• No results found

Sources of stress and worry in the development of stress-related mental health problems: A longitudinal investigation from early- to mid-adolescence

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "Sources of stress and worry in the development of stress-related mental health problems: A longitudinal investigation from early- to mid-adolescence"

Copied!
15
0
0

Loading.... (view fulltext now)

Full text

(1)

http://www.diva-portal.org

This is the published version of a paper published in Anxiety, Stress, and Coping.

Citation for the original published paper (version of record):

Anniko, M K., Boersma, K., Tillfors, M. (2018)

Sources of stress and worry in the development of stress-related mental health

problems: A longitudinal investigation from early- to mid-adolescence

Anxiety, Stress, and Coping, 32(2): 155-167

https://doi.org/10.1080/10615806.2018.1549657

Access to the published version may require subscription.

N.B. When citing this work, cite the original published paper.

© 2018 The Author(s). Published by Informa UK Limited, trading as Taylor & Francis

Group This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons

Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives License (http:// creativecommons.org/licenses/

by-nc-nd/4.0/), which permits non-commercial re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any

medium, provided the original work is properly cited, and is not altered, transformed, or built

upon in any way.

Permanent link to this version:

(2)

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at

https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=gasc20

Anxiety, Stress, & Coping

An International Journal

ISSN: 1061-5806 (Print) 1477-2205 (Online) Journal homepage: https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/gasc20

Sources of stress and worry in the development

of stress-related mental health problems: A

longitudinal investigation from early- to

mid-adolescence

M. K. Anniko, K. Boersma & M. Tillfors

To cite this article: M. K. Anniko, K. Boersma & M. Tillfors (2019) Sources of stress and worry in the development of stress-related mental health problems: A longitudinal investigation from early- to mid-adolescence, Anxiety, Stress, & Coping, 32:2, 155-167, DOI: 10.1080/10615806.2018.1549657

To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/10615806.2018.1549657

© 2018 The Author(s). Published by Informa UK Limited, trading as Taylor & Francis Group

Published online: 22 Nov 2018.

Submit your article to this journal Article views: 2013

View related articles View Crossmark data

(3)

Sources of stress and worry in the development of stress-related

mental health problems: A longitudinal investigation from

early-to mid-adolescence

M. K. Annikoa, K. Boersmaaand M. Tillfors b

a

School of Law, Psychology and Social Work, Örebro University, Örebro, Sweden;bFaculty of Arts and Social Sciences, Department of Social and Psychological Studies: Psychology, Karlstad University, Karlstad, Sweden

ABSTRACT

Background and Objectives: Stress and stress-related mental health complaints are common and increasing among adolescents, especially girls. Identifying typical sources of stress as well as central intervention targets is an important effort in the development of effective prevention and treatment protocols. This study investigated worry as potential mediator in the development of mental health problems in response to common stressors in adolescence. We also examined to what sources adolescents ascribe their stress over the years from the 7th through the 9th grade.

Design: Prospective cohort study.

Methods: Self-reported subjective stressor load, worry, anxiety and depressive symptoms were assessed in a sample of Swedish 7th graders (N = 1137; 46% girls, mean age 13.2) with follow-up assessments one and two years later.

Results: School was the most common source of stress across all time-points, with girls reporting considerable more stress than boys. Worry mediated the relationship between overall stressor load and depressive symptoms and anxiety over time and was not moderated by gender. Conclusions: Worry may be an important target in stress prevention and efforts to prevent stress-related problems would benefit from focusing on early adolescence as especially school stress is already relatively common in grade 7. ARTICLE HISTORY Received 31 July 2017 Revised 26 October 2018 Accepted 5 November 2018 KEYWORDS

Stress; adolescence; worry; anxiety; depressive symptoms

Adolescent stress and stress-related mental health complaints are common and have been increasing over the past decades. Especially girls in mid- to late adolescence report high levels of stress and stress-related symptoms such as anxiety, depressive symptoms, sleeping difficulties and somatic complaints (MacLean et al.,2013; Ravens-Sieberer et al.,2009; Sweeting, Young, & West,2009). Study-ing the sources to which adolescents ascribe their stress as well as cognitive mechanisms drivStudy-ing the development of increases in stress-related mental health problems is an important effort as it will provide insight into possible targets for preventive intervention. Indeed, recent treatment research efforts are starting to direct more attention to adapting CBT treatment models to the prevention of mental health problems in adolescents. For example Topper, Emmelkamp, Watkins, and Ehring (2017) successfully adapted rumination focused CBT to prevent the development of anxiety disorders and depression in adolescents and young adults. However, more research is needed to delineate

© 2018 The Author(s). Published by Informa UK Limited, trading as Taylor & Francis Group

This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives License (http:// creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/), which permits non-commercial re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited, and is not altered, transformed, or built upon in any way.

CONTACT M. K. Anniko malin.anniko@oru.se ANXIETY, STRESS, & COPING

2019, VOL. 32, NO. 2, 155–167

(4)

common stressors in adolescence and the processes driving the development of increases in mental health problems. More data on this will give us better knowledge on when to intervene and who and what we should target in preventive interventions.

When asked, adolescents often report that school (e.g., academic demands) is one of the major sources of stress (de Anda et al., 2000; Östberg et al., 2015). But also social stressors, such as conflicts with parents, fitting in with peers and handling romantic relationships increase in both fre-quency and relevance in adolescence, especially for girls (Rudolph & Hammen,1999; Stange, Hamil-ton, Abramson, & Alloy, 2014). Although the link between specific stressor domains and mental health outcomes is well established, fewer studies have investigated the relative importance of different stressor domains longitudinally. Such a focus on the relative importance of specific stressor domains may inform decisions as to when and where to intervene with preventive efforts. Given the transitional nature of adolescence, different domains of stress may be more or less salient depending on time. Adolescence is often divided into early adolescence (10–13), middle adolescence (14–17), and late adolescence (18 until early twenties) (Smetana, Campione-Barr, & Metzger, 2006). For example, conflicts with parents tend to increase somewhat in early adolescence when new bound-aries and responsibilities are negotiated (Laursen, Coy, & Collins,1998) whereas stress due to roman-tic relationships likely becomes more important later in adolescence when such relationships increase in frequency (Collins,2003). Knowledge of which stressor domains become prominent at what time-point in development is therefore an important research target. In this study the focus was on early- to mid-adolescence as this may be a crucial time to study the development of stress-related problems given the increase of stressors at the onset of adolescence (Arnett, 1999; Larson & Ham,1993) and the increases of stress-related mental health problems in mid- to late ado-lescence (Avenevoli, Swendsen, He, Burnstein, & Merikangas,2015; Larson & Ham,1993; Merikangas et al.,2010).

