• No results found

Hiring Discrimination Against Transgender People: Evidence from a Field Experiment

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "Hiring Discrimination Against Transgender People: Evidence from a Field Experiment"

Copied!
13
0
0

Loading.... (view fulltext now)

Full text

(1)

ContentslistsavailableatScienceDirect

Labour

Economics

journalhomepage:www.elsevier.com/locate/labeco

Hiring

Discrimination

Against

Transgender

People:

Evidence

from

a

Field

Experiment

Mark

Granberg

,

Per

A.

Andersson

,

Ali

Ahmed

Division of Economics, Department of Management and Engineering, Linköping University, 581 83 Linköping, Sweden

a r t i c l e

i n f o

JEL Classifications: C93 J15 J16 J23 J71 Keywords: Field experiment Correspondence test Transgender people Discrimination Labor market

a b s t r a c t

Thispaperpresentstheresultsofthefirstcorrespondencestudythatexaminedhiringdiscriminationagainst transgenderpeople.Fictitiousjobapplications(N=2,224)weresenttoemployerswithjobpostingsin12 low-skilloccupationsinSweden.Overall,40percentofcisgenderapplicantsand34percentoftransgenderapplicants receivedapositiveemployerresponsetotheirapplications.ThisresultwasnotrobusttotheHeckman-Siegelman critique.However,whentransgenderapplicantswerecomparedtothedominantgenderinmale-and female-dominatedoccupations,estimatesofdiscriminationwerelargerandrobusttothecritique.Therewasnoclear supportforthestatisticaldiscriminationhypothesis.

1. Introduction

Transgenderpeople report experiencinghighrates of discrimina-tion.1 Forexample,inanEU-widesurvey,54 percentoftransgender respondents reported that they “have felt personally discriminated againstorharassedbecauseofbeingperceivedastrans” withinthe pre-cedingyear(European UnionAgencyforFundamentalRights,2014). Inasurveyof6,436transgendermenandwomenfrom theU.S.,50 percentreportedexperiencingharassmentatworkand44percent re-portedhavingexperiencedhiringdiscriminationbasedontheiridentity (Grantetal.,2011).Hiringdiscriminationcouldleadtoincreasedrates of unemployment, adversely affecting individual mental health and wellbeing(PaulandMoser,2009).Thismeansthathiring discrimina-tioncouldbeapartialexplanationfortheincreasedratesofpsychiatric hospitalizationandsuicidalityamongtransgenderpeople(Dhejneetal., 2011).Yet,thereisalackofexperimentalstudiesgoingbeyond self-reportedexperiencesthatidentifytheactual discriminationfaced by

Correspondingauthor.

E-mailaddresses:mark.granberg@liu.se(M.Granberg),per.a.andersson@liu.se (P.A.Andersson),ali.ahmed@liu.se(A.Ahmed).

1 Inthispapertransgenderdesignatesapersonwhosegenderidentity

mis-matcheswiththesexthepersonwasidentifiedashavingatbirth.Incontrast, cisgenderdesignatesapersonwhosegenderidentitymatcheswiththesexthe personwasidentifiedashavingatbirth.

transgenderpeopleinthelabormarket.Weconductedapreregistered fieldexperimenttodocumentcausalevidenceofhiringdiscrimination againsttransgenderpeoplein12labormarketsectorsinSweden.2

Therehavealreadybeensomeeffortstostudytransgenderpeople in thelabormarketthroughnonexperimental methods.Forexample, Geijtenbeek and Plug (2018) found that Dutch transgender people suffered an approximate11 percent wagepenalty. There are, how-ever, some well-known difficulties in using registry data to study discrimination. First, these nonexperimental statistical approaches cannot fully separate discriminationfrom other explanationsdue to issues with omissionof unobservable productivity-relevant variables (Neumark,2018) which preventsidentificationof average treatment effects. Second,andmorespecifictothecase oftransgenderpeople, registrydatainmostcasescaptures asubsetofindividualswhohave gone through, orstarted,theprocessoflegally changingtheirname and/orgender.InSweden,thefirstpointofcontactforanindividual wishingtostartthisprocesswouldbeapsychologisttodeliveragender

2The preregistration includes a preset data collection period

and an outline of hypotheses, variables and methods (available at: https://aspredicted.org/sn8sj.pdf). For more on preregistration, see Noseketal.(2018).

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.labeco.2020.101860

Received4December2019;Receivedinrevisedform20May2020;Accepted26May2020 Availableonline31May2020

0927-5371/© 2020TheAuthors.PublishedbyElsevierB.V.ThisisanopenaccessarticleundertheCCBY-NC-NDlicense. (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/)

(2)

dysphoriadiagnosis.3Thenumberoftreatmentreferralsforyouth gen-derdysphoriahasgrowndrasticallyinSwedeninrecentyears(Frisén et al., 2017; Socialstyrelsen, 2020).4 As this increase is especially pronouncedamongyoungpeoplewhoaresoontoenterthelaborforce itisimportanttoexplorethehiringsituationtheymightface.

