• No results found

Running lights : Conspicuity, glare and accident reduction. ( Transport Ergonomics Conf., Birmingham, Feb 1978)

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "Running lights : Conspicuity, glare and accident reduction. ( Transport Ergonomics Conf., Birmingham, Feb 1978)"

Copied!
24
0
0

Loading.... (view fulltext now)

Full text

(1)

Nr43 - 1979 _ ' _ : , ,. * Statens Qäga oéhf'tr'afikinsfitm(VTI)L- Fack 5581 O1, Linköping

. . - . a . 1 g » __ _,Nationalkoad &?Trafficheseafch Institute-Fack-58.101Linköping SWedén

Paperpresented tid-the_iTranSPO PEE'irgomi icsf

(2)
(3)

:B'ritainziFebrua-Nr 43 ' 1979

43

]

Statens väg- och trafikinstitut (VTI) - Fack - 581 O1 Linköping National Road & Traffic Research Institute - Fack - 581 O1 Linköping Sweden

Running lights conspicuity, glare

and accident reduction

by Kåre Rumar

Paper presented to the Transport Ergonomics

(4)
(5)

FOREWORD

In the 60:ies it was quite a discussion in USA concer-ning the possibility to improve safety by using day light running lights. The Greyhound Bus Company was one

of the pioneers to use running light as a safety

measure .

The idea was taken up concerning motorcycles in many countries. But it is so far only in Sweden and Finland that running lights are compulsory for both automobiles and motorcycles. Canada and Norway are showing conside

rable interest.

This report presents the research arguments behind the Nordic efforts and actions concerning running lights.

It reviews and reports behavioural studies and accident studies carried out in Sweden and Finland as well as in other countries. The effect on accidents of the laws passed is presently being followed up.

(6)

CONTENTS

Page

Background 1

Detection of cars in rural traffic 2

Experimental studies of vehicle detection 4

Running light effect on accidents 8

Conclusions 10

(7)

Background

It is more or less a truism to state that the main part of the information collected by road users in traffic is of visual nature. It is likewise generally accepted that a considerable part of the traffic accidents are caused by the fact that the road users do not detect

each other in time.

The visual system is constructed to react to contrasts and contrast changes in the visual field. Blackwell

(1946) made thorough studies of the contrast

sensiti-vity. He showed that if the brightness contrast between a target and its background is lowered also the proba bility for detection of the target is diminished. The periphery of the eye is less sensitive to contrasts than the central parts. When the ambient illumination levels are lowered (dawn, dusk, twilight) the contrast sensitivity of the eye is diminished. On top of these deteriorations situations often occur when the illumi nation in the visual field is very unevenly distributed. This may lead to misadaptation (bright sky, dark road situation) or to glare (low sun). Such situations further lower the visual contrast sensitivity.

The ambient illumination is consequently very important for the contrast conditions. The ambient illumination is due to the sun's altitude and the weather. The sun's altitude is a function of latitude and season while weather has a more complex background.

The closer to the poles the longer are the twilight periods as are the periods with low sun and generally low ambient illumination. The twilight period in June is e.g. in Stockholm (6OON) 215 minutes and in Rome

(4OON) 65 minutes. In December the ambient illumination

(8)

With this as a background it is natural that the

inte-rest for daylight running light to increase visual contrast between the vehicle and the background has been met with special interest in the Nordic countries and in Canada. A special working group within the Nordic Road Safety Council is studying the problem and has produced two reports concerning running lights on

motorcycles (NTR 1975) and on cars (NTR 1976).

In the following a series of studies of daylight running lights is reported. In the first part the detection con ditions in real rural traffic are observed. In the

second part vehicle detection in the peripheral and

central visual field is studied as a function of running light characteristics and level of ambient illumination. In the third part the effect of daylight running light on accident statistics is analyzed based on Finnish data. Finally conclusions are drawn and the present

status is described.