Another crucial question for stress prevention concerns who and what we should target in preven-tion. After all, although most adolescents experience school- and social stress at some point not all develop stress-related mental health problems (e.g., anxiety and depressive symptoms). Identifying the mechanisms involved in the development of mental health problems in response to stress is thus an important focus of research. A better understanding of mechanisms could assist in both iden-tifying individuals at risk for developing such problems and in developing effective preventive inter-ventions. One potentially central mechanism is the tendency to worry (Brosschot, Gerin, & Thayer,

2006). Worry is a form of repetitive negative thinking which revolves around future events for which the outcome is uncertain but hold the potential of being negative (Sibrava & Borkovec,

2006). Worry is commonly conceptualized as a form of cognitive avoidance where motives often involves to prepare for potential threat and negative outcomes (Borkovec & Roemer,1995; Sibrava & Borkovec,2006). By worrying about an upcoming stressful event (e.g., an exam) or the conse-quences of a past event (e.g., an argument with a friend) stressor related emotions, cognitions and physiological responses are prolonged and intensified (Brosschot et al.,2006; Stange et al.,2014; Verkuil, Brosschot, Gebhardt, & Thayer,2010). This suggests that worry might be an important mech-anism driving the relationship between stress and the development of stress-related mental health problems. Indeed worry has empirically been linked to prolonged and intensified anxiety and depression, stress-related physiology and stress-symptoms (Eisma, Boelen, Schut, & Stroebe,2017; Ottaviani et al.,2016; Roussis & Wells,2008; Watkins, 2008). Considering that worry seems to play a role in the maintenance of several of the emotional disorders it is suggested to be a transdiagnostic factor (Harvey, Watkins, Mansell, & Shafran,2004; McEvoy, Watson, Watkins, & Nathan,2013; Watkins,

2008). However, while there is convincing evidence that stressors and worry are both linked to the development of mental health problems such as anxiety and depressive symptoms, there is a need for longitudinal research investigating worry as a mediator between common stressors in ado-lescence and the development of mental health problems. This is important since this might be a crucial period in establishing and shaping specific response styles that may persevere into adulthood. Indeed, perseverative cognitions such as worry and rumination in response to stressors has been

(5)

found to increase substantially in early adolescence, especially in girls (Jose & Brown,2008; Zimmer-Gembeck & Skinner,2011). Thus, if worry would mediate the relationship between stressors and mental health problems, it could be a marker to identify at risk individuals but would also offer a promising target in prevention of stress-related mental health problems.

Thus, the aim of this study was twofold. Thefirst aim of this study was to longitudinally investigate the relative importance of different stressor domains within both the social and the school context over the years from early- to mid-adolescence. More specifically we investigated relative differences in subjective stressor load in the family-, peer-, romantic-, and school performance domains across gender in adolescents from the 7ththrough 9thgrade. The second aim was to investigate the role of worry in the development of stress-related mental health problems over time. We hypothesized that overall subjective stressor load would predict depressive symptoms and anxiety over time and that part of this relationship would be mediated by worry. We further hypothesized that this relationship would be moderated by gender with girls showing a stronger relation between overall stressor load and increases in worry over time than boys.

Methods

Participants and procedure

This study uses a prospective design, following adolescents from 7th to 9th grade with yearly measurement points. The study sample is a sub-sample of participants of a larger project, an ongoing longitudinal study on the development of psychosocial problems in adolescence. The study targets adolescents in 7th and 8th grade enrolled in public secondary schools in three Swedish communities. All 18 schools within the catchment areas of the three communities were included. The study used active consent from adolescents and passive consent from parents to reduce sampling bias (Shaw, Cross, Thomas, & Zubrick,2015). This procedure was approved by the regional ethics board (ref. number 2013/384). Parents of eligible children (N = 3336) received a letter informing about the study. If they did not want their child to participate they were urged to return a notification of this included in the letter using a prepaid return envelope. Parents did not consent to participation of their child in 122 cases (3.6%). A total of 446 students (13.4%) either declined or were absent the day of the data collection, leaving a total sample at baseline of N = 2768 (83%). For this study, only data from the 7th graders were used given that we aimed to inves-tigate subjective stressor load within different stressor domains and the development of stress-related mental health problems from grade 7–9 of the Swedish secondary school. This left a sample of N = 1453 (47.4% female) at baseline with a mean age of 13.19 (SD = .42, range 12–15). The majority of the participants were born in Sweden (88.9%), 15.1% had parents that were born outside of Europe and 33% (n = 476) reported that their parents were divorced or separated. The majority of participants reported that they had a bedroom of their own (89.4%) and that they had been away on holiday with their family at least once over the past 12 months (86.5%) which is in accordance with the national population at the time point of the data collection (Statistics Sweden,2014–2015).

Data was collected in the classrooms during school hours. Trained questionnaire administrators informed each class about the study and its voluntary basis, administered the questionnaires and answered questions when necessary. Teachers were asked to leave the classrooms for confidentiality. Each class received 300 SEK for their class fundraising.

A second and third assessment took place one and two years later following the same procedure. The retention rate was good with 91.9% of the baseline sample participating at the Time 2 assess-ment (n = 1336), and 81.3% (n = 1181) of the baseline sample participating at the Time 3 assessassess-ment. In total, 78.3% (n = 1137, 46% girls) of the baseline sample participated at all three assessment points. Participants who completed only one or none of the follow-up assessments were more likely to have parents that were separated (B = .49, SE = .14, OR = 1.62) or parents that had a non-European back-ground (B = .59, SE = .22, OR = 1.81) than participants who completed all three assessments. They ANXIETY, STRESS, & COPING 157

(6)

were also more likely to report higher levels of subjective stressor load (B = .02, SE = .004, OR = 1.02) and depressive symptoms (B = .02, SE = .01, OR = 1.02). However there were no differences in terms of gender, worry, anxiety or being born in Sweden or not (ps > .10, range .42–.64). Although the overall model was significant (χ2(8) = 79.64, p < .001), due to low Nagelkerke R2(.08) we expect these di ffer-ences to have had a fairly low impact on our results.