Inthepresentstudy,weusedabroadandmoreinclusivedefinitionof transgenderpeople.Wedefinedtransgenderpeopleasthosewholegally havechangedtheirgenderaswellasthosewhosociallyidentifyasthe oppositegenderassignedtothematbirth.Itisdifficulttosayfor cer-tainhowmanypeoplefitthisbroaderdefinition;accordingonestudy only34 percentSwedish transgendermenandwomenreported hav-inglegallychangedtheirgender(Zelufetal.2016).5Thissuggeststhat alargeproportionoftransgenderpeoplemaynotappearinregistries butdoidentifywiththeoppositegender.Researchhasshownthat26 percentoftransgenderpeopleinSwedenhaveexperienced discrimina-tioninaccesstohealthcare(EuropeanUnionAgencyforFundamental Rights,2014)and69percenthaveexperiencednegativetreatmentin receivinghealthcare(Zelufetal.,2016).Theseexperiencescouldmake itdifficultfortransgenderpeopletostartorcompletetheprocessof for-malandlegalgendertransition,buttheymaystillbeatriskoffacing discriminationinthelabormarket.

Asanalternativetononexperimentalmethods,Bertrandand Mul-lainathan(2004) 6 popularizedtheuse of correspondencetesting to detect discrimination. This method has become the standard way of testing for discrimination in housing and labor markets. Corre-spondencetests have been used to document discriminationagainst many groups in Sweden (e.g., Ahmed et al., 2012; Ahmed et al., 2009; Ahmedand Lång,2017, 2019; Bursell, 2014; Carlsson, 2011; CarlssonandRooth,2007;Rooth,2004;CarlssonandEriksson,2019). Correspondencestudiesarefieldexperimentsinwhichfictitious appli-cationsaresenttorealemployerswithjobpostings.Theassociated cur-riculavitae(CVs)arekeptthesameforallapplicantsbuttherelevant groupsignalisrandomizedbytheresearcher.Ratesofpositiveemployer responsestojobapplicationsfromeachgroupcanthenbecomparedto acquireaveragetreatmenteffects.Themethodhasprovidedcredible evidenceof hiringdiscrimination,especiallyagainstminoritygroups inmanydifferentcountries.(Forrecentreviewsofthisliterature,see BertrandandDuflo,2017;Baert,2018;andNeumark,2018)

BetweenJanuaryandMayof2019,wesent2,224fictitious appli-cationstoemployerswithjobvacanciesin12low-skilledsectorsinthe Swedishlabormarket.WeexploitedthecommonpracticeinSwedenof prefacingone’sCVwithashortcoverletter.Inthisletter,ourfictitious applicantsintroducedthemselvesbyname(e.g.,“Hello,mynameisA Eriksson).” Inthesecondparagraph,theythenmentioned:“Forfuture contactIwouldliketoclarifythatIhavechangedmyname(fromB Eriksson)soafewofmyolddocumentsareinmypreviousname.” For transgenderapplicants,thenamesAandBweredrawnfromapoolof commondifferentlygenderednamesandforcisgenderapplicantsthe nameswheredrawnfromthesamegender.Ourdesignmakesuse of

3 InSweden,5,841peoplewerediagnosedwithgenderdysphoriaasof2018

(Socialstyrelsen,2020).Withapopulationof10,230,185,theproportionwith thediagnosiswas0.057percent.

4 TherewerenearzeroreferralsataStockholmyouthclinicin2006,anumber

whichincreasedto197in2016(Figure1inFrisénetal.,2017).Theincreaseis especiallymarkedamongyoungpeople,andnatalgirls(Socialstyrelsen2020).

5 OwncalculationbasedonnumbersreportedbyZelufetal.(2016).Weused

thenumberofrespondentswhohadchangedtheirlegalgender(114)and di-videditbythenumberofrespondentswhowereeither“transfeminine” or“trans masculine” (149+187)toarriveatthe33.9figure.Includinganyoftheother categoriesinthesurvey,gendernonbinary(346)andcross-dresser(112),would decreasethepercentageandonlyservetostrengthenourpresentargument.

6 This study started a virtual explosion in use of correspondence

stud-iesin recentyears,butuseof the methodgoesas farback asJowelland Prescott-Clarke(1970)anditsawsemi-frequentusebeforeBertrandand Mul-lainathan(2004),forinstance,Neumarketal.(1996).

thefactthatchangingone’snameisfairlycommonplaceinSweden. Be-tween2013and2018,95,090peopleappliedtochangetheirfirstname inSweden(owncalculationsusingdataobtainedfromtheSwedishTax Agency).Itis,therefore,reasonabletoassumethatcisgenderapplicants werenotseenasparticularlyspecialbyevaluatorsbecauseoftheirname change.

Fromaninternationalperspective,studyingthesituationfor trans-genderpeopleinSwedenmeansthatweestimatealowerboundof hir-ingdiscriminationagainstthisgroup.Swedenisconsideredoneofthe mostprogressivecountriesintheworldwhenitcomestogender equal-ity(WorldEconomicForum,2018)andLGBTacceptance(Floresand Park,2018).Furthermore,Swedishtransgenderpeoplereportsomeof thelowestratesofexperienceddiscriminationintheEUwhensearching forwork(EuropeanUnionAgencyforFundamentalRights,2014).

