• No results found

Technology, expertise, and social cognition in human evolution

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "Technology, expertise, and social cognition in human evolution"

Copied!
11
0
0

Loading.... (view fulltext now)

Full text

(1)

Technology, expertise and social cognition in human

evolution

Dietrich Stout,1Richard Passingham,2Christopher Frith,3Jan Apel4and Thierry Chaminade5 1

Department of Anthropology, Emory University, 1557 Dickey Drive, Atlanta, GA 30322, USA

2

Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK

3

Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging, University College London, London, UK

4Department of Archaeology and Osteology, Gotland University College, Visby, Sweden 5

Mediterranean Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, CNRS – Universite´ Aix-Marseille, Marseille Cedex, France Keywords: cognitive processes, functional MRI, gesture, imitation, learning comprehension

Abstract

Paleolithic stone tools provide concrete evidence of major developments in human behavioural and cognitive evolution. Of particular interest are evolving cognitive mechanisms implied by the cultural transmission of increasingly complex prehistoric technologies, hypothetically including motor resonance, causal reasoning and mentalizing. To test the relevance of these mechanisms to specific Paleolithic technologies, we conducted a functional magnetic resonance imaging study of Naı¨ve, Trained and Expert subjects observing two toolmaking methods of differing complexity and antiquity: the simple ‘Oldowan’ method documented by the earliest tools 2.5 million years ago; and the more complex ‘Acheulean’ method used to produce refined tools 0.5 million years ago. Subjects observed 20-s video clips of an expert demonstrator, followed by behavioural tasks designed to maintain attention. Results show that observational understanding of Acheulean toolmaking involves increased demands for the recognition of abstract technological intentions. Across subject groups, Acheulean compared with Oldowan toolmaking was associated with activation of left anterior intraparietal and inferior frontal sulci, indicating the relevance of resonance mechanisms. Between groups, Naı¨ve subjects relied on bottom-up kinematic simulation in the premotor cortex to reconstruct unfamiliar intentions, and Experts employed a combination of familiarity-based sensorimotor matching in the posterior parietal cortex and top-down mentalizing involving the medial prefrontal cortex. While no specific differences between toolmaking technologies were found for Trained subjects, both produced frontal activation relative to Control, suggesting focused engagement with toolmaking stimuli. These findings support motor resonance hypotheses for the evolutionary origins of human social cognition and cumulative culture, directly linking these hypotheses with archaeologically observable behaviours in prehistory.

Introduction

Neither toolmaking (Beck, 1980) nor cultural transmission (Whiten et al., 2007) is unique to humans. Yet there is a vast gulf between the accumulated (Tennie et al., 2009) complexity of human technology and that of any other living species. This disparity has been attributed to uniquely human physical (Johnson-Frey, 2003) or social (Tomasello et al., 2005) cognition, or both (Passingham, 2008).

Motor hypotheses of action understanding (Gallese & Goldman, 1998; Blakemore & Decety, 2001) suggest a possible unification of these explanations. The ‘Motor Cognition Hypothesis’ (Gallese et al., 2009) proposes that human social cognition has its phylogenetic and ontogenetic origins in ‘motor resonance’. Distinctive human capacities for technology, language and intersubjectivity might thus have a single origin in evolutionary modifications of a primate ‘mirror neuron system’ (Rizzolatti & Craighero, 2004). However, it is not clear to what extent action understanding relies on motor resonance as

opposed to intention reading (Saxe, 2005; Grafton, 2009), nor at which level(s) action representations are shared between individuals (Jacob & Jeannerod, 2005; de Vignemont & Haggard, 2008).

To address these issues, we conducted a neuroimaging study in which human subjects observed the making of Paleolithic stone tools. Stone toolmaking is the earliest known uniquely human behaviour (Roux & Bril, 2005), dating back at least 2.6 million years (Semaw et al., 2003). Previous research (Stout & Chaminade, 2007) used FDG-positron emission tomography (PET) to study brain activation during stone toolmaking. In the earliest, ‘Oldowan’, technology a ‘hammerstone’ held in the dominant hand is used to strike sharp ‘flakes’ from a cobble ‘core’ manipulated by the other hand. We found this method to be associated with activation of parietal and frontal brain regions involved in sensorimotor coordination, grip selection and 3D shape perception. After that, 1.7 million years ago, more complex ‘Acheulean’ technology developed. Here cores were inten-tionally shaped into large cutting tools known as ‘handaxes’. We found this method to be associated with activation of the right inferior frontal gyrus (Stout et al., 2008), a region implicated in the hierarchical organization of action (Koechlin & Jubault, 2006).

Correspondence: Dr D. Stout, as above. E-mail: dwstout@emory.edu

Received 22 April 2010, revised 15 December 2010, accepted 23 December 2010

European Journal of Neuroscience, pp. 1–11, 2011 doi:10.1111/j.1460-9568.2011.07619.x

(2)

In the present study we used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to compare brain activation during the observation of Oldowan and Acheulean toolmaking. The Motor Cognition Hypothesis pro-poses that action understanding is tied to motor expertise (Gallese et al., 2009), but learning clearly requires understanding of actions not yet in the observer’s repertoire. Our design crossed observer expertise (Naı¨ve, Trained, Expert) with technological sophistication (Oldowan, Acheulean) to examine the contribution of resonance and interpreta-tion in understanding acinterpreta-tions of varying familiarity and complexity. An account in terms of motor resonance predicts expertise effects in the putative human mirror neuron system (Rizzolatti & Craighero, 2004) and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (Buccino et al., 2004; Vogt et al., 2007), regardless of complexity. An inferential account (Saxe, 2005) predicts complexity effects in brain regions associated with mental state attribution, including the medial prefrontal cortex (Frith & Frith, 2006). A mixed model (Grafton, 2009) makes less exclusive predictions, but might involve a shift from resonance to inference with increasing complexity and expertise.

Methods

Paleolithic toolmaking

The Paleolithic technologies investigated here are the same that were addressed in previous FDG-PET studies of subjects actually making stone tools (Stout & Chaminade, 2007; Stout et al., 2008). Oldowan flaking, known from approximately 2.6–1.6 million years ago, is a simple process of striking sharp cutting flakes from a stone core using direct percussion. However, even this simple technology requires substantial visuomotor coordination, including visual evaluation of core morphology (e.g. edge angles, location of convexities and concavities) in order to select appropriate targets for percussion, as well as active proprioceptive sensation and precise bimanual coordi-nation to guide forceful blows to small targets on the core.

After approximately 1.7 million years ago, flake-based Oldowan technology began to be replaced by ‘Acheulean’ technology, involving the intentional shaping of cores into large cutting tools known as ‘picks’, ‘handaxes’ and ‘cleavers’. Such shaping requires greater perceptual-motor skill to precisely control stone fracture patterns and more complex action plans that relate individual flake removals to each other in pursuit of a distal goal. By 500 000 years ago, some Acheulean tools exhibit a high level of refinement that additionally requires the careful preparation of edges and surfaces, known as ‘platform preparation’, before flake removals. Platform preparation is often done on the face opposite a planned flake removal: the core is flipped over (‘inverted’) and a new hammerstone and⁄ or hammerstone grip is selected and used to abrade⁄ micro-flake the edge through light, tangential blows. This preparatory operation introduces a new sub-routine to toolmaking action plans, increasing their hierarchical depth. It is the ‘Late Acheulean’ method that is studied here.

As in previous FDG-PET studies, the current study also includes a control condition that consists of simple bimanual percussion of an unmodified core without any attempt to detach flakes. This condition is designed to include general demands of striking and manipulating a core, while omitting any more specific demands for percussive accuracy, core support, target selection and strategic planning involved in actual toolmaking.

Subjects and training

Three subject groups were included in the study, comprising technologically Naı¨ve (n = 11), Trained (n = 10) and Expert (n = 5)

individuals. All subjects were right-handed by self-report and had no history of neurological illness. The study was approved by the National Hospital for Neurology and Neurosurgery and the Institute of Neurology joint Ethics Committee. Twenty-one individuals with no prior experience of stone toolmaking were recruited via advertise-ments posted to electronic mailing lists maintained by the University College London Functional Imaging Lab and Institute of Archaeology. Respondents chose to participate in the Naı¨ve or Trained group. Individuals who elected training attended 16 1-h training sessions over an 8-week period, in groups of 2–3 subjects per session. During training, subjects were provided with tools and raw materials for practice, as well as demonstrations and interactive verbal and gestural instruction by the first author. Subjects improved with training, but none achieved expertise in shaping handaxes (Supporting Information Fig. S1). Products of the 1st, 8th and 16th sessions of each subject were collected for further analysis (forthcoming). Because it became clear during training that subjects would not achieve the desired expertise, a group of five subjects with pre-existing expertise was included in the ongoing study. Expert subjects were drawn from the small extant community of academic and craft stone toolmakers, and were contacted directly. Imaging sessions for Naive, Trained and Expert subjects were interspersed over the course of the study.