Measures

Subjective stressor load

Subjective stressor load was assessed with the Adolescent Stress Questionnaire-Short version (ASQ-S; Anniko, Boersma, van Wijk, Byrne & Tillfors,2018). The ASQ-S is a 27 item shortened version of the 58 item Adolescent Stress Questionnaire-II (ASQ-II; Byrne, Davenport, & Mazanov,2007). The ASQ-S asks respondents to indicate how stressful a range of situations have been for them during the past six months. Each item is rated on a Likert scale ranging from 1 = not at all stressful (or has not happened) to 5 = very stressful. The ASQ-S consists of nine separate domains reflecting stress due to arguments at home, school performance, school attendance, romantic relationships, peer pressure, teacher inter-action, future uncertainty, school/leisure conflict and financial pressure. For the first aim of this study the domains of stress due to arguments at home (4 items; example item“disagreements between you and your mother”), romantic relationships (3 items; example item “making the relationship with you boy/girlfriend work”) and peer pressure (4 items; example item “being hassled for not fitting in”) were selected to capture social stressor load and the domain of school performance (3 items; example item“keeping up with school work”) was selected to capture school-related stress. For the second aim of the study, to investigate worry as a mediator in the relationship between overall stressor load in the 7th grade and stress-related mental health problems in the 9thgrade, the total score of the full ASQ-S was used. Cronbach’s alphas for the four subscales ranged from .77 to .84 at baseline, from .79 to .85 at one year up, and from .84 to .88 at two-year follow-up. For the full ASQ-S Cronbach’s alphas were .93, .93, and .94 respectively.

Depressive symptoms

Depressive symptoms were assessed with The Center for Epidemiology Studies Depression Scale for Children (CES-DC; Weissman, Orvaschel, & Padian,1980). The CES-DC is a reliable and valid measure of depressive symptoms in Swedish adolescents (Olsson & von Knorring,1997). The CES-DC asks respon-dents to indicate how often they have experienced each item statement over the past week. It consist of 20 items with response scales ranging from 0 = Not at all to 3 = A lot. The scale has a score range from 0–60, with higher scores indicating more severe symptoms of depression. Cronbach’s alpha for the CES-DC was .94 at baseline, .91 at one year follow-up and .92 at two year follow-up.

Anxiety

Anxiety was assessed with the Overall Anxiety and Impairment Scale (OASIS; Norman, Hami Cissell, Means-Christensen, & Stein,2006). The OASIS is afive item self-report measure of severity and impair-ment of general anxiety symptoms. The OASIS asks respondents to indicate how often they have experienced each item statement over the past week. Items are rated on a response scale, ranging from 0–4 with different labeling depending on the wording of the item, but with a higher score indi-cating more severe anxiety or impairment (range 0–20). The OASIS has demonstrated sound psycho-metric properties with non-clinical as well as clinical samples (Campbell-Sills et al.,2009; Norman et al.,2006). Cronbach’s alpha was .87 at baseline and .88 at one and two year follow-up.

Worry

Worry was assessed with the Penn State Worry Questionnaire for Children (PSWQ-C; Chorpita, Tracey, Brown, Collica, & Barlow,1997). The PSWQ-C is a 14 item self-report scale assessing trait worry. The PSWQ-C asks respondents to indicate to what degree each item statement is true for them in general

(7)

with no specific time frame given. Items are rated on a response scale, ranging from 0 = Never to 3 = Always. The maximum score is 42, with a higher score indicating higher levels of trait worry. The PSWQ-C has demonstrated good psychometric properties in non-clinical as well as clinical samples of children and adolescents (Chorpita et al.,1997; Esbjørn, Reinholdt-Dunne, Caspersen, Christensen, & Chorpita,2013; Pestle, Chorpita, & Schiffman,2008). In the current sample Cronbach’s alpha was .89 at baseline, .91 at one year follow-up and .92 at two year follow-up.

Data analysis

The analyses were conducted in SPSS version 23. Gender differences and associations between stres-sor load, worry, anxiety and depressive symptoms across the three measurement points were inves-tigated with t-tests and Pearson correlations respectively. Repeated measures analysis of variance (rANOVA) was used to investigate whether differences in symptoms and worry across time points were significant for boys and girls respectively.

Descriptive statistics were used to graph the means for subjective stressor load due to arguments at home, peer pressure, romantic relationships and school performance in the 7th through the 9th grade for boys and girls separately. To facilitate comparison between domains, average responses on the sub-scales were used instead of sum scores. Using the three assessment points we then con-ducted a repeated measures analysis of variance (rANOVA) for each subscale to investigate changes over time.

Two separate mediation models (one with anxiety and one with depressive symptoms as outcome) were then fitted to test the theoretical model of worry as a mediator in the relation between stressor load and emotional symptoms. These analyses were conducted in PROCESS v 2.16 macro (Hayes,2013) for SPSS with 5000 bootstrap samples and heteroscedasticity-consistent standard errors. Subjective stressor load in the 7th grade were entered as predictor of emotional symptoms (depressive symptoms or anxiety) in the 9th grade and worry in the 8th grade was entered as a mediator. Baseline levels of emotional symptoms and worry, were entered as covariates in all steps of the models. Last, gender was added as a moderator of the path from stressor load to worry in a conditional process model to test for moderated mediation. The ratio of the indirect effect to the total effect (PM) was used for effect size as recommended by Wen and Fan (2015). The PMis an

indicator of how much of the total effect the indirect effect accounts for. For example if PM= .47, the

indirect effect accounts for about half of the total effect (Wen & Fan,2015).

Multivariate imputation by chained equations in R (mice; Buuren & Groothuis-Oudshoorn,2011) was used to handle missing data due to internal attrition. In line with Kärnä et al. (2011) a two-step procedure was followed where the quark function in R was initially used to create principal com-ponents. These were then used as auxiliary variables in the mice function in R to impute 100 datasets (see Howard, Rhemtulla, & Little,2015). The average or modal imputed values were then calculated and imputed in the original dataset.

Given our design with participants from 18 different schools, the data of the individuals are nested within schools as well as classes. However as we did not have any a priori multilevel hypothesis and the intra-class correlations were small at both the school (range: .000–.018) and class level (range: .005–.045) we did not proceed with multilevel models.