Followingourpreregistrationplan,webeganbyexaminingoverall discriminationagainsttransgenderapplicantsacrossall12occupations in theexperiment.Besidesestimatingtheaveragetreatmenteffectof beingtransgenderonpositiveemployerresponserateswhenapplying forwork, ourdesignallowedustotestapredictionofthestatistical discriminationtheory(Arrow,1973;Phelps,1972);thatdiscrimination shoulddiminishasinformationaboutotherrelevantskillindicators in-creases. Wealsoexamined whetheremployers’based theirdecisions ontheircustomers’tastes(Becker,1957)byexaminingdifferencesin discriminationbetweenoccupationswhichtendtorequireface-to-face customerinteraction(FFCI)andthosewhichdonot.7Wethenapplied Neumark’s(2012)methodtocheckwhetherourestimatesof discrimi-nationwererobusttothecritiquepresentedbyHeckmanandSiegelman (1993).8

Additionally,weexploredtheroleofoccupationalgender distribu-tion.Thiswasmainlymotivatedbyevidencefromcorrespondence stud-iesofhiringdiscriminationwhichtendstoshowheterogeneityacross oc-cupationswithdifferentgenderratios(Rich,2014;Ahmedetal.,2013a; Carlsson,2011).Anotherreasonwasthatsomesurveydatasuggestthat menmaybemoreprejudicedagainsttransgenderpeoplethanwomen (Nagoshietal.,2008).Weperformedsubsampleanalyseswhere occu-pationalsectorswerecategorizedbyoccupationalgenderratio,splitting thetotalsampleintomale-dominated,female-dominated,andmixed oc-cupations. Discriminationin occupationswithdifferentgender ratios wasevaluated inasimilarwaytooveralldiscrimination(i.e., check-ingforsignsofstatisticalandcustomerdiscrimination).Inaneffortto betterunderstandtheheterogeneousdiscriminationuncoveredacross occupationswithdifferentgendercompositions,wealsoexamined dif-ferences inthetreatmentoftransgendermaleandfemaleapplicants. Lastly,wetestedtherobustnesstotheHeckman-Siegelmancritiqueof theestimateddiscriminationinthesegenderratiosubsamples.

2. CorrespondenceStudyDesign

Weconductedourpreregisteredcorrespondencetestbetween Jan-uaryandMay2019.During thistime,wesent fictitiousapplications to2,224employerswhohadpostedvacanciesontheSwedishPublic EmploymentOfficewebsite,thelargestrepositoryforjobvacanciesin Sweden.Thefirsthalfof2019wasarelativelygoodtimefortheSwedish labormarket.Thejobvacancyratewas2.8percentinSwedenwhilethe

7Customerdiscrimination,whichwasalsoproposedbyBecker(1957),has

beenshowntobeimportantinthecontextofotherminorities(e.g.Holzerand Ihlanfeldt,1998).

8This method isanincreasinglycommonrobustness checkin the

litera-ture(forrecentapplications,seeNeumark,2012;Neumarketal.,2016,2019; CarlssonandEriksson,2019)

(3)

Europeanjobvacancyratewas2.4percent.9Also,theunemployment ratewas6.8percent,amongthelowestratesforSwedenin15years.10 We sent only one job application to each employer because paireddesignsriskviolatingtheStableUnitTreatmentValue Assump-tion (SUTVA) and can introduce bias into discrimination estimates (Phillips,2019).Also,anunmatcheddesignallowedustouseaprecise signalingoftransgenderidentity.Ascommonaschangingone’sname isinSweden,employerswouldhavelikelygrownsuspiciousiftheyhad receivedtwoapplicationsfromapplicantswhohadbothchangedtheir name.Wefocusedon12occupationalcategorieswhereeducational re-quirementswererarelyhigherthanasecondaryeducation;hencewe refertothemaslow-skilljobs.Weexaminedlow-skilljobsbecausewith thegrowingratesofgenderdysphoriaamongyoungpeople(Frisénet al.,2017;Socialstyrelsen,2020)wewantedtoassessthesituationthey wouldfacewhenenteringthelaborforce.Anotheradvantageto focus-ingonlow-skilljobswasthatthehiringfirmstendedtobesmall busi-nessesandthereforedecision-makingisnotasdispersedthroughoutthe organization.Wewerethereforemorelikelytoelicitthetastesofthe businessownersthemselvesratherthanthoseofanotheremployee.

Theoccupationalcategorieswerechosensothattherewere suffi-cientnumbersofvacanciestoaccommodatestatisticalanalyseswhile representingdifferentoccupationalgenderratios.Occupationalgender ratioswasanimportantcriteriontoconsiderbecausetheseratioshave beenfoundtobeakeyfactorinearlierstudiesofconventionalgender discrimination(Carlsson,2011;Rich,2014).11Ofthe12low-skill occu-pationsincludedinourexperiment,fourweremale-dominated(3.9to 21.9percentfemale),fourweremixed(43.3to62.5percentfemale), andfourwerefemale-dominated(70.1to91.6percentfemale) occupa-tions.Dataongenderratiosinoccupationswerefrom2016andwere basedoneveryemployedpersoninSwedenaged16to64.12

Table1summarizesthedatabyshowingtheoccupationalcategories, theoccupationlevelcharacteristics(genderratioandamountof face-to-faceinteraction),thenumberofapplicationssenttovacanciesineach occupation,andthenumbersofpositiveemployerresponsesthese ap-plicationsgenerated.

Wecodedanyresponsefromemployerswhichseemedpositively in-clinedtocontinuethehiringprocessasa“positiveemployerresponse,” thisincludedbothdirectinvitationstointerviewsandpositivequeries ofmoreinformationabouttheapplicants.Positiveemployerresponses werepolitely declined,assoonaspossible,bytellingemployers that theapplicantalreadyhadfoundanotherjob.Someemployerresponses weredifficulttocategorizeaspositiveornegative.These50responses werecategorizedaccordingtoourbestjudgmentandweremarkedas uncertain.IntheSupplementaryMaterial(TableA8)weshowthatour resultsarerobusttorecodingthese50casesaseitherallpositiveorall negative.