Subjects in all groups received the same instructions before scanning, consisting of a scripted briefing, accompanying PowerPoint presentation, and Cogent script showing instructions and exemplar stimuli (not used in experiment) as presented in the scanner. Crucially, instructions included a description of the methods and aims of Paleolithic stone toolmaking so that even Naı¨ve subjects had basic conceptual knowledge of the technology.

Stimuli

Twenty-second video clips (Supporting Information Video S1) were extracted from full-length videos of an expert toolmaker (right-handed) engaged in Oldowan flaking (n = 6), Acheulean shaping (n = 6) and the Control condition (n = 6). All videos were recorded on the same day with constant camera position and lighting. The demonstrator was seated facing the camera, and supported the core on his left thigh or above his lap in his left hand. The field of view included this workspace and the full range of arm movements, but did not extend to the face. Flint from a single quarry in Suffolk, UK was used for all toolmaking, and video segments were deliberately selected from early stages of flaking⁄ shaping (e.g. prior to establishment of symmetrical ‘handaxe’ shape) so that size, shape, colour and other large-scale visual characteristics of cores did not differ systematically across stimulus types. Nevertheless, action sequences portrayed in the clips clearly reflected technological differences.

Nine types of technological action were identified in the videos, and their frequencies in the actual stimuli used recorded using the EthoLog 2.2.5 behavioural transcription tool (Table 1). These are: (i) percussive strikes with the right hand; (ii) shifts of the left-hand core grip; (iii) rotations of the core in the left hand; (iv) shifts of the right-hand hammerstone grip; (v) inversions (flipping over) of the core with the left hand; (vi) changing of the hammerstone (here the demonstrator reached off camera to exchange one hammerstone for another, see Supporting Information Video S1); (vii) abrasion⁄ micro-flaking of core edges with right hand; (viii) sweeping of detached flakes and fragments off the thigh with the right hand (the hammerstone itself or an extended finger may be used); (ix) grasping of a detached flake or fragment with the right hand to remove it from the thigh, usually with a side-to-side ‘scissor’ grip of index and middle

(3)

fingers, rarely (twice) with a pad-to-pad ‘pincer’ grip of thumb and index finger (Supporting Information Video S1). Importantly, the total number of actions declines from Control to Oldowan to Acheulean stimuli. This is also true for right- and left-hand actions considered separately. Thus, increasing activation across conditions must be explained in terms of the increasing diversity and causal⁄ intentional complexity of actions rather than their simple quantity.

There are four action types that are substantially more numerous in, or unique to, Acheulean stimuli: hammerstone grip shifts; hammer-stone changes; core inversions; and abrasion⁄ micro-flaking. These actions are all components of the distinctive ‘platform preparation’ operation discussed above, and their frequency directly reflects the greater technological complexity of Late Acheulean toolmaking. This complexity includes increased contingency on detailed variation in hammerstone properties, grips and gestures, and in core morphology, orientation and support, as well as a greater hierarchical depth of action planning.

fMRI paradigm

Subjects lay supine in the 3T Siemens Allegra MRI scanner at the Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging, pads positioned on the side of the head to reduce movement. Subjects underwent six sessions of approximately 7 min, and each session comprised 12 trials, corre-sponding to one repetition of six experimental conditions defined by a three· two factorial plan.

1. Stimulus: 20-s video clips of the Control stimulus, Oldowan or Acheulean toolmaking.

2. Task: following stimulus presentation, subjects were instructed either to simulate themselves continuing to perform the action they saw (Imagine) or to decide whether, in their opinion, the actor was successful in achieving his goal (Evaluate).

Prior to entering the scanner, subjects were instructed to watch each video ‘carefully’, to ‘try to understand what the demonstrator is doing’ and that after each video they would be ‘asked to do one of two things’, which were then explained. In the scanner, each trial was started by the presentation of the stimulus, followed by: (i) 1.5 s of a fixation cross; (ii) a written instruction indicating the Task (‘Imagine’ or ‘Evaluate’) that remained on screen for 5 s; and (iii) a response screen displaying the appropriate question (‘Did you finish?’ or ‘Was he successful?’). The side for yes and no responses was randomly assigned to the left and right button press and indicated by the position of the words ‘Yes’ and ‘No’ on screen. The response screen remained visible for 1.5 s or until subjects replied, and was followed by a fixation screen (minimum 1 s) for a total trial duration of 29 s. In addition, each session included four 12-s rest trials, each of which started with a 1-s ‘Rest’ indication, and ended with a 1-s ‘End of rest’

indication plus a 1-s fixation screen, giving a total duration of 15 s. Trials were interleaved so that in each session, experimental trials took place in blocks of two or three. Each session comprised one repetition of each of the six conditions; the order was pseudo-randomized to avoid repetition of the type of stimulus in consecutive trials and of individual stimulus videos within a session. Presentation of stimuli and recording of participants’ responses were carried out using Cogent (http://www.vislab.ucl.ac.uk/cogent_graphics.php) running in Mat-lab6.5 (MathWorks).

fMRI recording and preprocessing

In each of the six experimental sessions, a T2*-weighted, gradient-echo, echo-planar imaging sequence was used to acquire 164 40-slice (2 mm thickness and 1 mm gap; TE = 65 ms; a = 90) volumes covering the whole brain and cerebellum with an in-plane resolution of 3· 3 mm (64· 64 matrix, fov 192· 192 · 144 mm3

; TR = 2600 ms). A high-resolution (1· 1 · 1 mm3) structural image

(MPRAGE sequence) was also collected.

fMRI data were analysed using SPM8 (http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/ spm) procedures, running in Matlab 7.6 (MathWorks), after discarding the first four dummy volumes in each session to allow for T1 equilibrium effect. Slice timing correction was applied to correct for offsets of slice acquisition. EPI volumes were realigned to the first volume for each subject to correct for interscan movement, and unwarped for movement-induced inhomogeneities of the magnetic field using realignment parameters (Andersson et al., 2001). EPI volumes were stereotactically normalized into the standard space defined by the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) using a two-step procedure: the mean EPI image created during realignment was coregistered with the structural image, which was spatially normalized to the SPM T1 template using a 12-parameter affine and non-linear cosine basis function transformation, both transformations being subsequently applied to all EPI volumes. Normalized images were smoothed using an 8-mm isometric Gaussian kernel to account for residual inter-subject differences in functional anatomy (Friston et al., 2007).

fMRI statistical analysis

Analysis of the functional imaging data entailed the creation of statistical parametric maps representing a statistical assessment of hypothesized condition-specific effects (Friston et al., 1994). A random effect procedure was adopted for data analysis. Within individual subjects, the 20-s stimulations were modelled for the three types of stimuli (Control, Oldowan, Acheulean), the 5-s tasks were modelled for the three types of stimuli and two tasks (Imagine, Evaluate), and the motor responses were modelled as events (duration 0) irrespective of the experimental condition. Rest was modelled as a 12-s condition. Each condition was defined with a boxcar function convolved with SPM8 canonical haemodynamic response function to estimate condition-specific effects with the General Linear Model. Low-frequency drifts were removed by a high-pass filtering with a cut-off of 128 s.

The current analysis focused on the response to Acheulean, Oldowan and Control stimuli, with individual subjects’ statistical maps for the four contrasts [Toolmaking: (Acheulean–Control) + (Oldowan–Control); Acheulean–Control, Oldowan–Control, Acheu-lean–Oldowan] entered in second-level analyses of variance, with the group (Naı¨ve n = 11, Trained n = 10, Expert n = 5) as between-subjects factor. As all groups comprise neurotypical adults, we hypothesized equal variance between populations in order to control for differences in group size (Penny & Holmes, 2003).