Results

Descriptive statistics of total stressor load, depressive symptoms, anxiety and worry

Table 1 presents the means and standard deviations of total stressor load, stress-related mental health problems and worry for the total sample and for girls and boys separately. Girls reported sig-nificantly higher levels of stressor load, depressive symptoms, anxiety and worry at all time points. For girls, there was a significant increase in depressive symptoms (F(1.90, 989.61) = 34.04, p < .001, ω2 = .02), anxiety (F(1.97, 1022.39) = 36.2, p < .001,ω2= .02) and worry (F(1.93, 1005.14) = 68.52, p < .001,

(8)

ω2= .04) over the three time-points. Pairwise comparisons between grades (Bonferroni corrected)

showed that depressive symptoms significantly increased from the 7th and the 8th grade (p < .001), but not from the 8th to the 9th grade (p < .10). Anxiety and worry significantly increased at all time points (ps < .01).

For boys, the increase in mean level depressive symptoms (F(1.87, 1152.03) = 59.56, p < .001, ω2= .04), anxiety (F(1.93, 1189.23) = 13.70, ω2= .01), and worry (F(1.95, 1200.44) = 25.92, p < .001,

ω2= .01) was significant. Pairwise comparisons showed that depressive symptoms significantly

increased at all time points (ps < .01), while anxiety increased significantly from the 8th to the 9th grade (p < .01) but not from the 7th to the 8th grade (p > .05). Worry increased significantly from the 7th to the 8th grade (p < .01) but not from the 8th to the 9th grade (p > .90).

Table 2 displays the zero-order correlation coefficients between total subjective stressor load, depressive symptoms, anxiety and worry. All measures were significantly correlated with each other cross-sectionally but also over time with medium to large effect sizes.

Subjective stressor load in the 7th through the 9th grade

Figure 1displays mean subjective stressor load levels in grade 7 through 9. For girls, there was an overall increase in reported stressor load due to arguments at home F(1.90, 985.52) = 37.35, p < .001, ω2= .02, romantic relationships F(1.94, 1009.12) = 13.89, p < .001,ω2= .01, and school per-formance F(1.92, 997.18) = 113.07, p < .001, ω2= .07 while no overall increase was found for stress due to peer pressure F(1.95, 1013.96) = 2.64, p = .07, ω2= .00. Pairwise comparisons between grades (Bonferroni corrected) showed that all increases were significant (ps ≤05).

For boys, there was an increase in mean level reported stress due to arguments at home F(1.93, 1185.10) = 7.94, p < .001,ω2= .01, romantic relationships F(1.90, 1165.16) = 16.55, p < .001,ω2= .01, and school performance F(1.93, 1186.20) = 21.67, p < .001, ω2= .01. No increase was found for stress due to peer pressure F(1.95, 1197.46) = 1.36, p = .26,ω2= .00. Pairwise comparisons (Bonferroni corrected) showed that stressor load due to arguments at home and romantic relationships did not significantly increase from the 7th to the 8th grade (ps > .10) but did significantly increase from the 8th to the 9th grade (p < .05). Stressor load due to school performance significantly increased from the 7th to the 8th grade (p < .001) but not from the 8th to the 9th grade (p > .10).

Worry as a mediator between stressor load and stress-related mental health problems over time

Depressive symptoms

There was a direct effect of overall subjective stressor load in the 7th grade on depressive symptoms in the 9th grade (b = .09, p < .001), controlling for baseline levels of depressive symptoms and worry. There was also an indirect effect through worry b = .02, 95% PCa CI [.009; .041] with an effect size of PM= .21, where the total effect of overall stressor load on subsequent levels of depressive symptoms

was b = .11, 95% PCa CI [.059; .164]. We then added gender as a moderator of the relationship between stressor load and worry in a moderated mediation model (seeFigure 2.). However, the inter-action of gender and stressor load (b =−.01, p = .68) was not significant. Given the gender differences found between levels of subjective stressor load within specific stressor domains and worry in the previous analyses, we ran a second set of moderated mediation models but with subjective stressor load within the specific domains (arguments at home, peer pressure, romantic relationships and school performance) as predictors. Results showed a significant interaction of gender (p < .05) for the model with subjective stressor load due to peer pressure as a predictor, with an indirect effect through worry for girls (b = .17, 95% PCa CI [.05; .31]) but not for boys (b = .02, 95% PCa CI [−.06; .11]). For the other domains the interaction of gender and domain specific stressor load was not sig-nificant (ps > .20). Also, worry was found to mediate the relationship between stress of school per-formance and depressive symptoms for both genders (Girls: b = .17, 95% PCa CI [.059; .290]; Boys:

(9)

b = .10, 95% PCa CI [.014; .187]), but the mediational path between the other two domain specific stressors was not significant as neither stress of arguments at home nor stress of romantic relation-ships predicted increases in worry (ps > .70).

Anxiety

There was a direct effect of subjective stressor load in the 7th grade on anxiety in the 9thgrade (b = .02, p = .01), controlling for baseline levels of anxiety and worry. There was also an indirect effect through worry b = .01, 95% PCa CI [.005; .016] with an effect size of PM= .32, where the total effect

of stressor load on subsequent levels of anxiety was b = .03, 95% PCa CI [.014; .050]. We then added gender as a moderator of the relationship between stressor load and worry (seeFigure 3.). The interaction was not significant (b = −.01, p = .66).

As with depressive symptoms as an outcome, we also ran a second set of moderated mediation models for anxiety and with subjective stressor load within the specific domains as pre-dictors. Results showed a significant interaction of gender (p < .05) for the model with subjective stressor load due to peer pressure as a predictor, with an indirect effect through worry for girls (b = .07, 95% PCa CI [.02; .12]) but not for boys (b = .01, 95% PCa CI [−.03; .04]). For the other domains the interaction of gender and domain specific stressor load was not significant (ps > .10). Worry was found to mediate the relationship between stress of school performance and anxiety for both genders (Girls: b = .07, 95% PCa CI [.024; .114]; Boys: b = .04, 95% PCa CI [.006; .073]), but the mediational path between the other two domain specific stressors was not signifi-cant as neither stress of arguments at home nor stress of romantic relationships predicted increases in worry (ps > .50).

Discussion

Thefirst aim of this study was to prospectively investigate the relative importance of common stres-sors in the social and school context over the years from early to mid-adolescence. Our results show that, although girls in general reported higher levels of stress than boys in both domains, the pattern of salient stressors were similar across gender and grade. School was by far the biggest source of stress, whereas levels of stress due to social factors, such asfitting in with peers and romantic relation-ships, were relatively low at all time-points. The second aim of this study was to investigate the role of worry in the development of stress-related mental health problems. We hypothesized that overall subjective stressor load would predict depressive symptoms and anxiety over time and that part of this relationship would be mediated by worry. We also hypothesized that this relationship would be moderated by gender with girls showing a stronger relation between stressor load and increases in worry over time than boys.