Detection of cars in rural traffic

Dahlstedt & Rumar (1973) studied the conditions charac teristics at the moment of detection of oncoming

vehicles in rural daylight driving in Sweden. The

subjects were driving along normal two lane roads in

central Sweden. As soon as they detected an oncoming

vehicle they pressed a button which released the shutter of a telelens equipped camera with colour film and

started a tape recorder. In the tape recorder the driver verbally described what he experienced to be the cause to detection. The experiments were done in the 60:ies when very few cars had low beams on during daylight. On

the basis of the photos and the recordings the causes

to detection during summer and winter conditions were

(9)

Summer (no low beam cars)

o\

0

Brightness contrast 52 (70% positive, 30% negative)

o\

0

Colour contrast

Reflexes % (in paint, crome or glass surface)

Silhouette 3% (normally towards sky)

Motion 8%

100%

Winter (with snow on the ground)

Brightness contrast 40% (40% positive, 60% negative)

Low beam 25% (= all cars with low beam) Reflexes l7%

Silhouette l4% (sky plus snow fields and haze)

Motion 4%

100%

These observations show the importance of brightness contrast generally and of low beams as running lights specificly.

Based on these observations a systematic study of car conspicuity as a function of colour of car and back-ground and low beams on/off was carried out. The con spicuity criteria used was primarily detection time of a mock up vehicle with various colours as photographed

on roads against various backgrounds. Besides these

laboratory measurements also some subjective conspicuity

ratings were done.

In short the results showed very strong interactions

between colour of vehicle and colour of background. The best conspicuity is obtained with different car colours depending on the colour of the background. But and this was maybe the most interesting result - a conspi

(10)

cuity equal with the one obtained with the best colour

in each situation could be obtained for all colours if the vehicle had lights on. However, since this was a

laboratory experiment the validity is not known.

Experimental studies of vehicle detection

Allen & Clerk (1964), King & Finch (1969) and Hörberg & Rumar (1975) have studied central conspicuity as a function of ambient illumination level and running light intensity. The conclusion is that when fixated also running lights with low intensities ( lOO cd) increase vehicle conspicuity - especially under levels

of low ambient illumination.

From behavioural and safety point of view it can be argued however the most interesting detection situation is not the central but the peripheral one. The main motives are that when fixated also very low contrasts

are detected (also vehicles without running lights). In other words when vehicles appear where they are

expected, where road users focus their attention the effect of running lights is probably very limited, vehicles are easily detected anyway and no accidents occur. But when vehicles appear where they are not expected they are not fixated but appear in the peri pheral field of vision where contrast sensitivity is considerably lower. In such situations the accident risk is high and the favourable effect of daylight running lights should be good.

Hörberg & Rumar (1975) have studied systematically the peripheral (300 and 600) detection distance of various

running lights (intensity, colour, area) at various

levels of ambient illumination (bad daylight, twilight). Figure l shows the experimental set up and figure 2

(11)

shows the results in summary.

VISUAL

QSK

/

APPROACHlNG

/

CAR

SUBJECT

,Åx

~

PERlPHERAL ANGLE

ow-

<

J

Figure l. Experimetal set up in the daylight test of peripheral vehicle detection distance. The visual task consisted of successively

projected letters and digits that the subject should read

The angles chosen can be taken as representative for the position of rear view mirrors or for the attention angles normal in urban traffic (street crossings).

The running lights testedvm xaspecial running lights of 50 and 150 cd each, normal halogen Continental European low beam of 400 cd each and high beam of

60 0002 cd each. The luminous areas used for the lower

intensities were 70 and 200 cm2 each.

The results show that in 300 and 600 peripheral vision the lower intensities (50 and 150 cd) do not seem to have any effect. At those levels luminous areas do not seem to influence conspicuity. At 300 the low beam has a considerable effect and the high beam is very effec

(12)

30

600

400-

50

200

*-DE

TE

CT

IO

N

DI

ST

AN

CE

(m

)

%

OLH! ' '

'

50 150 400

60000

LOG LIGHT INTENSITY(cd)

A 70 cm2 SURFACE

0 200 cm2 SURFACE

a NO LIGHT ON THE CAR

Figure 2. Mean vehicle detection distance (m) in day

light for two peripheral angels as a function

of running light intensity (cd) and surface area (cm )

In some special studies it was shown that although yellow colour was rated as having better conspicuity

than white light no real difference could be measured

at the experiments. It was further indicated as a side result that some glare problems occurred with the high beams also under the general ambient daylight (sky) levels prevalent during the experiments (3 000 6 000 lx).