Table 1. Frequency of technological actions in fMRI stimuli

Action Hand Control Oldowan Acheulean

Percussion R 81 36 26

Shift core grip L 39 12 13

Rotate core L 37 16 7

Shift hammerstone grip R 3 0 8

Invert core L 0 2 7 Change hammerstone R 0 1 3 Abrade⁄ micro-flake R 0 0 12 Sweep R 0 14 8 Grasp flake R 0 8 3 Flakes detached N⁄ A 0 29 16

(4)

Common brain response irrespective of expertise was investigated using a minimum statistic conjunction (Nichols et al., 2005) between the three groups. Brain response specific of each group was assessed by masking exclusively the effect of this group by a global null conjunction (P < 0.05 uncorrected) of the other two groups; for instance, the contrast between Acheulean and Oldowan in Naı¨ve is exclusively masked by a conjunction of the same contrast in Trained and Expert subjects. Our procedure used exclusive masking instead of interactions, which were not significant at the threshold used, to favour the effects within the group of interest over the reversed effects in the other groups, which are included in the statistics of interactions (Culham, 2006). All contrasts were thresholded at P < 0.05 FDR-corrected with an extent threshold of 20 voxels.

Anatomical localization was performed using a brain atlas (Duver-noy, 1999) and, in particular for inferior frontal and parietal clusters, functional localization made use of distribution analysis of the activated voxels on the basis of probabilistic cytoarchitectonic maps (Eickhoff et al., 2007) implemented in SPM (Eickhoff et al., 2005). For the sake of consistency, only anatomical labels are used in the tables. Thus, clusters attributed to Brodmann area (BA) 44 were labelled ‘pars opercularis’ (Amunts et al., 1999), those attributed to BA45 were labelled ‘pars triangularis’ (Amunts et al., 1999), and those attributed to BA6 were labelled ‘precentral gyrus’ (Geyer, 2003). In the parietal cortex, clusters attributed to areas PF and PG (Caspers et al., 2006) were labelled ‘inferior parietal lobule’, and those attributed to hIP1 and hIP2 (Choi et al., 2006) were labelled ‘anterior intraparietal sulcus’. While recognizing that functional localization and anatomical landmarks may not strictly overlap in individuals, these conventions were adopted in the interest of coherence in the presentation of results. Statistical maps were rendered on FreeSurfer’s fsaverage pial surface with 50 inflation steps (http://surfer.nmr. mgh.harvard.edu).

Local activity

In order to assess the effect of Group, local activity in clusters of interest was further characterized using the SPM extension toolbox MarsBar (http://marsbar.sourceforge.net/) to extract percentage signal change in 5-mm radius volumes centred on the maximum of each cluster, then analysed with spss.

Results

Toolmaking–Control Common response

Across all subject groups, the contrast of Toolmaking conditions with Control yielded activations is a series of cortical regions, including a large cluster extending from the primary visual and lateral occipital cortices to the inferior temporal cortices, intraparietal sulci, inferior parietal cortices and postcentral gyrii bilaterally. In the frontal cortex, responses were found in right pars triangularis and bilateral pars opercularis of the inferior frontal gyrus, as well as in the dorsal precentral gyrii bilaterally (Fig. 1; Table 2).

Two-way (Stimulus, Group) analysis of variance of the extracted percent signal change in the right pars triangularis revealed a main effect of Stimulus (P < 0.001), with no effect of Group (P = 0.9) or interaction between Stimulus and Group (P = 0.5; see Fig. 1, right). All pairwise comparisons between stimuli are significant (Oldowan vs. Control P = 0.001; Acheulean vs. Control P < 0.001; Acheulean vs. Oldowan P = 0.016).

Effect of expertise

The exclusive masking procedure used to isolate brain responses to the observation of Toolmaking stimuli unique to each level of expertise identified clusters (Fig. 2; Table 2) in the bilateral ventral precentral gyrus and left middle occipital gyrus in the Naı¨ve group, and in the left superior parietal and right postcentral gyrus of Experts. Activations unique to the Trained group were much more numerous particularly in the frontal cortices, including medial frontal cortex, the right pars orbitalis, left pars triangularis, bilateral pars opercularis, right anterior insula, left posterior middle frontal gyrus and left precentral gyrus, as well as left middle temporal gyrus and right inferior temporal gyrus.

Acheulean vs. Oldowan Common response

The minimum statistic conjunction between the three groups for the contrast Acheulean–Oldowan identified increases in activity in the anterior part of the left intraparietal sulcus (Fig. 3; Table 3), and in the left prefrontal cortex within the inferior frontal sulcus.

Fig. 1. Left: local brain activity in Toolmaking–Control irrespective of subject expertise (FDR P < 0.05, extent k > 20). Rendered with freesurfer on top, lateral and ventral views of the two hemispheres in neurological convention (details in Methods and Table 1). Right: percent signal change across the three technologies in the right pars triangularis (***P < 0.001; *P < 0.05).

(5)

In agreement with SPM whole-brain investigation, analysis of variance of activity extracted in these clusters indicated a main effect of the stimulus (both P < 0.001), while there was no effect of Group or interaction between Group and Stimulus (all P > 0.3) in these regions. Activity in Acheulean was significantly increased compared with Oldowan (P < 0.001) and Control (P < 0.05) for the left prefrontal cortex cluster, and all pairwise comparisons were significant (P < 0.001) for the anterior intraparietal sulcus.

Effect of expertise

In Naı¨ve subjects, there were activations for Acheulean–Oldowan in the left frontal cortex, dorsally in the superior frontal gyrus and ventrally in

the pars opercularis of the inferior frontal gyrus (Fig. 4; Table 3). The latter activation was in a similar location to that previously reported for the actual performance (as opposed to observation) of stone toolmaking (Stout & Chaminade, 2007). No cluster survived the thresholds used in this analysis for Trained subjects.

In Experts (Fig. 4; Table 3), there were clusters in the right medial frontal and parietal cortices. The latter were localized in the inferior parietal lobule, and in the anterior intraparietal sulcus area hIP1 (Choi et al., 2006; see also Jubault et al., 2007).

Discussion

To identify brain systems involved in the observation of Paleolithic toolmaking, we examined contrasts of toolmaking observation with a control condition. Results were remarkably similar to those obtained from previous FDG-PET studies of toolmaking execution, despite the different experimental tasks and imaging modalities used. This indicates that neural systems involved in the observational under-standing of Paleolithic toolmaking are very similar to those involved in execution.

To investigate the effects of expertise on toolmaking observation, we examined the unique responses of each subject group (Naı¨ve, Trained and Expert) to Toolmaking stimuli. This provided evidence for the functional ‘reorganization’ (Kelly & Garavan, 2005) of activation between groups, reflecting expertise-dependent shifts in cognitive strategy.

To investigate the specific demands of understanding increasingly complex Paleolithic technologies, we examined the contrast in brain response to Acheulean vs. Oldowan stimuli in Naı¨ve, Trained and Expert subjects. This revealed a significant main effect of technological complexity across groups, as well as distinct responses in the Naı¨ve and Expert groups. The localization of these expertise-dependent effects suggests that stone toolmaking action understanding depends on a complex mixture of top-down, bottom-up, conceptual and embodied processes (cf. Grafton, 2009).

Contrasts of Toolmaking with Control condition Common response

Contrasts of toolmaking stimuli with Control yielded activations in a series of cortical regions, notably including inferior frontal gyrus, dorsal premotor cortex, intraparietal sulcus and the inferior parietal lobule (Fig. 1; Table 1). Activations in these regions have com-monly been reported in imaging studies of action observation (Grezes & Decety, 2001; Grafton, 2009; Caspers et al., 2010), and they are thought to comprise a network supporting action

Table 2. Brain activity in response of the observation of Toolmaking compared with Control stimuli, common to the three groups (minimum statistic conjunction) and by subject expertise (exclusive masking)

Location x y z Z-score Extent

Conjunction Frontal lobe

Left Dorsal precentral gyrus )32 )8 58 4.39 421 Right Dorsal precentral gyrus 30 )8 54 4.66 1003

Right Pars opercularis 60 10 26 5.68 747

Left Pars opercularis )54 8 22 4.89 612

Right Pars triangularis 50 44 10 3.23 53

Right Insula 40 )4 8 5.40 328

Left Insula )42 )4 2 4.04

Parietal lobe

Left Intraparietal sulcus )26 )52 62 5.25 – Left Postcentral gyrus )30 )44 62 5.91 19 098 Right Intraparietal sulcus 32 )60 56 5.39 –

Right Postcentral gyrus 36 )40 56 5.06 –

Right Supramarginal gyrus 66 )20 36 5.82 – Left Supramarginal gyrus )64 )20 36 5.46 – Temporal lobe