In line with ourfirst hypothesis, we found an indirect effect of stressor load through worry on both depressive symptoms and anxiety. This suggests that worry is driving part of the relationship between overall stressor load and emotional symptoms in the period from early to mid-adolescence and may thus be important to target in prevention of stress-related mental health problems. Impor-tantly, worry was not found to mediate the relationship between two of the stressor domains (stress of arguments at home and romantic relationship) and depressive symptoms or anxiety. This may suggest that the mechanism between stressor load and stress-related mental health problems may differ depending on different sources of stress. Thus, differentiating between different sources of stress is important in future studies of mechanisms between stressors and stress-related mental health problems.

Contrary to our second hypothesis, the relationship between overall stressor load and worry was not moderated by gender. Still, a closer look at the domain specific stressors suggests a possibly more nuanced conclusion. More specifically, worry was found to mediate the relationship of stress due to peer pressure and increases in depressive symptoms and anxiety for girls but not for boys. Thus, while no overall moderation was confirmed, the influence of worry on the development of depressive ANXIETY, STRESS, & COPING 161

(10)

symptoms and anxiety in the face of stressors may differ depending on the type of stressor domain. Specifically, some stressor domains such as stress due to peer pressure only seem to affect the ten-dency to worry in girls whereas others, such as stress due to school performance affects subsequent levels of worry in both genders.

The current study contributes in several ways to the literature in the area. First, ourfinding that adolescents consistently rated higher levels of stress due to school performance relative to the other domains is consistent with previous findings where stress of academic achievement and future goals and performance expectations have been found to dominate among mid- to late ado-lescents (de Anda et al.,2000; Lin & Yusoff,2013). However, our results also add to the current knowl-edge. Specifically, our results show that the relative difference in importance of stress due to school and achievement over other types of stressors is prevalent already early on, and that the level of stress in this domain incrementally increases from early to mid-adolescence. Not in the least for girls there is a steep increase in reported stress from school from 7th to 8th grade. These high levels of reported stress due to school performance suggest that prevention efforts aimed at reducing school related stress might be especially potent earlier in adolescence.

Second, we found that levels of stress due to arguments at home and romantic relationships were much lower than stress due to school performance. Similarly, albeit more gradually compared to school performance stress, levels increased over this time period from early- to mid-adolescence. Previousfindings suggest that while prevalence of conflicts with parents tends to peak already in early adolescence, conflicts during the period from early to mid-adolescence may become increas-ingly intense and affect laden (Laursen et al.,1998; Smetana et al.,2006). The increase in the level of reported stress due to arguments at home in our sample could thus reflect changes in the quality of conflict. Also in line with developmental expectation, the gradual increase in stress from romantic relationships over time could reflect an increase in the frequency of experiences with these relationships. Indeed, romantic relationships tend to be rather uncommon in early ado-lescence but increases with age (Collins,2003). Somewhat surprising, there was no overall increase in stress due to peer pressure for either boys or girls. Considering that the peer group becomes a more important social arena in adolescence you might have expected this. However, past research has shown that peer influence and victimization may increase more during transitional periods such as the transition from middle to secondary school (Espelage & Holt, 2001; Stapinski, Araya, Heron, Montgomery, & Stallard,2015). Given that we followed the adolescents from 7th through 9th grade no such transition occurred at a group level at the time of the study. Nevertheless, stress due to peer pressure orfitting in with peers was generally low in comparison to school per-formance. This indicates that performance related questions and skills to plan and manage school activities should be a primary focus in universal preventive interventions aimed at reducing stress in secondary school students.

Third and of clinical importance, our results lend support to the key role of worry as a mechanism in the development of stress related mental health problems. Our results support that worry med-iates the relationship between self-reported overall stressor load and anxiety and depressive symptoms over time. This suggest that worry may be a promising target in preventive efforts aimed to reduce stress-related mental health problems. Prevention protocols that specifically target excessive perseverative cognitions such as worry and rumination have recently been devel-oped with promising results in terms of mental health outcomes in adolescent samples (Topper et al.,2017). More specifically, by helping adolescents become aware of habitual rumination and worry, identifying situations and events that trigger such responses, and teaching alternative responses (e.g., problem solving skills, relaxation, concrete thinking) Topper et al. (2017) found sig-nificant reductions in levels of worry and rumination in a group of adolescents. This reduction was in turn related to considerable lower prevalence rates of depression and generalized anxiety dis-order in the intervention group compared to a waitlist control group at one year follow-up. Further testing of the feasibility and long-term effectiveness of such protocols to prevent stress-related mental health problems is an important focus within stress prevention. Moreover,

(11)

investigating the role of worry in other types of stress-related problems such as somatic com-plaints or substance use in adolescent samples would also be important to elucidate whether excessive worry has transdiagnostic reach and could be a potential target for stress-related health complaints beyond internalizing emotional symptoms. Further, worry levels may also be a potent marker for adolescents that are at greater risk of developing mental health problems in response to common stressors and could thus be screened for together with overall stressor load in more targeted prevention. Doing so would not only assure that we actually are offering preventive interventions to those most at risk, but might also assure that the intervention is experienced as relevant, and hence improve adherence (Topper et al., 2010).

Interestingly, gender did not moderate the effect of overall stressor load on worry, indicating that although girls both report a higher stressor load and levels of worry at all time points, the effect of overall stressor load on worry is similar across gender. This is in line with more recent findings for rumination. More specifically, boys have been found to be as likely to ruminate in response to stressors as girls (Hamilton, Stange, Abramson, & Alloy,2015; Michl, McLaughlin, Shep-herd, & Nolen-Hoeksema, 2013; Stange et al., 2014). An explanation for the observed gender difference in levels of worry could be that girls are more exposed to stressors, which would be in line with our finding that girls reported a greater overall stressor load at all time points. There is some support for this “exposure” explanation for rumination, at least for dependent social stressors (stressors that are in some way dependent on the behavior or characteristics of an individual such as arguments with friends, as oppose to independent stressors that are beyond the control of the individual such as parental divorce) (Hamilton et al.,2015). More speci fi-cally, Hamilton et al. (2015) found that dependent social stressors predicted subsequent levels of rumination in both genders, but that exposure to such stressors was greater in girls which explained the gender difference in rumination.