9 Source: Eurostat.Available at: https://appsso.eurostat.ec.europa.eu/nui/

show.do?dataset=jvs_q_isco_r2&lang=en

10 Source: Statistics Sweden. Available at:

https://www.scb.se/hitta-statistik/statistik-efter-amne/arbetsmarknad/arbetskraftsundersokningar/ arbetskraftsundersokningarna-aku

11 Wealsotestedthisonpre-existingdatacollectedin2016(Thedatawere

originallyusedinAhmed,Granberg,andLång2017;AhmedandLång2017, 2019).Thisanalysisconfirmsthatoccupationalgenderratioisimportantwhen studyingconventionalsexdiscriminationinSweden.Resultsfromthatanalysis: Femaleapplicantswerefavoredingeneral;thiseffectwasdrivenby female-dominatedoccupationswhiletherewasnosignificantmale-femaledifference inpositiveemployerresponserateseitherinmixedorinmale-dominated occu-pations.SeeSupplementaryMaterialformoredetails.

12 Source:StatisticsSweden.Availableat:https://www.statistikdatabasen.scb.

se/pxweb/sv/ssd/START__AM__AM0208__AM0208B/YREG61/?rxid= 0e62353a-d331-4916-8730-8997ab1b72af

2.1. Procedureandexperimentalmaterials

InSweden,jobsaretypicallyappliedtousingashortletterof in-terest(acoverletter)followedbyaCV,aformatwhichwefollowed (seetheSupplementaryMaterial,Example A2,foratranslated exam-pleofanapplicationletter).Eachprospectiveemployerwascontacted throughemailandwassentasingleapplicationwheregenderidentity oftheapplicantandfourattributesrelatingtoobservableworkerskills wererandomized.13 These randomizedelementswillbe discussedin Sections2.2and2.3.

Thefictitiousapplicationscontainednospecificaddressandthe con-tactinformationconsistedsolelyofanemailaddressandamobile tele-phonenumber.Allapplicantshadthesamebirthdate(thisiscommon toreportinSwedishjobapplications)andstatedthattheywere28years oldintheintroductiontotheircoverletter.Wedecidedtonotgivea postaladdressbecauseearlierstudieshavefoundthatemployerstend tonotrespondtoapplicantsviaordinarypostinSweden(Ahmedetal., 2012).Theapplicationincludedinformationabouteducationandwork experiencewhichalsoimpliedthattheapplicantshadgrownup and livedaroundStockholm.Tobe abletoapply tojobpostingsallover Sweden,andnotjustinthegreaterStockholmarea,theapplicantsstated earlyintheircoverletterthat“…IthinkIwouldbeagreatfitforthejob, andIamabouttomovetothearea” wheretheword“area” was man-uallychangedtothenearestmajorpopulationcenterbasedonthejob beingappliedto(IntheSupplementaryMaterial,TableA14,weshow thatresultsareunaffectedbyusingdifferentcontrolsforjoblocation).14 Anyreferencetojobtitleintheapplicationletterwascustomized man-uallyforeachjob.

2.2. Experimentalmanipulationoftransgenderidentity

Eachapplicant’sgenderidentity(i.e.,cisgenderortransgender)was signaledthroughastatednamechangeintheapplication.Transgender applicantsstatedthattheyhadchangedtheirnamefromafemalename toamalenameorviceversa.Cisgenderapplicantsstatedthattheyhad changedtheirnamefromonemalenametoanotherorfromonefemale nametoanother.Hence,oursignalofbeingacisgenderortransgender personwascleartoanyonereadingtheapplicationwithoutthefictitious applicant explicitlystatingtheir genderidentityorhavingtoprofess affiliationwithsome LGBTinterestgroup(awaysometimesused to signalhomosexualityinthesetypesofstudies,see,e.g.,Drydakis,2015). WeconsultedhumanresourcespersonnelatalargeSwedish univer-sitytogettheirinputonhowtomakethename-changesignalfeelas naturalaspossible.Wealsoconsultedwithsmallbusinessownersinthe restaurantindustrywhoprovideduswithsomeexamplesoftypical ap-plicationstheyhadreceived.Followingthisfeedback,thenamesofthe fictitiousapplicantswereshownthreetimes;intheinitialandclosing greetingsinthecoverletterandatthetopoftheCV.Thebirthname wasfirstpresentedinaparentheticalsentenceinthesecondparagraph ofthecoverletter.Thepassageread:“ForfuturecontactIwouldliketo clarifythatIhavechangedmyname(fromBEriksson)soafewofmy olddocumentsareinmypreviousname,butwiththesamesocial secu-ritynumber.” Theinformationaboutthenamechangewasreiteratedin parenthesesatthetopoftheCVnexttothecurrentnameforinstance, “AEriksson(prev.BEriksson).” Weareconfidentthatthesignaledname changewascleartomostrecruiters,notleastbecauseafewemployers explicitlyacknowledgeditduringtheexperiment.

13Weavoidedapplyingforjobsthatrequirefillinginstandardizedonline

ap-plicationformsfortworeasons:(i)theytypicallyrequirepersonalinformation whichisdifficulttofalsifysuchasSwedishsocialsecuritynumbers;and(ii) theyoftenrequireacceptingthetermsofservicerelatingtothestorageof per-sonalinformation.Withfictitiousapplications,thistypeofacceptanceis ethi-callyquestionable.

