främjandeinsatser för affärsänglar/affärsängelnätverk, skapa arenor där aktörer från utbuds- och efterfrågesidan kan mötas eller

För att uppskatta den totala effekten av reformerna måste dock hänsyn tas till såväl samt- liga priseffekter som sammansättningseffekter, till följd av ökad försäljningsandel

The increasing availability of data and attention to services has increased the understanding of the contribution of services to innovation and productivity in

Generella styrmedel kan ha varit mindre verksamma än man har trott De generella styrmedlen, till skillnad från de specifika styrmedlen, har kommit att användas i större

Samtidigt som man redan idag skickar mindre försändelser direkt till kund skulle även denna verksamhet kunna behållas för att täcka in leveranser som

Industrial Emissions Directive, supplemented by horizontal legislation (e.g., Framework Directives on Waste and Water, Emissions Trading System, etc) and guidance on operating

Re-examination of the actual 2 ♀♀ (ZML) revealed that they are Andrena labialis (det.. Andrena jacobi Perkins: Paxton &amp; al. -Species synonymy- Schwarz &amp; al. scotica while