Left Inferior temporal gyrus )48 )64 )10 5.40 –

Left Fusiform gyrus )48 )50 )20 5.57 –

Right Fusiform gyrus 34 )46 )28 5.72 –

Occipital lobe

Right Middle occipital gyrus 44 )84 12 5.23 –

Left Calcarine fissure )8 )84 4 3.40 209

Right Middle occipital gyrus 40 )78 )4 5.12 – Basal ganglia

Right Thalamus 14 )26 8 4.89 947

Left Thalamus )16 )32 )4 3.32 114

Naı¨ve only

Left Precentral gyrus )58 )2 30 3.81 34

Right Precentral gyrus 64 0 28 3.80 29

Left Middle occipital gyrus )22 )88 14 4.76 31 Trained only

Left Precentral gyrus )18 )10 64 4.44 242 Left Middle frontal gyrus )24 4 54 4.19 – Left Medial frontal cortex )12 4 48 3.95 –

Left Precentral gyrus )38 )6 42 4.04 –

Right Intraparietal sulcus 44 )48 40 3.83 31

Left Pars opercularis )42 8 30 4.19 369

Right Pars opercularis 60 20 12 3.78 –

Left Pars triangularis )54 40 8 4.81 287

Right Anterior insula 34 20 2 3.94 165

Right Pars orbitalis 48 48 )4 3.77 107

Left Middle temporal gyrus )60 )62 )6 4.04 36 Right Inferior temporal gyrus 62 )56 )10 4.24 38 Experts only

Left Superior parietal lobule )10 )52 74 4.46 159 Right Postcentral gyrus 48 )30 62 3.87 25 All results are FDR P < 0.05, extent k > 20. All coordinates MNI.

Fig. 2. Local brain activity in Toolmaking–Control for Naı¨ve (left), Trained (centre) and Expert (right) subjects (FDR P < 0.05, extent k > 20).

(6)

understanding through the covert simulation of observed behaviours. In keeping with this, the observed activations closely match (see also Supporting Information Fig. S2; Tables S1 and S2) those reported in previous FDG-PET studies, in which subjects actively produced tools rather than simply observing toolmaking (Stout & Chaminade, 2007; Stout et al., 2008).

Particularly notable is activation of the pars triangularis of the right inferior frontal gyrus. Pars triangularis activation is more typically associated with linguistic processing (e.g. Bookheimer, 2002; Musso et al., 2003), but has been reported during action observation (Johnson-Frey et al., 2003; Molnar-Szakacs et al., 2005; Caspers et al., 2010). It has been proposed (Rizzolatti & Craighero, 2004) that such activation reflects the ‘syntactic’ processing of hierarchically organized actions (cf. Koechlin & Jubault, 2006). This leads to the expectation that pars triangularis activity should respond to variation in the complexity of observed actions (Caspers et al., 2010). Such an effect of stimulus complexity is observed here (Fig. 1), in keeping with previous findings of pars triangularis activation during the execution of Acheulean, but not Oldowan, toolmaking (Stout et al., 2008; Table 2).

Fig. 3. Left: local brain activity in Acheulean–Oldowan irrespective of subject expertise (FDR P < 0.05, extent k > 20). Right: percent signal change across the three technologies in the left anterior intraparietal sulcus (top) and inferior frontal sulcus (bottom; ***P < 0.001; *P < 0.05).

Table 3.Brain activity in response of the observation of Acheulean compared with Oldowan toolmaking stimuli, common to the three groups (minimum statistic conjunction) and by subject expertise (exclusive masking)

Location x y z Z-score Extent

Conjunction

Left Anterior intraparietal sulcus )50 )36 42 4.74 53

Left Pars triangularis )46 32 26 6.02 94

Naı¨ve only

Left Superior frontal gyrus )24 22 56 4.21 51

Left Pars opercularis )60 12 24 4.46 83

Experts only

Right Medial frontal cortex 6 42 50 5.25 24

Right Anterior intraparietal sulcus 46 )48 46 5.65 44

Right Inferior parietal lobule 62 )52 36 4.80 22

All results are FDR P < 0.05, extent k > 20. All coordinates MNI.

Fig. 4. Local brain activity in Acheulean–Oldowan for Naı¨ve (left) and Expert (right) subjects (FDR P < 0.05, extent k > 20).

(7)

Effect of stone toolmaking method

Across groups, the increased technological complexity of Acheulean stimuli compared with Oldowan (Table 1) was associated with activation of the anterior intraparietal sulcus and inferior frontal sulcus, both in the left hemisphere (Fig. 3; Table 3). The anterior intraparietal sulcus is a core component of the putative human mirror neuron system (Grafton & Hamilton, 2007). It is thought to contribute to the understanding of ‘immediate’ action goals, such as grasping to eat vs. to place in macaque monkeys (Fogassi et al., 2005), or taking a cookie vs. a diskette in humans (Hamilton & Grafton, 2006).

In monkeys, the anterior bank of the intraparietal sulcus changes its connectivity and response patterns when the animals train to use tools (Hihara et al., 2006), enabling an integration of visual and somato-sensory stimuli. This is argued to support tool use through assimilation of the tool into the monkey’s body schema (Maravita & Iriki, 2004), such that ‘tools become hands’ (Umilta` et al., 2008). However, human left anterior inferior parietal lobule displays a specific response to observed tool use (as opposed to unassisted manual prehension) that is absent in monkeys (Peeters et al., 2009). This suggests that hominoid anterior inferior parietal cortex may be evolutionarily derived to play a new role in coding the distinct functional properties of hand-held tools (Johnson-Frey et al., 2005; Peeters et al., 2009; Jacobs et al., 2010; Povinelli et al., 2010).

The centre of anterior inferior parietal cortex activation reported here is somewhat posterior ()50, )36, 42 vs. )52, )26, 34) to that of Peeters et al. (2009); however, extraction of the volume of interest used by Peeters et al. (coordinates from Orban, pers. comm.) confirms that the same effect of stimulus is indeed present in this region. This response to increasingly complex Paleolithic toolmaking is consistent with the hypothesis that human technological evolution was sup-ported, at least in part, by the emergence of enhanced neural mechanisms for representing the causal properties of hand-held tools (Johnson-Frey, 2003; Wolpert, 2003; Peeters et al., 2009).

The main effect in the prefrontal cortex was centred on the inferior frontal sulcus. In macaques, this region is heavily interconnected with the anterior inferior parietal lobule (Pandya & Seltzer, 1982) and the parietal operculum (Preuss & Goldman-Rakic, 1989), in keeping with the co-activation observed here, and suggesting involvement in the integration of visuospatial and somatosensory information. In an fMRI study with macaques, there was activation in this area during the observation of actions (Nelissen et al., 2005). In contrast to more the posterior premotor cortex (F5c) where mirror neurons were originally recorded, the ventral prefrontal cortex also responded to abstract or context-free stimuli, including isolated hands, robotic hands and shapes (Nelissen et al., 2005), indicating representation and integra-tion of acintegra-tions at a relatively high level. In humans, activaintegra-tion of similar coordinates is reported during observation and preparation to imitate complex hand postures (guitar chords), perhaps indicating a role for this region in the selection and combination of motor elements into novel actions (Vogt et al., 2007).

It is thus likely that the increase in prefrontal activation for Acheulean–Oldowan reflects the greater temporal and relational complexity of Acheulean toolmaking actions, which, to a greater extent than Oldowan flaking, are organized into flexible and internally variable action chunks, such as ‘platform preparation’ vs. ‘primary flake removal’ (Pelegrin, 2005; Stout, 2011). No significant prefrontal activation was observed for Oldowan–Control, in keeping with previous conclusions regarding the relative simplicity of Oldowan action sequences (Stout & Chaminade, 2007; Stout et al., 2008).

On this interpretation, the anterior inferior parietal cortex and the inferior frontal sulcus form a parieto-frontal circuit involved in

representing episode-specific intentions, causal relations and multi-component action sequences during toolmaking observation. The apparent abstraction (Hamilton & Grafton, 2006; Badre & D’Esposito, 2009) of causal⁄ intentional processing in this circuit may be compared with a proposed ‘intermediate’ level representing ‘intentions in action’ as goal-oriented sequences of motor commands and predicted outcomes (de Vignemont & Haggard, 2008).

Expertise effects on response to stimuli

Varying expertise across subject groups was associated with qualitative shifts in the set of brain regions activated in response to Acheulean compared with Oldowan stimuli (Fig. 4; Table 3). These differences suggest a functional reorganization (Kelly & Garavan, 2005) involving the adoption of different cognitive strategies for action understanding. Naı¨ve subjects show activation in core motor resonance structures together with the ventral prefrontal cortex, as expected for a low-level strategy of novel action understanding through kinematic simulation. Trained subjects show strong, statistically indistinguishable responses to both Oldowan and Acheulean stimuli, perhaps reflecting the particular social context and motivational set associated with training. Finally, Expert subjects display activation in the medial prefrontal cortex, a classic ‘mentalizing’ region, suggesting a relatively high-level, inferential strategy of intention reading.