14OwntranslationfromSwedish.SeeSupplementaryMaterialforatranslated

(4)

Table1

OccupationalCategories

Occupation Gender ratio % FFCI CVs sent Positive responses (%) Male-dominated

Vehicle mechanic 3.9 Low 170 62 (36.5)

Forklift operator 6.2 Low 35 9 (25.7)

Delivery/Truck driver 8.4 High 230 117 (50.9)

Warehouse worker 21.9 Low 185 33 (17.8)

Mixed

Telemarketing 43.3 Low 92 61 (66.3)

Chef 52.5 Low 343 151 (44.0)

Waitstaff 58.9 High 274 107 (39.1)

Store clerk 62.5 High 226 35 (15.5)

Female-dominated

Customer service 70.1 Low 116 42 (36.2)

Cleaner 75.1 High 303 96 (31.7)

Childcare 82.1 High 127 46 (36.2)

Enrolled nurse 91.6 High 123 65 (52.9)

Total (Mean) (50.2) - 2,224 824 (37.1)

Note.—“FFCI” is short for face-to-facecustomer interaction.“Gender ratio %” is the percentageoffemaleworkersinthegivenoccupationin2016(datafromStatistics Sweden)themeansinparenthesisonthebottomrowreportsthesamplemean,notthe meanofthecolumnvalues.TheoccupationalcategoriesofStatisticsSwedenandthe onesusedontheSwedishPublicEmploymentOfficewebsitedonotmatchcompletely, andassuchwehaveusedthenumbersfortheoccupationalcategorywhichwefound tobethemostsimilar.Theupperfouroccupationsareclassifiedasmale-dominated becausethefractionofwomenislessthanonethird,likewisethelastfourwereclassified asfemale-dominatedbecausethefractionofmenislessthanonethirdandlastlythe middlefourwereclassifiedasmixedoccupations.

Figure1. MeanPositiveEmployerResponseRatesforDifferentApplicants.Note.—Thesefiguresshowthemeanpositiveemployerresponseratesforapplicants withdifferenttypesofnamechangesinoursample.Theblacklinesindicate95percentconfidenceintervals.Thetypesofnamechangesare:FemaletoFemale(FF), FemaletoMale(FM),MaletoFemale(MF),andMaletoMale(MM).GraphAaggregatesFMwithMFintoatransgendercategoryandFFwithMMintoacisgender category.

Both cisgender and transgender applicants disclosed the name changeintheircoverletter.Thestatedreasonfordisclosingthis infor-mationwasthatsomeoftheapplicants’documentation,suchas refer-encesorgrades,wouldbeintheoldnameandtheprospectiveemployer shouldbeawareofthisifgoingforwardwiththeapplication.The de-signmimickedtheway,forexample,newlymarriedpeopledealwitha surnamechangeinofficialmattersuntiltheirdocumentationisupdated withthenewsurname—theysimplyprovideboththeformerand cur-rentname.Inourcase,theapplicantschangedtheirgivennamesand, therefore,informedtheemployeraboutboththeformerandcurrent givenname.

Atthis pointitisworth mentioning thatitis notuncommon for Swedish people to change their given names, even for reasons that havenothingtodowith genderchange.Accordingtodatafromthe Swedishtaxagency,95,090peopleinSwedenofficiallychangedtheir

givennamebetween2013and2018(owncalculationsbasedondata re-ceivedfromtheSwedishTaxAgency).Somepeopleinformallychange thenametheyuseforeverydaypurposes,prénomusuel,toanotherof theirgivennames.Itis,therefore,altogetherreasonabletoarguethat employersareaccustomedtopeoplechangingtheirnames.Alongwith thefactthatapplicantsinourstudygenerallyexperiencedhigherrates ofpositiveemployerresponsesthandidapplicantsinpreviousstudies, makesusconfidentthatourcisgenderapplicantswerenotseenasless employableorodderbecauseoftheirnamechange.

Acritiquesometimesleviedatcorrespondencestudiesisthatnames carrymoresignalsthantheresearchermayintend.Anexampleisthat socioeconomicsignalsmayaffecttheresults ofcorrespondence stud-iesofracediscriminationifnamesareusedtosignalgroupbelonging (FryerandLevitt,2004).Toaddressthispotential issue,eachof our applicationshadtworandomlyassignedcommonSwedishfirstnames

(5)

drawnfromalistoffiveforeachgender(femalenames:Amanda,Elin, Emma,Hanna,Julia;malenames:Anton,Erik,Filip,Oscar,Simon).The nameswerepickedfromalistofthefivemostcommonSwedishbaby namesforeach genderin1998.15 Thisensuredthatany signals con-tainedwithinthesenameswerenotheldconstantwithinan experimen-talcondition(addingfixedeffectsforthesenamesdoesnotaffectoverall results,seeSupplementaryMaterial,TableA14).Forexample,boththe cisgenderandtransgenderapplicantcouldhavehadthesameSwedish name,forinstance,Simon.Theonlydifferencewasthatthenamethey statedtohavechangedfromwasoneoftheothermale(forcisgender) orfemale(fortransgender)names.Thismeansourexperimentdidnot restontheuseofsomespecificnametosignalgroupbelongingasin many otherstudies. Yet,we exploitedthegender saliencyof names toclearlycommunicateapplicants’genderidentity.Bothcisgenderand transgenderapplicantsweregiventhefifthmostcommonSwedish sur-name:Eriksson.Usingthesecommoncombinationsofnamesensured thatourapplicantsweredifficulttofindin searchesonsocialmedia platformsandinotheronlinesourcesofpersonalinformation.