(13)

compared at 6 000 lx ambient illumination and at 300 peripheral vision. The running light of the motorcycle was low beam (ca 250 cd). Without running lights the detection distance for the motorcycle was 83 meters and for the car 175 meters. When the motorcycle used low beam the detection distance increased to 183 meters

roughly equal to that of the car without running lights. Janoff et al (1970) report that the number of car drivers reporting having detected an oncoming motor-cycle increased two to three times when the motormotor-cycle

had its low beam on.

Hörberg (1977) in a special study of peripheral (200)

detection distances under twilight conditions (100

2 000 lx) reports that under no snow conditions even

such a weak running light as 100 cd has considerable effect at ambient illumination levels lower than about 800 lx. In snow conditions on the other hand even a 300 cd running light has no marked effect above about 600 lx ambient illumination and a 100 cd running light has no clear effect until the ambient illumination is

lower than about 350 lx.

Attwood & Angus (1975) and Attwood (1976) in Canada have shown that against some hypotheses the possibilities

to determine the position on the road of an oncoming

careuxaimproved by turned on low beams. They also

report that according to perceptual laws a vehicle with

lights is estimated to be somewhat closer to the obser ver than a vehicle without lights. The "lights on" condition on oncoming cars resulted in somewhat safer

(longer) marginals in overtaking situation. Also possibilities correctly to judge motion direction of distant vehicles should be improved by running lights.

It is generally accepted that in night driving within at least well stationary lighted areas oncoming low

(14)

beams deteriorate the visual conditions. But on the other hand parking lights are often not conspicuous enough. Also here a running light could be a solution

this is the so called city beam. But could the day

light running light also be used as a night driving

city beam? In order to study this problem Hörberg & Ru

mar (1975) carried out a series of experiments. The results show that the glare effect (detection distance

of a dark obstacle) of a 150 cd city beam is only about half that of a low beam. On the other hand peripheral

(800) detection distance of a car with low beam is

about 50% longer than that of a car with 150 cd city beam. In conclusion it could be said that if it were not for the visibility of retroflective materials on

road markings, road signs, vehicles and unprotected

road users the city beam should have an intensity which is considerable lower than that of a daylight running light. This has also been suggested by several authors

(e.g. Balder 1957, Dahlstedt 1973, Fisher 1974, Hisdal

1973, Jehn 1963, Nielsen 1974, Schreuder 1974).

Running light effect on accidents

In the 60:ies several more or less controlled studies of the effect of running lights on accidents were carried out in USA. The best known is probably the Greyhound bus study from 1962. The company reported at lO% decrease of accidents. Allen (1965) reports from a questionnaire study of about 50 companies that the

accident reduction for vehicles with running lights was close to 40%. Cantilli (1968) studies the running light accident effect of vehicles belonging to Port of New York Authority and reports a reduction of 18%. Allen (1970) states that a Chicago taxi company obtained an

accident reduction of 12%. Janoff et al (1970) in the

probably best controlled accident study of running lights so far reports about experience from four USA

(15)

states with compulsory running lights for motor cycles

and concludes that the accident reduction for motor cycles due to running lights was 4%. Attwood (1975) describes a large Canadian study in progress with 350 matched cars. In this study the accident effect as well as the practical, technological and economical effects

will be studied.

But the most recent, largest and best controlled

accident study is the one carried out by Andersson, Nilsson & Salusjärvi (1976) on Finnish data 1968 1974. As a result of successive measures taken in Finland

(campaigns 1968 1970, recommendation 1970 1972,

obligation 1972 1974) the frequency of cars using low

beam during daylight in rural areas during the winter

half year has successively increased so that during the compulsory period it was 93 99%. The basic

hypothesis in the study is that during the winter half"

year running lights should affect multiple accidents

in daylights, while other accident types should not be affected and could consequently be used as controls. The control groups used are multiple accidents in

darkness, single accidents in daylight, single accidents

in darkness.