Naı¨ve subjects

One cluster exclusive to technologically Naı¨ve subjects occurred in the pars opercularis of the left posterior inferior frontal gyrus (Fig. 4, left). Pars opercularis is another core component of the putative human mirror neuronal system (Rizzolatti & Craighero, 2004), which, in contrast with the performance-monitoring functions of the anterior inferior parietal cortex described above, is thought to be responsible for the generation of the kinematic models used to execute (Fagg & Arbib, 1998) or simulate (Carr et al., 2003; Grafton & Hamilton, 2007; Kilner et al., 2007) motor acts.

Also unique to Naı¨ve subjects was activation of the superior frontal gyrus, anterior to the frontal eye field (Lobel et al., 2001). Activation of this area is associated with the selection among competing responses (Petrides, 2005), and the more superior portion activated here is especially involved in the spatial domain (Volle et al., 2008). During imitation, this region may serve to maintain a representation of the observed goal in short-term working memory for later execution (Chaminade et al., 2002). Co-activation of the superior frontal gyrus and posterior inferior frontal gyrus may thus reflect Naı¨ve reliance on kinematic simulation and top-down direction of attention to task-relevant spatial cues. When combined with the anterior inferior parietal and ventral prefrontal activations observed across all groups, these Naı¨ve activations match the general expectations of a simulation model of novel action understanding (Buccino et al., 2004; Vogt et al., 2007).

Trained subjects

No activations exclusive to Trained subjects were observed in the Acheulean–Oldowan contrast. Comparison with the numerous activa-tions observed in the contrast of Toolmaking–Control for Trained subjects (Table 2; Fig. 2) indicates that this result derives from the presence of similar responses to Oldowan and Acheulean stimuli rather than from the absence of significant differences from Control. This is corroborated by the observation of similar activations in separate contrasts of Oldowan–Control and Acheulean–Control (Supporting Information Figs S3 and S4; Tables S1 and S2). The Trained response to both Oldowan and Acheulean stimuli includes:

(8)

(i) clusters in the anterior insula, lateral premotor cortex, frontal eye field and supplementary eye field likely related to attentional and affective engagement with the stimuli; and (ii) ventral prefrontal clusters likely associated with parsing of observed action sequences.

Insular activations unique to Trained subjects are in an anterior region associated with interoception, subjective feeling and perceptual awareness (Kikyo et al., 2002; Ploran et al., 2007; Craig, 2009). Activations of the left medial frontal cortex (close to y = 0) and posterior middle frontal gyrus appear to fall within the supplementary and frontal eye fields (Tehovnik et al., 2000), functional regions associated with saccades, visual attention and visual learning (Tehovnik et al., 2000; Grosbras et al., 2005). Together with activa-tion of the precentral gyrus, a region commonly recruited during action observation (Grezes & Decety, 2001; Caspers et al., 2010), these activations likely indicate intense engagement by Trained subjects with the Toolmaking stimuli. These effects of training were not predicted, but are consistent with the pragmatic social and motivational context created by the training programme.

Also unique to Trained subjects were inferior frontal gyrus activations of bilateral pars opercularis, left pars triangularis and right pars orbitalis. These are probably best understood in terms of the putative role of the inferior frontal gyrus in the multi-level processing of stimuli along a posterior to anterior gradient of increasing hierarchical complexity (Koechlin & Jubault, 2006; Caspers et al., 2010), and may reflect the intense processing of all Toolmaking stimuli by highly motivated Trained subjects.

Expert subjects

Activations exclusive to Expert subjects were observed in the medial frontal cortex, anterior intraparietal sulcus and inferior parietal lobule of the right hemisphere (Fig. 4, right). The medial frontal cortex is a core element in the network of brain regions associated with the attribution of mental states (Frith & Frith, 2006), suggesting that Expert subjects rely on top-down interpretation of the demonstrator’s intentions in order to differentiate Acheulean from Oldowan toolmaking. The activation is centred at the border between a posterior region associated with the attribution of ‘private’ action intentions and an anterior region associated with communicative intentions (Gre`zes et al., 2004a,b; Amodio & Frith, 2006), in a position closely approximating that activated when mentalizing about the internal states of a dissimilar other (Mitchell et al., 2006). It may reflect inference about the private technological ‘prior intentions’ of the demonstrator (Chaminade et al., 2002), rather than meta-cognition about the demonstrator’s communi-cative intentions toward the observer (Amodio & Frith, 2006: 274).

Activation of the right anterior intraparietal sulcus in Experts is comparable to expertise effects found in studies of dance observation (Calvo-Merino et al., 2005, 2006; Cross et al., 2006). The more anterior location the current activation may reflect somatotopy of response to the observation of upper vs. lower limb actions (Buccino et al., 2001). This particular region of right anterior intraparietal sulcus has also been linked with the preparation of successive sensorimotor task-sets during action sequence execution (Jubault et al., 2007).

Also activated in Experts was a region of right inferior parietal lobule known to support the stimulus-driven allocation of spatial attention (Corbetta & Shulman, 2002; Mort et al., 2003) during visuospatial sequence learning (Rosenthal et al., 2009). This activa-tion is posterior to the region associated with acactiva-tion outcome monitoring by Hamilton & Grafton (2008), and together with the right anterior intraparietal sulcus activation probably reflects Expert recognition of familiar toolmaking action sequences.

Broader implications

Contrasts with Control show that the observation of Paleolithic toolmaking recruits cognitive control mechanisms in the pars triangularis of the right inferior frontal gyrus, and that this response increases with the technological complexity of the observed actions. This matches results from earlier studies of subjects actively making stone tools, and is consistent with an evolutionary scenario in which manual and perceptual-motor adaptations were critical to the earliest stages of human technological evolution (Wynn & McGrew, 1989; Ambrose, 2001; Byrne, 2004; Bril & Roux, 2005; Stout & Chamin-ade, 2007), but later developments were more dependent on enhanced cognitive control (Faisal et al., 2010; Stout, 2010). These findings support long-standing intuitions regarding the cognitive sophistication of Acheulean technology (e.g. Oakley, 1954; Wynn, 1979; Gowlett, 1986), and specifically highlight the complex hierarchical organization (Holloway, 1969; Stout et al., 2008) of Acheulean action sequences. This interpretation is further supported by the main effect of stimulus in the anterior inferior parietal and ventral prefrontal cortices across subject groups.

Differing responses to stimulus complexity between groups provide insight into the effects of expertise on action observation strategies. Activations specific to Naı¨ve subjects suggest a strategy reliant on kinematic simulation (inferior frontal gyrus) and the top-down direction of visuospatial attention (superior frontal gyrus). This supports an account of early observational learning in which simulation of low-level action elements interacts with representations of mid-level intentions in action to produce a ‘best-fit’ understanding of complex, unfamiliar actions (cf. Vogt et al., 2007).

Interestingly, Trained subjects responded equally to Oldowan and Acheulean stimuli, activating a set of frontal regions related to subjective awareness, visual attention and multi-level action parsing. This unexpected result may reflect a strong motivation to attend to, analyse and understand all Toolmaking stimuli, generated by the social and pragmatic context of being a ‘learner’ (cf. Lave & Wenger, 1991; Stout, 2002). There is increasing awareness of the importance of such social and affective dimensions in understanding human cognitive evolution (Holloway, 1967; Hare & Tomasello, 2005; Burkart et al., 2009; Stout, 2010).

Unlike Naive and Trained subjects, Experts recruited a mixture of bottom-up, familiarity-based posterior parietal mechanisms for visuo-spatial attention (right inferior parietal lobule) and sensorimotor matching (anterior intraparietal sulcus) with high-level inference regarding technological ‘prior intentions’ in the medial frontal cortex. In this context, shared pragmatic skills may provide the foundation for sharing of higher level intentions, in keeping with the Motor Cognition Hypothesis (Gallese et al., 2009). More broadly, the apparent shift in observation strategy from Naive kinematic simulation to Expert mentalizing is consistent with a ‘mixed’ model of action understanding (Grafton, 2009) involving contextually variable inter-actions between bottom-up resonance and top-down interpretation.