2.3. TheHeckman-Siegelmancritiqueandrandomizationofobservables

Besidesrandomizingapplicants’genderidentity,fourother observ-ableswererandomizedintheCVs:(i)thenumberofyearsofeach ap-plicant’srelevantworkexperience,rangingfromoneyeartonine;(ii) whethertheapplicantwascapableofusingacomputer,(iii)whetherthe applicantwasproficientinbothSwedishandEnglish;and(iv)whether theapplicanthadinterestsandhobbiesthatimplyphysicalactivity out-sideofwork. Previousstudieshaveincludedvariationsinadditional informationsuchaslanguageskills(LeeandKhalid,2016),personality characteristics(Drydakis,2014),computerskills,andworkexperience (BertrandandMullainathan,2004).Inthisstudywerandomizedallfour ofthesevariablessothatanapplicationcouldcontainanycombination ofskills.

VariationinskillswasrequiredtouseNeumark’smethodtorespond toHeckmanandSiegelman’s(1993)critique.Theessentialpartofthe critiqueis thatestimates fromfieldexperimentalstudiesinthelabor market can be biasedby differences in thevariances of unobserved productivityacross groupsevenifobservableproductivityindicators arekeptconstantacrossexperimentalconditions.Weacknowledgethat differentialtreatmentbased onassumptions aboutdifferences in the variancesbetweengroupsshouldstillbeconsidereddiscrimination,as stereotyping isessentially assumingavery peakedprobability distri-butionfor theproductivityof astereotyped group.Indeed, such dif-ferentialtreatmentbasedonthevarianceinunobservedproductivity issometimescalledsecond-momentstatisticaldiscriminationHowever, theproblemisthatcorrespondencestudyestimatesarebiased depend-ingonthequalityoftheCVsusedinthestudyandhowthatquality relatestothethresholdofacceptanceagainstwhichitisbeingjudged. Bothoftheseareverydifficulttocontrolfor(forsomeintuitive exam-plesseeHeckman,1998).

NeumarkandRich(2018)reanalyzeddatafromsomeearlier cor-respondence studies and found that accounting for the Heckman-SiegelmancritiqueusingNeumark’smethodchangedtheresultsfora fewofthere-examinedstudies.Neumark’smethod disaggregatesthe estimateofdiscriminationintoalevelpartwhichincludesboth taste-basedandfirst-moment statisticaldiscriminationandavariancepart whichisduetosecond-momentstatisticaldiscrimination.Weusedthis method as a robustness check. We used the same Stata code as in Neumarketal.(2016).

Thesetofskillswasgeneralenough tohave someapplicationto eachoccupationinthestudy.Wedidnotwanttousedifferentskillsfor

15 Source: Statistics Sweden. Available at:

https://www.scb.se/hitta-statistik/statistik-efter-amne/befolkning/amnesovergripande-statistik/ namnstatistik/

Table2

PositiveEmployerResponseRates– FullSample

Positive response FF MM FM MF No 302 337 380 381 (57.97) (61.27) (66.43) (65.58) Yes 432 213 192 200 (40.34) (38.73) (33.57) (34.42) 𝜒2 (1, 𝑁 = 2 , 224 ) = 10.908, 𝑝 = . 012 Cisgender Transgender No 639 761 (59.66) (66.00) Yes 432 392 (40.34) (34.00) 𝜒2 (1, 𝑁 = 2 , 224 ) = 9.562, 𝑝 = . 002 Note.—Percentagesin parentheses.Thep-valueswere pro-ducedwiththePearson𝜒2-test.

differentoccupationsbecauseitwouldmakeitdifficulttoaccountfor theHeckman-Siegelmancritiqueacrossoccupations.Further,thereare notenoughjobpostingswithinmostoccupationsinSwedentoallowfor useofNeumark’smethodwithinsingleoccupations,unlessdatawereto becollectedforaverylongtime.

3. AnalysisandResults

AswecanseeinPanelAofFigure1,therewasadifferencein posi-tiveemployerresponsesbetweencisgenderandtransgenderapplicants. PanelBshowsthepositiveemployerresponseratesforallfourtypesof namechanges:male-to-female(MF),female-to-male(FM),male-to-male (MM)andfemale-to-female(FF).Itisevidentthattherewaslittletono differenceinmeanpositiveemployerresponsesbetweentheMFandFM categoriesandsomedifferencesbetweenMMandFFcategories.The dif-ferencesbetweenMMandFFwasnotunexpected;conventionalsex dis-criminationisalargefieldofstudybutoutsideourscope.Inthisstudy, however,weneededtodecidewhichoftheconventionalgender cate-gories,MMandFF,weshoulduseasareferencegroupintheanalyses. WecouldincludeacontrolforeitherMMorFF,leavingtheotherasthe referencegroup,andarriveatdifferentlevelsfordifferencesinpositive employerresponsestotransgenderapplicants.Alternatively,wecould abstainfromincludingsuchcontrolsandhaveourestimatereflectthe differencebetweenthemeanpositiveemployerresponserateforMM andFFapplicantstogether,compared totransgenderapplicants.Our solutionwastodobothwheneverpossible:Wegeneratedresultsusing bothabinarycisgender/transgenderspecificationandafour-category MM/FF/MF/FMspecificationwiththehighestmeanresponserategroup asthereferencecategory,whichwasusuallyFFexceptwhenwe subdi-videdthedatabygenderratioinoccupation.