For these four groups a regression analysis was made for the period 1968 1974. The results show

Multiple accidents in daylight decreased by 32% Multiple accidents in darkness decreased by 4%

Single accidents in daylight decreased by 4%

s

w

a

I

J

Single accidents in darkness increased by 6%

(16)

lO

By calculating the ratio

number of daylight multiple accidents & = number of darkness multiple accidents

number of daylight single accidents

the effect of measures having general influence,

multiple/single influence and daylight/darkness

influence on acccidents may be eliminated. With this method of analysis a reduction of daylight multiple accidents of 36% during the period 1968-1974 is

reported. A more detailed analysis of accident types

shows that accidents with meeting direction decreased

by 28%, accidents with crossing direction decreased by l7% and accidents with the same direction increased by 9%. Also daylight pedestrian accidents decreased during the period (24%).

The study which was initiated by the above mentioned

Working Group within The Nordic Road Safety Council

concludes that although accident analyses such as this one always are difficult due to other measures and variation in external conditions the results strongly

indicate the favourable effect of running lights.

Conclusions

In Sweden the use of running lights during daylight was considerable especially in lowered daylight

conditions (e.g. twilight, bad weather). In the winter

half 1975/76 the usage of running lights in rural areas

in Sweden was studied by Sävenhed (1977). He reports that on the average 3 cars out of 4 use low beams or special running lights. In poor illumination conditions the usage is above 90% while in good illumination

conditions 1 of 2 cars have lights on.

(17)

ll

from October 1, 1977, low beam or special running

lights are compulsory in Sweden during daylight. As mentioned above Finland already has the law, Denmark has it for motorcycles (as several other countries e.g. France) and Norway is expected to follow in 1979.

Two problems concerning the running lights remain to be solved. The first one is the optimal characteristics of a running light. A special Swedish committee is

presently drafting the suggestions for a standard. The main question is the intensity interval. The present proposal is 300 - 800 cd. In this context the integra-tion with a city beam is also discussed but no

conclusion has been made.

The other problem concerns the vehicles for which

running lights are presently not compulsory (e.g.

tractors, mOpeds). It is a reasonable hypothesis that the conspicuity for those vehicle categories will be relatively lowered when a majority of the vehicles in traffic use running lights. The above mentioned Working Group within The Nordic Road Safety Council is

(18)

12

List of references

ALLEN, M.J., CLARK, J.R. (1964): Automobile running lights A research report, Am. Jour. of

Optometry and Archives of Am. Acad. of Optometry, Vol 41, No.5, May.

ALLEN, M.J. (1965): Running light questionnaire, Am. Jour. of Optometry and Archives of Am. Acad. of

Optometry, Vol 42, No.3, March.

ALLEN, M.J. (1970): Vision and Highway Safety, Chilton Book Co., USA.

ANDERSSON, K., NILSSON, G., SALUSJÄRVI, M. (1976):

Effect on road accidents by recommended and

compulsory running light in Finland. (In Swedish) Rapport 102, National Swedish Road & Traffic

Research Institute, Linköping. Sweden.

ATTWOOD. D.A. (1975): Daytime Running Lights Project. I: Research Program and Preliminary Results. Canadian Ministry of Transport, Road Safety Unit Report RSU 75/1. II: Vehicle Detection as a Function of Headlight Use and Ambient

Illumination, Canadian Ministry of Transport, Road Safety Unit Report RSU 75/2, Toronto,

Canada.

ATTWOOD, D.A., ANGUS, R.G. (1975): Daytime Running

Lights Project. III: Pilot Validation Study of a Fields Detection Experiment, Canadian Ministry of Transport, Road Safety Unit Report RSU 75/3, Toronto, Canada.

ATTWOOD, D.A. (1976): Daytime Running Lights Project. IV: Two lane passing performance as a function of headlight intensity and ambient illumination, Canadian Ministry of Transport, Road Safety Unit Report RSU 76/1, Toronto, Canada.

BALDER, I.J.J. (1957): Side lights on well lit roads,

International Road Safety and Traffic Review,

Vol. V, No.2, Spring.