Complex, pragmatic skills like stone toolmaking can only be acquired through deliberate practice (Pelegrin, 1990; Whittaker, 1994) and experimentation (Ericsson et al., 1993), leading to the discovery of subtle causal relations that would remain ‘opaque’ (Gergely & Csibra, 2006) to observation and simulation alone. Mid-level ‘intentions in action’ represented in the anterior inferior parietal and the ventral prefrontal cortices, though likely to be inaccurate at first, appear to be important across skill levels and may play an important role in guiding such practice, perhaps contributing to the high fidelity of human social learning (the ‘ratchet effect’: Tomasello, 1999; Tennie et al., 2009). The effect of Toolmaking complexity in the anterior inferior parietal lobule

(9)

in particular suggests that this phylogenetically derived (Peeters et al., 2009) region may have played a key role in human technological evolution 2.6–0.5 million years ago.

Supporting Information

Additional supporting information may be found in the online version of this article:

Fig. S1. Handaxes produced (a–c) by Trained subjects, (d) by the expert demonstrator, and (e) from the Middle Pleistocene (approxi-mately 500 000 years ago) site of Boxgrove, West Sussex, UK. Fig. S2. Local brain activity in Oldowan–Control (left) and Acheu-lean–Control (right) irrespective of subject expertise (FDR P < 0.05, extent k > 20). To more directly compare current results with previous FDG-PET studies of Oldowan and Acheulean toolmaking execution, we examined separate contrasts of Oldowan and Acheulean toolmak-ing with the Control. This yielded activations of left ventral premotor cortex in both contrasts (Oldowan: )56, 8, 22; Acheulean: )58, 10, 32), and of right pars triangularis in the Acheulean (46, 36, 4) but not Oldowan contrast. This directly matches results from the execution of Oldowan (ventral premotor cortex:)52, 6, 28) and Acheulean (ventral premotor cortex:)52, 6, 28; pars triangularis: 48, 34, 10) toolmaking (Stout et al., 2008; Table 2).

Fig. S3. Local brain activity in Oldowan–Control for Naı¨ve (left), Trained (centre) and Expert (right) subjects (FDR P < 0.05, extent k > 20).

Fig. S4. Local brain activity in Acheulean–Control for Naı¨ve (left), Trained (centre) and Experts (right) subjects (FDR P < 0.05, extent k > 20).

Table S1. Brain activity in response of the observation of Oldowan compared with Control stimuli, common to the three groups (minimum statistic conjunction) and by subject expertise (exclusive masking). All results are FDR P < 0.05, extent k > 20.

Table S2. Brain activity in response of the observation of Acheulean compared with Control stimuli, common to the three groups (minimum statistic conjunction) and by subject expertise (exclusive masking). All results are FDR P < 0.05, extent k > 20.

Video S1. Examples of Control, Oldowan and Acheulean stimuli used in the experiment.

Please note: As a service to our authors and readers, this journal provides supporting information supplied by the authors. Such materials are peer-reviewed and may be re-organized for online delivery, but are not copy-edited or typeset by Wiley-Blackwell. Technical support issues arising from supporting information (other than missing files) should be addressed to the authors.

Acknowledgements

This research was funded by European Union project HANDTOMOUTH. We thank Bruce Bradley for acting as the expert demonstrator, and Stefan Vogt and an anonymous reviewer for helpful comments.

Abbreviations

BA, Brodmann area; fMRI, functional magnetic resonance imaging; PET, positron emission tomography.

References

Ambrose, S. (2001) Paleolithic technology and human evolution. Science, 291, 1748–1753.

Amodio, D.M. & Frith, C.D. (2006) Meeting of minds: the medial frontal cortex and social cognition. Nat. Rev. Neurosci., 7, 268–277.

Amunts, K., Schleicher, A., Burgel, U., Mohlberg, H., Uylings, H.B. & Zilles, K. (1999) Broca’s region revisited: cytoarchitecture and intersubject variability. J. Comp. Neurol., 412, 319–341.

Andersson, J.L., Hutton, C., Ashburner, J., Turner, R. & Friston, K. (2001) Modeling geometric deformations in EPI time series. Neuroimage, 13, 903– 919.

Badre, D. & D’Esposito, M. (2009) Is the rostro-caudal axis of the frontal lobe hierarchical? Nat. Rev. Neurosci., 10, 659–669.

Beck, B.B. (1980) Animal Tool Behavior: The Use and Manufacture of Tools by Animals. Garland STPM Press, New York.

Blakemore, S.-J. & Decety, J. (2001) From the perception of action to the understanding of intention. Nat. Rev. Neurosci., 2, 561–567.

Bookheimer, S. (2002) Functional MRI of language: new approaches to understanding the cortical organization of semantic processing. Annu. Rev. Neurosci., 25, 151–188.

Bril, B. & Roux, V. (2005) Synthesis and speculations. In Roux, V. & Bril, B. (Eds), Stone Knapping: The Necessary Conditions for a Uniquely Hominin Behaviour.McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research, Cambridge, pp. 353–355.

Buccino, G., Binkofski, F., Fink, G.R., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., Seitz, R.J., Zilles, K., Rizzolatti, G. & Freund, H.-J. (2001) Action observation activates premotor and parietal areas in a somatotopic manner: an fMRI study. Eur. J. Neurosci., 13, 400–404.

Buccino, G., Vogt, S., Ritzl, A., Fink, G.R., Zilles, K., Freund, H.-J. & Rizzolatti, G. (2004) Neural circuits underlying imitation learning of hand actions: an Event-Related fMRI Study. Neuron, 42, 323–334.

Burkart, J.M., Hrdy, S.B. & Schaik, C.P.V. (2009) Cooperative breeding and human cognitive evolution. Evol. Anthropol., 18, 175–186.

Byrne, R. (2004) The manual skills and cognition that lie behind hominid tool use. In Russon, A.E.B. & David, R. (Ed), The Evolution of Thought: Evolutionary Origins Of Great Ape Intelligence. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 31–44.

Calvo-Merino, B., Glaser, D.E., Grezes, J., Passingham, R.E. & Haggard, P. (2005) Action observation and acquired motor skills: an fMRI Study with expert dancers. Cereb. Cortex, 15, 1243–1249.

Calvo-Merino, B., Grezes, J., Glaser, D.E., Passingham, R.E. & Haggard, P. (2006) Seeing or doing? Influence of visual and motor familiarity in action observation Curr. Biol., 16, 1905–1910.

Carr, L., Iacoboni, M., Dubeau, M.-C., Mazziotta, J.C. & Lenzi, G.L. (2003) Neural mechanisms of empathy in humans: a relay from neural systems for imitation to limbic areas. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA, 100, 5497–5502. Caspers, S., Geyer, S., Schleicher, A., Mohlberg, H., Amunts, K. & Zilles, K.

(2006) The human inferior parietal cortex: cytoarchitectonic parcellation and interindividual variability. Neuroimage, 33, 430–448.

Caspers, S., Zilles, K., Laird, A.R. & Eickhoff, S.B. (2010) ALE meta-analysis of action observation and imitation in the human brain. NeuroImage, 50, 1148–1167.

Chaminade, T., Meltzoff, A.N. & Decety, J. (2002) Does the end justify the means? A PET exploration of the mechanisms involved in human imitation NeuroImage, 15, 318–328.

Choi, H.-J., Zilles, K., Mohlberg, H., Schleicher, A., Fink, G.R., Armstrong, E. & Amunts, K. (2006) Cytoarchitectonic identification and probabilistic mapping of two distinct areas within the anterior ventral bank of the human intraparietal sulcus. J. Comp. Neurol., 495, 53–69.

Corbetta, M. & Shulman, G.L. (2002) Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci., 3, 201–215.

Craig, A.D. (2009) How do you feel – now? The anterior insula and human awareness Nat. Rev. Neurosci., 10, 59–70.

Cross, E.S., Hamilton, A.F.d.C. & Grafton, S.T. (2006) Building a motor simulation de novo: observation of dance by dancers. NeuroImage, 31, 1257–1267.

Culham, J.C. (2006) Functional neuroimaging: experimental design and analysis. In Cabeza, R. & Kingstone, A. (Eds), Handbook of Functional Neuroimaging of Cognition, 2nd edn. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, pp. 53– 82.

Duvernoy, H.M. (1999) The Human Brain: Surface, Blood Supply, and Three-dimensional Anatomy, 2nd edn, completely revised. Springer-Verlag, Wien New York.

Eickhoff, S.B., Stephan, K.E., Mohlberg, H., Grefkes, C., Fink, G.R., Amunts, K. & Zilles, K. (2005) A new SPM toolbox for combining probabilistic cytoarchitectonic maps and functional imaging data. Neuroimage, 25, 1325– 1335.