However, the results of our test for moderation effects at the level of the specific stressor domains are not completely in line with this. Although we did notfind interactions between gender and some social stressors (arguments at home or romantic relationships) we didfind that gender moderated the effect of stress due to peer pressure on subsequent levels of worry. One explanation for this may be that our study specifically measured subjective stressor load and not frequency of exposure to stressors as was done in the Hamilton et al. (2015) study. Our measure of subjective stressor load may instead reflect girls greater reactivity in response to peer related stressors which is well in line with previous findings (Hankin et al., 2015; Hankin, Mermelstein, & Roesch, 2007). Girls tend to form more close and intimate friendships and to rely more heavily on emotional support in the peer context than boys, which has been suggested to make them more susceptible to disruptions in peer relationships (Maccoby,1990; Rudolph,2002). Thus, our results support that girls are more inclined to worry than boys when they experience stress in the peer context which further intensifies and prolongs negative mood and anxiety.

This study also has some limitations. First, our results rely on self-report data and using multiple informants and a multimethod approach (e.g., physiological indicators of stress) would have added to the methodological rigor. However, given the size of our sample and the longitudinal design this was not feasible within this study. Also, given that the measures used are intended to capture internalizing processes it can be argued that adolescents themselves would be the best infor-mers. Second, while we have a generally large and representative study sample, some of our study variables were related to attrition and may influence the external validity of our study. Specifically, adolescents that did not participate in all three time-points were higher on depressive symptoms and overall stressor load. Also, adolescents whose parents were divorced or who had parents of non-European origin, which potential could add to the overall stressor load, were more likely to be absent at follow-ups. It could therefore be that our results do not extend to adolescents with these characteristics. Similarly, caution should be taken before generalizing our results to clinical populations. Anxiety and depressive symptoms as measured in the current study cannot be paral-leled to clinical disorders. However, in taking a dimensional approach to mental health problems, ANXIETY, STRESS, & COPING 163

(12)

symptoms and mechanisms exists on a continuum. From this approach, it is important to investigate which mechanisms may affect movements up along the continuum whether crossing the cut-off for clinical disorders or not.

Despite these limitations, this study also has considerable strengths. First, we made use of a longi-tudinal design with a large and diverse sample of adolescents, a high response rate and high quality measures. This enabled us to not only investigate the relative importance of different sources of stress across development, but also the role of worry in the development of stress-related mental health problems over an important developmental time period. Second, we measured a broad range of sources of stress that have previously been identified as typical by adolescents themselves (Byrne et al., 2007). This allowed us to tap into what the most common sources of stress are over this time period. Still, the relatively brief measure of subjective stressor load used in the current study, with only a few items tapping in to each stressor domain, should be considered a limitation. Especially in contrast to semi-structured interview measures commonly used that has been rec-ommended as a gold standard in the stressor measurement literature (e.g., Dohrenwend, 2006; Monroe,2008). Such measurements would allow for more reliable and in-depth conclusions to be drawn as they tap into both the frequency of actual exposure to a wide range of different stressors and to more objective qualitative differences in severity. However, given the longitudinal design and the relatively large sample size in the current study, the use of semi-structured interviews were not feasible. Moreover, the ASQ-S have been found to possess similar psychometric properties as the full ASQ-2, making it a viable option for large longitudinal studies (Anniko, Boersma, van Wijk, Byrne, & Tillfors,2018). Nevertheless, the results from the current study should be viewed in the light of our measure at best capturing how stressful the participants find the measured event categories in general.

In summary, our results indicate that school is the most common as well as increasing source of stress from early to mid-adolescence. Girls report considerably more stress than boys, especially in terms of school performance and arguments at home. High overall stressor load is predictive of higher levels of anxiety and depressive symptoms across this time period and worry mediates this relationship. This suggests that a higher stressor load may lead to increased worry, which in turn negatively affects emotional health. These results implicate that school stress as well as worry may be important targets for preventive interventions aimed to reduce stress-related mental health pro-blems among adolescents. However, ourfindings also provide some potential nuances and indicate that the role of worry may be dependent on the type of stress as well as gender. This suggest that an important goal for future research would be to further confirm and explore if and why some sources of stress are more likely to lead to increases in worry than others and to identify other important mechanisms between adolescent stress and stress-related mental health problems.

Acknowledgements

Acknowledgements go to local leadership, management and students of public schools in Örebro, Köping and Karlskoga for their participation in the Three city study.

Disclosure statement

No potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors.

Funding

This work was supported by the Swedish research council Formas under Grant [no. 2012–00065].

ORCID

(13)

References

Anniko, M. K., Boersma, K., van Wijk, N. P. L., Byrne, D., & Tillfors, M. (2018). Development of a shortened version of the adolescent stress questionnaire (ASQ-S): Construct validity and sex invariance in a large sample of Swedish adoles-cents. Scandinavian Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Psychology, 6, 4–15.doi:10.21307/sjcapp-2018-001

Arnett, J. J. (1999). Adolescent storm and stress, reconsidered. American Psychologist, 54, 317–326. doi:10.1037//0003-066X.54.5.317

Avenevoli, S., Swendsen, J., He, J.-P., Burnstein, M., & Merikangas, K. R. (2015). Major depression in the national comorbid-ity survey–adolescent supplement: Prevalence, correlates, and treatment. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 54, 37–44.e2.doi:10.1016/j.jaac.2014.10.010

Borkovec, T., & Roemer, L. (1995). Perceived functions of worry among generalized anxiety disorder subjects: Distraction from more emotionally distressing topics? Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 26, 25–30.doi:10. 1016/0005-7916(94)00064-S

Brosschot, J. F., Gerin, W., & Thayer, J. F. (2006). The perseverative cognition hypothesis: A review of worry, prolonged stress-related physiological activation, and health. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 60, 113–124.doi:10.1016/j. jpsychores.2005.06.074

Buuren, S., & Groothuis-Oudshoorn, K. (2011). Mice: Multivariate imputation by chained equations in R. Journal of Statistical Software, 45.doi:10.18637/jss.v045.i03

Byrne, D., Davenport, S., & Mazanov, J. (2007). Profiles of adolescent stress: The development of the Adolescent Stress Questionnaire (ASQ). Journal of Adolescence, 30, 393–416.doi:10.1016/j.adolescence.2006.04.004