3.1. Overalldiscriminationagainsttransgenderapplicants

Table2showstherawpositiveemployerresponseratesforalltypes of applicantsandtheresultsof 𝜒2-tests ofno differencesin positive

employerresponseratesacrossapplicantswithdifferent gender iden-tities.16Ifourrandomizationofgenderidentitywassuccessful,lower positiveemployerresponseratesfortransgenderapplicantsshouldbe anestimateoftheaverageeffectoftransgenderstatusonreceivinga positive employerresponse.Theprocedureweusedwhengenerating theCVsandassigningthemtovacanciesensured thattreatmentwas randomlyassigned,ascorroboratedbyatestforbalanceofcovariates betweenourtreatmentandtheobservablesinourdata(see Supplemen-taryMaterial,TableA1).

16Therewasatypoinourpreregistrationstatingthatwewouldbeperforming t-testsofourhypotheses.Thiswasneverourintention,butweperformedall relevanttestsast-testsaswell.Resultswerenotdifferenttoanyappreciable degreeandarepresentedintheSupplementaryMaterial(TableA8).

(6)

Table3 Variables

Variables Explanation

Outcome variable

Positive response 1 if the applicant received a positive employer response, 0 otherwise Treatment variable

Transgender 1 if the applicant changed gender with name change, 0 otherwise

Skill variables

Work experience 1-9 indicating the years of relevant work experience in application

Square of work exp. Work experience times work experience

Computer skills 1 if the applicant claimed to have computer skills, 0 otherwise

Language skills 1 if the applicant claimed to have skills in Swedish and English, 0 otherwise Active lifestyle 1 if the applicant claimed to have an active lifestyle, 0 otherwise

Occupation level control variables

Occupation fixed effects Fixed effect dummies for each of the twelve occupations Gender ratio Takes the values from Table 1 , column 2 for each occupation

High face-to-face customer interaction (FFCI) 1 if occupation was classified as typically requiring high amounts of face-to-face customer interaction, 0 otherwise Vacancy controls

Full time work 1 if job posting was offering full time work, 0 otherwise

Indefinite contract length 1 if job posting was offering work with no determined period length, 0 otherwise Month fixed effects Fixed effect dummies for each month of data collection

Urban 1 if job posting was for work in one of the three greater metropolitan areas of Sweden, 0 otherwise

Note.—Thistablecontainsdefinitionsofthevariablesusedinregressions.Naturally,occupationFEandotheroccupation-levelvariables(genderratioandFFCI)are neversimultaneouslyincludedinregressions.

Figure2. PredictedMarginalPositiveEmployerResponseRatesAcrossWorkExperience.Note.—GraphAshowsthepredictedprobabilityofapositiveemployer responseforcisgenderandtransgenderapplicantsgivendifferentamountsofworkexperience.Theunderlyingprobitmodelincludesthefullsetofcontrolsdetailed inTable3.GraphBshowsdifferencesinmarginaleffectsbetweencisgenderandtransgenderapplicantsandthe95percentconfidenceintervalforthatdifference. Notethedifferencesiny-axisscales.SeeSupplementaryMaterialTableA3forprobitestimates.

Overallandinrawfigures,transgenderapplicantsreceivedpositive employerresponsesin34.0percentofthecasesandcisgenderapplicants receivedpositiveemployerresponsesin40.3percentofthecases.There wasastatisticallysignificant6.3percentagepointpenaltyinpositive employerresponserateforbeingtransgender,𝜒2(1,𝑁=2,224)=9.56, 𝑝=.002.17 Inotherwords, anapplication fromacisgenderapplicant was18.6percentmorelikelytoreceiveapositiveemployerresponse thananapplicationfromatransgenderapplicant.

Tocontrolforpotentialconfoundingfactors,weusedaprobitmodel toestimatethemarginaleffectofbeingtransgenderontheprobability

17 Asthisisthefirsttimewereportedstatisticalsignificanceinthispaper,it

isworthemphasizingthatweusedstricterthresholdsforstatisticalsignificance thanusual(inthespiritofBenjaminetal.,2018).Wheneverp-valuesarenot reported,oneasteriskwill,therefore,indicatesignificanceatthe5percentlevel, twoasteriskswillindicatesignificanceatthe1percentlevel,andthreeasterisks willindicatesignificanceatthe0.1percentlevelofsignificance.

ofreceivingapositiveemployerresponse.Differentsetsofcontrol vari-ableswereusedineachmodeltoexaminetherobustnessofourresults. Insomemodels,fixedeffectswereusedtofullycontrolfortheoretically importantconfoundingvariablesattheoccupationlevelsuchas gen-derratioandface-to-facecustomerinteraction.Theprobitwasofthe followingform:

Pr(𝑦𝑖=1|𝑥𝑖,zi,𝛾𝑖)=Φ(𝑥𝑖𝛽 +𝑧𝑖𝛼 +γ′𝑖𝜃),

wherethedependentvariableyiwasadummytakingthevalue1ifthe applicationreceivedapositiveemployerresponseand0otherwise.xi wasavectorofexplanatoryvariables,ziwasavectorofproductivity relevantskills,and𝛾iwasavectorofothercontrolvariables.Thefull listofvariableswiththeirdefinitionsispresentedinTable3.