Eickhoff, S.B., Paus, T., Caspers, S., Grosbras, M.-H., Evans, A.C., Zilles, K. & Amunts, K. (2007) Assignment of functional activations to probabilistic cytoarchitectonic areas revisited. NeuroImage, 36, 511–521.

(10)

Ericsson, K.A., Krampe, R.T. & Tesch-Romer, C. (1993) The role of deliberate practice in the acquisition of expert performance. Psychol. Rev., 100, 363–406. Fagg, A. & Arbib, M.A. (1998) Modeling parietal-premotor interaction in

primate control of grasping. Neural Netw., 11, 1277–1303.

Faisal, A., Stout, D., Apel, J. & Bradley, B. (2010) The Manipulative complexity of lower paleolithic stone Toolmaking. PLoS ONE, 5, e13718.

Fogassi, L., Ferrari, P.F., Gesierich, B., Rozzi, S., Chersi, F. & Rizzolatti, G. (2005) Parietal Lobe: from Action Organization to intention understanding. Science, 308, 662–667.

Friston, K.J., Holmes, A.P., Worsley, K.J., Poline, J.P., Frith, C.D. & Frackowiak, R.S.J. (1994) Statistical parametric maps in functional imaging: a general linear approach. Hum. Brain Mapp., 2, 189–210.

Friston, K.J., Ashburner, J.T., Kiebel, S., Nichols, T.E. & Penny, W.D. (Eds) (2007) Statistical Parametric Mapping: The Analysis of Functional Brain Images. Elsevier, London, UK.

Frith, C.D. & Frith, U. (2006) The neural basis of mentalizing. Neuron, 50, 531–534.

Gallese, V. & Goldman, A. (1998) Mirror neurons and the simulation theory of mind-reading. Trends Cogn. Sci., 2, 493–501.

Gallese, V., Rochat, M., Cossu, G. & Sinigaglia, C. (2009) Motor cognition and its role in the phylogeny and ontogeny of action understanding. Dev. Psychol., 45, 103–113.

Gergely, G. & Csibra, G. (2006) Sylvia’s recipe: the role of imitation and pedagogy in the transmission of cultural knowledge. In Enfiled, N.J. & Levenson, S.C. (Eds), Roots of Human Sociality: Culture, Cognition and Human Interaction. Berg Publishers, Oxford, pp. 229–255.

Geyer, S. (2003) The microstructural border between the motor and the cognitive domain in the human cerebral cortex. In Advances in Anatomy, Embryology and Cell Biology, vol. 174. Springer, Berlin, 92 pp.

Gowlett, J.A.J. (1986) Culture and conceptualisation: the Oldowan-Acheulian gradient. In Bailey, G.N. & Callow, P. (Eds), Stone Age Prehistory: Studies in Memory of Charles McBurney. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 243–260.

Grafton, S.T. (2009) Embodied cognition and the simulation of action to understand others. Ann. N Y Acad. Sci., 1156, 97–117.

Grafton, S.T. & Hamilton, A.F.d.C. (2007) Evidence for a distributed hierarchy of action representation in the brain. Hum. Mov. Sci., 26, 590–616. Grezes, J. & Decety, J. (2001) Functional anatomy of execution, mental

simulation, observation, and verb generation of action: a meta-analysis. Hum. Brain Mapp., 12, 1–19.

Gre`zes, J., Frith, C. & Passingham, R.E. (2004a) Brain mechanisms for inferring Deceit in the actions of others. J. Neurosci., 24, 5500–5505. Gre`zes, J., Frith, C. & Passingham, R.E. (2004b) Inferring false beliefs from the

actions of oneself and others: an fMRI study. NeuroImage, 21, 744–750. Grosbras, M.-H., Laird, A.R. & Paus, T. (2005) Cortical regions involved in

eye movements, shifts of attention, and gaze perception. Hum. Brain Mapp., 25, 140–154.

Hamilton, A.F.d.C. & Grafton, S.T. (2006) Goal representation in human anterior intraparietal sulcus. J. Neurosci., 26, 1133–1137.

Hamilton, A.F.d.C. & Grafton, S.T. (2008) Action outcomes are represented in human inferior frontoparietal cortex. Cereb. Cortex, 18, 1160–1168. Hare, B. & Tomasello, M. (2005) Human-like social skills in dogs? Trends

Cogn. Sci., 9, 439–444.

Hihara, S., Notoya, T., Tanaka, M., Ichinose, S., Ojima, H., Obayashi, S., Fujii, N. & Iriki, A. (2006) Extension of corticocortical afferents into the anterior bank of the intraparietal sulcus by tool-use training in adult monkeys. Neuropsychologia, 44, 2636–2646.

Holloway, R.L. (1967) The evolution of the human brain: some notes toward a synthesis between neural structure and the evolution of complex behavior. Gen. Syst., 12, 3–19.

Holloway, R. (1969) Culture: a human domain. Curr. Anthropol., 10, 395–412. Jacob, P. & Jeannerod, M. (2005) The motor theory of social cognition: a

critique. Trends Cogn. Sci., 9, 21–25.

Jacobs, S., Danielmeier, C. & Frey, S.H. (2010) Human anterior intraparietal and ventral premotor cortices support representations of grasping with the Hand or a Novel tool. J. Cogn. Neurosci., 22, 2594–2608.

Johnson-Frey, S.H. (2003) What’s so special about human tool use? Neuron, 39, 201–204.

Johnson-Frey, S.H., Maloof, F.R., Newman-Norlund, R., Farrer, C., Inati, S. & Grafton, S.T. (2003) Actions or hand–object interactions? human inferior frontal cortex and action observation Neuron, 39, 1053–1058.

Johnson-Frey, S.H., Newman-Norlund, R. & Grafton, S.T. (2005) A distributed left hemisphere network active during planning of everyday tool use skills. Cereb. Cortex, 15, 681–695.

Jubault, T., Ody, C. & Koechlin, E. (2007) Serial organization of human behavior in the inferior parietal cortex. J. Neurosci., 27, 11028–11036. Kelly, A.M. & Garavan, H. (2005) Human functional neuroimaging of brain

changes associated with practice. Cereb. Cortex, 15, 1089–1102.

Kikyo, H., Ohki, K. & Miyashita, Y. (2002) Neural correlates for feeling-of-knowing: an fMRI parametric analysis. Neuron, 36, 177–186.

Kilner, J., Friston, K. & Frith, C. (2007) Predictive coding: an account of the mirror neuron system. Cogn. Process., 8, 159–166.

Koechlin, E. & Jubault, T. (2006) Broca’s area and the hierarchical organization of human behavior. Neuron, 50, 963–974.

Lave, J. & Wenger, E. (1991) Situated Learning: Legitimate Peripheral Participation. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.

Lobel, E., Kahane, P., Leonards, U., Grosbras, M.-H., Lehe´ricy, S., Bihan, D.L. & Berthoz, A. (2001) Localization of human frontal eye fields: anatomical and functional findings of functional magnetic resonance imaging and intracerebral electrical stimulation. J. Neurosurg., 95, 804– 815.

Maravita, A. & Iriki, A. (2004) Tools for the body (schema). Trends Cogn. Sci., 8, 79–86.

Mitchell, J.P., Macrae, C.N. & Banaji, M.R. (2006) Dissociable medial prefrontal contributions to judgments of similar and dissimilar others. Neuron, 50, 655–663.

Molnar-Szakacs, I., Iacoboni, M., Koski, L. & Mazziotta, J.C. (2005) Functional segregation within pars opercularis of the inferior frontal gyrus: evidence from fMRI studies of imitation and action observation. Cereb. Cortex, 15, 986–994.

Mort, D.J., Malhotra, P., Mannan, S.K., Rorden, C., Pambakian, A., Kennard, C. & Husain, M. (2003) The anatomy of visual neglect. Brain, 126, 1986– 1997.

Musso, M., Moro, A., Glauche, V., Rijinties, M., Reichenback, J., Buchel, C. & Weiller, C. (2003) Broca’s area and the language instinct. Nat. Neurosci., 6, 774–781.

Nelissen, K., Luppino, G., Vanduffel, W., Rizzolatti, G. & Orban, G.A. (2005) Observing others: multiple action representation in the frontal lobe. Science, 310, 332–336.

Nichols, T., Brett, M., Andersson, J., Wager, T. & Poline, J.B. (2005) Valid conjunction inference with the minimum statistic. Neuroimage, 25, 653–660. Oakley, K.P. (1954) Skill as a human possession. In Singer, C., Holmyard, E.J. & Hall, A.R. (Eds), A History of Technology. Volume I, From Early Times to Fall of Ancient Empires. Clarendon press, Oxford, pp. 1–37.