Campbell-Sills, L., Norman, S. B., Craske, M. G., Sullivan, G., Lang, A. J., Chavira, D. A.,… Stein, M. B. (2009). Validation of a brief measure of anxiety-related severity and impairment: The Overall Anxiety Severity and Impairment Scale (OASIS). Journal of Affective Disorders, 112, 92–101.doi:10.1016/j.jad.2008.03.014

Chorpita, B. F., Tracey, S. A., Brown, T. A., Collica, T. J., & Barlow, D. H. (1997). Assessment of worry in children and ado-lescents: An adaptation of the Penn State Worry Questionnaire. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 35, 569–581.

doi:10.1016/S0005-7967(96)00116-7

Collins, W. A. (2003). More than myth: The developmental significance of romantic relationships during adolescence. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 13, 1–24.doi:10.1111/1532-7795.1301001

de Anda, D., Baroni, S., Boskin, L., Buchwald, L., Morgan, J., Ow, J.,… Weiss, R. (2000). Stress, stressors and coping among high school students. Children and Youth Services Review, 22, 441–463.doi:10.1016/S0190-7409(00)00096-7

Dohrenwend, B. P. (2006). Inventorying stressful life events as risk factors for psychopathology: Toward resolution of the problem of intracategory variability. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 477–495.doi:10.1037/0033-2909.132.3.477

Eisma, M. C., Boelen, P. A., Schut, H. A. W., & Stroebe, M. S. (2017). Does worry affect adjustment to bereavement? A longi-tudinal investigation. Anxiety, Stress, & Coping, 30, 243–252.doi:10.1080/10615806.2016.1229464

Esbjørn, B. H., Reinholdt-Dunne, M. L., Caspersen, I. D., Christensen, L., & Chorpita, B. (2013). Penn State Worry Questionnaire: Findings form normative and clinical samples in Denmark. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 35, 113–122.doi:10.1007/s10862-012-9320-7

Espelage, D. L., & Holt, M. K. (2001). Bullying and victimization during early adolescence: Peer influences and psychosocial correlates. Journal of Emotional Abuse, 2, 123–142.doi:10.1300/J135v02n02_08

Hamilton, J. L., Stange, J. P., Abramson, L. Y., & Alloy, L. B. (2015). Stress and the development of cognitive vulnerabilities to depression explain sex differences in depressive symptoms during adolescence. Clinical Psychological Science, 3, 702– 714.doi:10.1177/2167702614545479

Hankin, B. L., Mermelstein, R., & Roesch, L. (2007). Sex differences in adolescent depression: Stress exposure and reactivity models. Child Development, 78, 279–295.doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.00997.x

Hankin, B. L., Young, J. F., Abela, J. R. Z., Smolen, A., Jenness, J. L., Gulley, L. D.,… Oppenheimer, C. W. (2015). Depression from childhood into late adolescence: Influence of gender, development, genetic susceptibility, and peer stress. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 124, 803–816.doi:10.1037/abn0000089

Harvey, A., Watkins, E. R., Mansell, W., & Shafran, R. (2004). Cognitive behavioural processes across psychological disorders: A transdiagnostic approach to research and treatment. New York, NY: Oxford University Press Inc.doi:10.1093/med:psych/ 9780198528883.001.0001

Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach. New York, NY: Guilford Press.doi:10.1111/jedm.12050.

Howard, W. J., Rhemtulla, M., & Little, T. D. (2015). Using principal components as auxiliary variables in missing data esti-mation. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 50, 285–299.doi:10.1080/00273171.2014.999267

Jose, P. E., & Brown, I. (2008). When does the gender difference in rumination begin? Gender and age differences in the use of rumination by adolescents. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 37, 180–192.doi:10.1007/s10964-006-9166-y

Kärnä, A., Voeten, M., Little, T. D., Poskiparta, E., Kaljonen, A., & Salmivalli, C. (2011). A large-scale evaluation of the KiVa antibullying program: Grades 4–6. Child Development, 82, 311–330.doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01557.x

Larson, R., & Ham, M. (1993). Stress and "storm and stress" in early adolescence: The relationship of negative events with dysphoric affect.. Developmental Psychology, 29, 130–140.doi:10.1037//0012-1649.29.1.130

Laursen, B., Coy, K. C., & Collins, W. A. (1998). Reconsidering changes in parent-child conflict across adolescence: A meta-analysis. Child Development, 69, 817–832.doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1998.tb06245.x

(14)

Lin, H. J., & Yusoff, M. S. B. (2013). Psychological distress, sources of stress and coping strategy in high school students. International Medical Journal, 20(6), 672–676.

Maccoby, E. E. (1990). Gender and relationships: A developmental account. American Psychologist, 45, 513–520.doi:10. 1037/0003-066X.45.4.513

MacLean, A., Sweeting, H., Egan, M., Der, G., Adamson, J., & Hunt, K. (2013). How robust is the evidence of an emerging or increasing female excess in physical morbidity between childhood and adolescence? Results of a systematic literature review and meta-analyses. Social Science & Medicine, 78, 96–112.doi:10.1016/j.socscimed.2012.11.039

McEvoy, P. M., Watson, H., Watkins, E. R., & Nathan, P. (2013). The relationship between worry, rumination, and comorbid-ity: Evidence for repetitive negative thinking as a transdiagnostic construct. Journal of Affective Disorders, 151, 313–320.

doi:10.1016/j.jad.2013.06.014

Merikangas, K. R., He, J.-P., Burstein, M., Swanson, S. A., Avenevoli, S., Cui, L.,… Swendsen, J. (2010). Lifetime prevalence of mental disorders in US adolescents: Results from the National Comorbidity Survey Replication–Adolescent Supplement (NCS-A). Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 49, 980–989.doi:10.1016/j. jaac.2010.05.017

Michl, L. C., McLaughlin, K. A., Shepherd, K., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2013). Rumination as a mechanism linking stressful life events to symptoms of depression and anxiety: Longitudinal evidence in early adolescents and adults. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 122, 339–352.doi:10.1037/a0031994

Monroe, S. M. (2008). Modern approaches to conceptualizing and measuring human life stress. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 4, 33–52.doi:10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.4.022007.141207

Norman, S. B., Hami Cissell, S., Means-Christensen, A. J., & Stein, M. B. (2006). Development and validation of an overall anxiety severity and impairment scale (OASIS). Depression and Anxiety, 23, 245–249.doi:10.1002/da.20182