BLACKWELL, H.R. (1946): Contrast thresholds of the

human eye. Journal of the Optical Society of

America, 36, 624.

CANTILLI, B.J. (1968): Accident experience with parking lights as running lights, Paper sponsored by Committee on Highway Safety and presented at

(19)

13

DAHLSTEDT, S. (1973): Comparison of town beam and low beam concerning pedestrian visibility on

stationary illuminated streets. (In Swedish) Rapport 24, The National Swedish Road Safety

Office, Stockholm, Sweden. ,

DAHLSTEDT & RUMAR (1973): Vehicle colour and front conspicuity in some simulated rural traffic

situations, Department of Psychology, University of Uppsala, Sweden.

FISHER, A.J. (1974): The Luminous Intensity Require

ments of Vehicle Front Lights for Use in Towns, Ergonomics, Vol 17, No. 1, 87.

HISDAL, B (1973): Running light (In Norwegian), Central

Institute for Industrial Research Oslo Blindern, Norway.

HÖRBERG & RUMAR (1975): Running lights conspicuity and glare, Report 178, Department of Psychology,

University of Uppsala, Sweden.

HÖRBERG, U. (1977): Running light twilight conspicuity

and distance judgement. Report 215, Department of Psychology, University of Uppsala, Sweden.

JANOFF, M.S., CASSEL, A., FERTNER, K.S., SMIERCIAK,

E.S. (1970): Daytime motorcycle headlight and taillight operation, The Franklin Institute Research Laboratories, final report. Prepared for National Highway Safety Bureau, US Depart ment of Transportation, Washington.

JEHU, V.J. (1963): A Dimmed Headlight System, Bull. Mot. Ind. Res. Ass.

KING, L.E. & FINCH, D.M. (1969): Daytime Running

Lights, Highway Research Record, No. 275. NIELSEN, B. (1974): Visibility obtained by use of

reduced low beam on stationary illuminated

roads (In Danish). Lysteknisk Laboratorium,

Lyngby, Denmark.

Nordisk Road Safety Council (1975): Running lights for motorcycles. (In Swedish) Rapport 12, Stock holm, Sweden.

Nordic Road Safety Council (1976): Running lights lights on cars during daylight. (In Swedish) Rapport 17, Stockholm, Sweden.

SCHREUDER, D.A. (1974): Vehicle lighting within built up areas. (Shortened version, Stencil).

Institute for Road Safety Research. SWOV. Voorburg, Netherlands.

(20)

l4

SÄVENHED, H. (1977): Use of vehicle lighting in Sweden

under different daylight levels during the winter l975/76. (In Swedish) Meddelande 37,

National Swedish Road & Traffic Research Institute, Linköping, Sweden.

(21)
(22)
(23)
(24)

Figure

Figure l. Experimetal set up in the daylight test of peripheral vehicle detection distance
Figure 2. Mean vehicle detection distance (m) in day light for two peripheral angels as a function of running light intensity (cd) and surface area (cm )

References

Related documents

Reduced MPFC activity can be found in a variety of dehumanizing contexts, such as when sexist men look at pictures of scantily clad women (Cikara et al., 2011), when people shoot

Målet med detta arbete var att göra en översikt av forskningen kring mikro- typografins inverkan på läsbarheten, med fokus på de resultat som upp- nåtts och de metoder

(2018) did two stepwise incremental tests with which they estimated the IAT; one at the 0.4% inclined treadmill and one on the running track. They then tested the correlation of

We have presented four different components and situations where the running identity can be examined by studying runners’ (1) perception of their own membership of the

MSCs (mesenchymal stem cells) have been used in the setting of cell therapy but are not believed to be able to migrate through the blood circulation. EPCs are believed to be at

ANOVA with repeated measure revealed a significant difference between the up- and downhill sections in the first and second lap respectively for oxygen uptake (both p&lt;0.01),

Analysis of single nucleotide polymorphisms by apyrase mediated allele-specific extension paper VII Allele-specific extension ASE methodology using alternating primers at the

En psykoterapeut beskrev att den ibland upplevt att det finns en ilska och besvikelsemur som byggts upp gentemot förälder, vilken kan brytas genom att man börjar prata om det