Pandya, D.N. & Seltzer, B. (1982) Intrinsic connections and architectonics of posterior parietal cortex in the rhesus monkey. J. Comp. Neurol., 204, 196– 210.

Passingham, R.E. (2008) What is Special about the Human Brain? Oxford University Press, Oxford; New York.

Peeters, R., Simone, L., Nelissen, K., Fabbri-Destro, M., Vanduffel, W., Rizzolatti, G. & Orban, G.A. (2009) The representation of tool use in humans and monkeys: common and uniquely human features. J. Neurosci., 29, 11523–11539.

Pelegrin, J. (1990) Prehistoric lithic technology: some aspects of research. Archaeol. Rev. Cambridge, 9, 116–125.

Pelegrin, J. (2005) Remarks about archaeological techniques and methods of knapping: elements of a cognitive approach to stone knapping. In Roux, V. & Bril, B. (eds), Stone Knapping: The Necessary Conditions for a Uniquely Human Behaviour. McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research, Cam-bridge, pp. 23–34.

Penny, W. & Holmes, A. (2003) Chapter 12, Random effect analysis. In Ashburner, J., Friston, K. & Penny, W. (Eds), Human Brain Function, 2nd edn, Academic Press, London, pp. 843–850.

Petrides, M. (2005) Lateral prefrontal cortex: architectonic and functional organization. Philos. Trans. R Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci., 360, 781–795. Ploran, E.J., Nelson, S.M., Velanova, K., Donaldson, D.I., Petersen, S.E. &

Wheeler, M.E. (2007) Evidence accumulation and the moment of recogni-tion: dissociating perceptual recognition processes using fMRI. J. Neurosci., 27, 11912–11924.

Povinelli, D.J., Reaux, J.E. & Frey, S.H. (2010) Chimpanzees’ context-dependent tool use provides evidence for separable representations of hand and tool even during active use within peripersonal space. Neuropsycholo-gia, 48, 243–247.

Preuss, T.M. & Goldman-Rakic, P.S. (1989) Connections of the ventral granular frontal cortex of macaques with perisylvian premotor and somato-sensory areas: anatomical evidence for somatic representation in primate frontal association cortex. J. Comp. Neurol., 282, 293–316.

Rizzolatti, G. & Craighero, L. (2004) The mirror-neuron system. Annu. Rev. Neurosci., 27, 169–192.

(11)

Rosenthal, C.R., Roche-Kelly, E.E., Husain, M. & Kennard, C. (2009) Response-dependent contributions of human primary motor cortex and angular gyrus to manual and perceptual sequence learning. J. Neurosci., 29, 15115–15125.

Roux, V. & Bril, B. (Eds) (2005) Stone Knapping: The Necessary Conditions for a Uniquely Hominin Behaviour. McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research, Cambridge.

Saxe, R. (2005) Against simulation: the argument from error. Trends Cogn. Sci., 9, 174–179.

Semaw, S., Roger, M.J., Quade, J., Renne, P.R., Butler, R.F., Dominguez-Rodrigo, M., Stout, D., Hart, W.S., Pickering, T. & Simpson, S.W. (2003) 2.6-Million-year-old stone tools and associated bones from 6 and OGS-7, Gona, Afar, Ethiopia. J. Hum. Evol., 45, 169–177.

Stout, D. (2002) Skill and cognition in stone tool production: an ethnographic case study from Irian Jaya. Curr. Anthropol., 45, 693–722.

Stout, D. (2010) The evolution of cognitive control. Top. Cogn. Sci., 2, 614–630. Stout, D. (2011) Stone toolmaking and the evolution of human culture and

cognition. Philos. Trans. R Soc. Lond. B, 366, 1050–1059.

Stout, D. & Chaminade, T. (2007) The evolutionary neuroscience of tool making. Neuropsychologia, 45, 1091–1100.

Stout, D., Toth, N., Schick, K.D. & Chaminade, T. (2008) Neural correlates of Early Stone Age tool-making: technology, language and cognition in human evolution. Philos. Trans. R Soc. Lond. B, 363, 1939–1949.

Tehovnik, E.J., Sommer, M.A., Chou, I.H., Slocum, W.M. & Schiller, P.H. (2000) Eye fields in the frontal lobes of primates. Brain Res. Brain Res. Rev., 32, 413–448.

Tennie, C., Call, J. & Tomasello, M. (2009) Ratcheting up the ratchet: on the evolution of cumulative culture. Philos. Trans. R Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci., 364, 2405–2415.

Tomasello, M. (1999) The Cultural Origins of Human Cognition. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA.

Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T. & Moll, H. (2005) Understanding and sharing intentions: the origins of cultural cognition. Behav. Brain Sci., 28, 675–691.

Umilta`, M.A., Escola, L., Intskirveli, I., Grammont, F., Rochat, M., Caruana, F., Jezzini, A., Gallese, V. & Rizzolatti, G. (2008) When pliers become fingers in the monkey motor system. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci., 105, 2209–2213. de Vignemont, F. & Haggard, P. (2008) Action observation and execution: what

is shared? Soc. Neurosci., 3, 421–433.

Vogt, S., Buccino, G., Wohlschlager, A.M., Canessa, N., Shah, N.J., Zilles, K., Eickhoff, S.B., Freund, H.-J., Rizzolatti, G. & Fink, G.R. (2007) Prefrontal involvement in imitation learning of hand actions: effects of practice and expertise. NeuroImage, 37, 1371–1383.

Volle, E., Kinkingnehun, S., Pochon, J.-B., Mondon, K., Thiebaut de Schotten, M., Seassau, M., Duffau, H., Samson, Y., Dubois, B. & Levy, R. (2008) The functional architecture of the left posterior and lateral prefrontal cortex in humans. Cereb. Cortex, 18, 2460–2469.

Whiten, A., Spiteri, A., Horner, V., Bonnie, K.E., Lambeth, S.P., Schapiro, S.J. & Waal, F.B.M.d. (2007) Transmission of multiple traditions within and between chimpanzee groups. Curr. Biol., 17, 1038–1043.

Whittaker, J.C. (1994) Flintknapping: Making and Understanding Stone Tools. University of Texas Press, Austin.

Wolpert, L. (2003) Causal belief and the origins of technology. Philos. Trans. R Soc. Lond. A, 361, 1709–1719.

Wynn, T. (1979) The intelligence of later acheulean hominids. Man, 14, 371–391.

Wynn, T. & McGrew, W. (1989) An ape’s view of the Oldowan. Man, 24, 383–398.

Figure

Table 1. Frequency of technological actions in fMRI stimuli
Fig. 1. Left: local brain activity in Toolmaking–Control irrespective of subject expertise (FDR P &lt; 0.05, extent k &gt; 20)
Fig. 2. Local brain activity in Toolmaking–Control for Naı¨ve (left), Trained (centre) and Expert (right) subjects (FDR P &lt; 0.05, extent k &gt; 20).
Table 3. Brain activity in response of the observation of Acheulean compared with Oldowan toolmaking stimuli, common to the three groups (minimum statistic conjunction) and by subject expertise (exclusive masking)

References

Related documents

Industrial Emissions Directive, supplemented by horizontal legislation (e.g., Framework Directives on Waste and Water, Emissions Trading System, etc) and guidance on operating

Activity 2: Optimisation of data acquisition with Mobile Mapping Systems ..9. Activity 4: How well can critical underground structures be mapped using Ground

Table V reveals a positive association between the technological disruptiveness of startups (using our text-based patent measure) and these ex-post mentions of market disruption

46 Konkreta exempel skulle kunna vara främjandeinsatser för affärsänglar/affärsängelnätverk, skapa arenor där aktörer från utbuds- och efterfrågesidan kan mötas eller

Data från Tyskland visar att krav på samverkan leder till ökad patentering, men studien finner inte stöd för att finansiella stöd utan krav på samverkan ökar patentering

This is the concluding international report of IPREG (The Innovative Policy Research for Economic Growth) The IPREG, project deals with two main issues: first the estimation of

Syftet eller förväntan med denna rapport är inte heller att kunna ”mäta” effekter kvantita- tivt, utan att med huvudsakligt fokus på output och resultat i eller från

Närmare 90 procent av de statliga medlen (intäkter och utgifter) för näringslivets klimatomställning går till generella styrmedel, det vill säga styrmedel som påverkar