Olsson, G., & von Knorring, A.-L. (1997). Depression among Swedish adolescents measured by the self rating scale Center for Epidemiology Studies-Depression Child (CES-DC). European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 6, 81–87.doi:10.1007/ BF00566670

Östberg, V., Almquist, Y. B., Folkesson, L., Låftman, S. B., Modin, B., & Lindfors, P. (2015). The complexity of stress in mid-adolescent girls and boys. Child Indicators Research, 8, 403–423.doi:10.1007/s12187-014-9245-7

Ottaviani, C., Thayer, J. F., Verkuil, B., Lonigro, A., Medea, B., Couyoumdjian, A., & Brosschot, J. F. (2016). Physiological con-comitants of perseverative cognition: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 142, 231–259.

doi:10.1037/bul0000036

Pestle, S. L., Chorpita, B. F., & Schiffman, J. (2008). Psychometric properties of the Penn State Worry Questionnaire for chil-dren in a large clinical sample. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 37, 465–471. doi:10.1080/ 15374410801955896

Ravens-Sieberer, U., Torsheim, T., Hetland, J., Vollebergh, W., Cavallo, F., Jericek, H.,… Erhart, M. (2009). Subjective health, symptom load and quality of life of children and adolescents in Europe. International Journal of Public Health, 54, 151 159.doi:10.1007/s00038-009-5406-8

Roussis, P., & Wells, A. (2008). Psychological factors predicting stress symptoms: Metacognition, thought control, and var-ieties of worry. Anxiety, Stress, & Coping, 21, 213–225.doi:10.1080/10615800801889600

Rudolph, K. D. (2002). Gender differences in emotional responses to interpersonal stress during adolescence. Journal of Adolescent Health, 30, 3–13.doi:10.1016/S1054-139x(01)00383-4

Rudolph, K. D., & Hammen, C. (1999). Age and gender as determinants of stress exposure, generation, and reactions in youngsters: A transactional perspective. Child Development, 70, 660–677.doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00048

Shaw, T., Cross, D., Thomas, L. T., & Zubrick, S. R. (2015). Bias in student surveyfindings from active parental consent pro-cedures. British Educational Research Journal, 41, 229–243.doi:10.1002/berj.3137

Sibrava, N. J., & Borkovec, T. D. (2006). The cognitive avoidance theory of worry. In G. C. L. Davey & A. Wells (Eds.), Worry and its psychological disorders: Theory, assessment and treatment (pp. 239–256). Hoboken, NJ, US: Wiley Publishing.

doi:10.1002/9780470713143.ch14.

Smetana, J. G., Campione-Barr, N., & Metzger, A. (2006). Adolescent development in interpersonal and societal contexts. Annual Review of Psychology, 57, 255–284.doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.57.102904.190124

Stange, J. P., Hamilton, J. L., Abramson, L. Y., & Alloy, L. B. (2014). A vulnerability-stress examination of response styles theory in adolescence: Stressors, sex differences, and symptom specificity. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 43, 813–827.doi:10.1080/15374416.2013.812037

Stapinski, L. A., Araya, R., Heron, J., Montgomery, A. A., & Stallard, P. (2015). Peer victimization during adolescence: Concurrent and prospective impact on symptoms of depression and anxiety. Anxiety, Stress, & Coping, 28, 105–120.

doi:10.1080/10615806.2014.962023

Statistics Sweden. (2014–2015). Retrieved from http://api.scb.se/OV0104/v1/doris/sv/ssd/START/LE/LE0106/LE0106A/ LE0106Ekonomi. April 27 2018.

Sweeting, H., Young, R., & West, P. (2009). GHQ increases among Scottish 15 year olds 1987–2006. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 44, 579–586.doi:10.1007/s00127-008-0462-6

Topper, M, Emmelkamp, P. M, & Ehring, T. (2010). Improving prevention of depression and anxiety disorders: Repetitive negative thinking as a promising target. Applied and Preventive Psychology, 14, 57–71.

(15)

Topper, M., Emmelkamp, P. M., Watkins, E. R., & Ehring, T. (2017). Prevention of anxiety disorders and depression by tar-geting excessive worry and rumination in adolescents and young adults: A randomized controlled trial. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 90, 123–136.doi:10.1016/j.brat.2016.12.015

Verkuil, B., Brosschot, J. F., Gebhardt, W. A., & Thayer, J. F. (2010). When worries make you sick: A review of perseverative cognition, the default stress response and somatic health. Journal of Experimental Psychopathology, 1, 87–118.doi:10. 5127/jep.009110

Watkins, E. R. (2008). Constructive and unconstructive repetitive thought. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 163–206.doi:10. 1037/0033-2909.134.2.163

Weissman, M. M., Orvaschel, H., & Padian, N. (1980). Children’s symptom and social functioning self-report scales com-parison of mothers’ and children’s reports. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 168, 736–740.doi:10.1097/ 00005053-198012000-00005

Wen, Z., & Fan, X. (2015). Monotonicity of effect sizes: Questioning kappa-squared as mediation effect size measure. Psychological Methods, 20, 193–203.doi:10.1037/met0000029

Zimmer-Gembeck, M. J., & Skinner, E. A. (2011). Review: The development of coping across childhood and adolescence: An integrative review and critique of research. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 35, 1–17.doi:10.1177/ 0165025410384923

References

Related documents

46 Konkreta exempel skulle kunna vara främjandeinsatser för affärsänglar/affärsängelnätverk, skapa arenor där aktörer från utbuds- och efterfrågesidan kan mötas eller

Both Brazil and Sweden have made bilateral cooperation in areas of technology and innovation a top priority. It has been formalized in a series of agreements and made explicit

Coping with ER stress takes place by the unfolded protein response by co-ordination of series of proteins, and one of the proteins which, I am interested in this study is P58

Investigating relations between activation and pressure stress, aggregate salivary cortisol measures and recurrent pain in mid-adolescent girls and boys, the present study

difference between stress levels during Group study and Individual study, with Group study generating higher levels of distress among the participants.. These findings are relevant

Because of the extensive information accrued (described in detail below), the Kupol study allows for the exploration of highly relevant areas in addition to its primary objectives,

The existing instruments to measure police stress, such as the Spielberger Police Stress Survey (S-PSS), are not contextualised for Swedish patrolling police work; given the changes

The EU exports of waste abroad have negative environmental and public health consequences in the countries of destination, while resources for the circular economy.. domestically