Table4presentsprobitestimatesofthemarginaleffect(atmeans) from disclosing transgender statusin applications. The estimates in panelAshowthattransgenderapplicantshadbetween6.3and7.1 per-centage pointslowerprobability ofreceivingapositiveemployer

(7)

re-Table4

ProbitEstimatesofDiscriminationAgainstTransgenderPeople

(1) (2) (3) (4)

Panel A: Cisgender and transgender specification

Trans -0.0632 ∗∗ (0.0206) -0.0646 ∗∗ (0.0207) -0.0700 ∗∗∗ (0.0210) -0.0707 ∗∗∗ (0.0211) Panel B: Four gender identities specification

FF REF REF REF REF

MM -0.0319 (0.0294) -0.0305 (0.0295) -0.0339 (0.0298) -0.0310 (0.0299) MF -0.0789 ∗∗ (0.0292) -0.0793 ∗∗ (0.0293) -0.0839 ∗∗ (0.0298) -0.0827 ∗∗ (0.0299) FM -0.0800 ∗∗ (0.0294) -0.0809 ∗∗ (0.0294) -0.0907 ∗∗ (0.0299) -0.0903 ∗∗ (0.0300) Skills Experience 0.0359 ∗ (0.0182) 0.0362 ∗ (0.0183) 0.0355 (0.0186) 0.0362 (0.0186) Experience 2 -0.0027 (0.0018) -0.0027 (0.0018) -0.0027 (0.0018) -0.0028 (0.0018) Computer -0.0026 (0.0018) (0.0018) -0.0027 (0.0018) -0.0027 (0.0018) -0.0028 Language 0.0063 (0.0206) 0.0055 (0.0207) 0.0042 (0.0210) 0.0034 (0.0210) Active 0.0089 (0.0206) 0.0109 (0.0207) 0.0208 (0.0210) 0.0226 (0.0211) Other controls

FFCI and gender ratio No Yes No No

Month FE No Yes No Yes

Occupation FE No No Yes Yes

Observations 2,224 2,224 2,224 2,224

Note.—Thistablereportstheestimatesofdiscriminationagainsttransgenderpeopleusingprobitmodels withmarginaleffects(atmeans)anddifferentsetsofcontrolvariables.PanelAshowsthemaineffectof interestformodelswithacisgenderandtransgendercategorization.PanelBshowsboththemainandskill effectswhenusingmodelswithfourgendercategories,withFFasthereferencegroup.Vacancycontrols areincludedinallmodels.Standarderrorsinparentheses.“FE” isshortforfixedeffectsand“FFCI” isshort forface-to-facecustomerinteraction.VariablesareexplainedinmoredetailinTable3.

p< .05. ∗∗p<.01. ∗∗∗p<.001.

sponsethancisgenderapplicants. Additionally,inthemostcomplete models(Models3–4)theeffectisstatisticallysignificantatthe0.1 per-centlevel.Theresultsforthefour-gendercategorizationinpanelBare alsostableacrossmodelspecifications.Becausethereferencegroupin panelBisthemostfavoredgender,FF,thepointestimatesareofcourse slightlylargerinabsolutetermsthaninpanelA.TheestimatesinPanel BforMFandFMareallstatisticallysignificantatthe1percentlevel.

TheresultsinTable4lineupwellwiththesimpletestspresented inTable2andreaffirmthatcisgenderapplicantsenjoyedatleasta20 percentadvantageinpositiveemployerresponserates over transgen-derapplicants.AnotherinterestingfindinginTable4isthattheonly skillemployersseemedtohavevaluedwasworkexperience,whichhad asmallbutstatisticallysignificantpositivemarginaleffectonpositive employerresponseratesinsomemodelspecifications(skillestimatesare onlyshownforthefour-gendercategorizationspecificationsinthetable, butweresimilarforthebinarymodel).Thethreeotherskills—computer skills,languageskills,andanactivelifestyle—allshowedstatistically in-significanteffectsonpositiveemployerresponserates.

Weproceededwithouranalysisbydividingthesampleintothree categoriesbased ontherandomizedworkexperiencefor each appli-cant,low(1–3years),medium(4–6years),andhigh(7–9years).We wanted toexamine whether discriminationdiminished as the infor-mation provided about the applicants’ qualificationsincreased. This wouldbethecaseiftheobserveddifferencesinpositiveemployer re-sponserateswereduetostatisticaldiscrimination.Wefoundthat trans-genderapplicantswithlowandmediumamountsofexperiencefaced no statistically significant discrimination—𝜒2(1,𝑁=713)=2.45,𝑝= .118,and𝜒2(1,𝑁=756)=2.54,𝑝=.111,respectively—while

transgen-derapplicantswithhighexperiencedid,𝜒2(1,𝑁=755)=4.75,𝑝=.029.

InFigure2,weplottedthepredictedprobabilitiesofpositiveemployer responsesforcisgenderandtransgenderapplicantsfordifferentamounts ofworkexperience,estimatedusingaprobitmodel.Thefigureclearly showsthatthedisparatetreatmentwhichtransgenderapplicantsfaced asregardspositiveemployerresponserates didnot diminishastheir reportedworkexperienceincreased.Takentogether,boththe𝜒2-tests

andtheprobitmodelshowninFigure2providenoevidenceof statisti-caldiscrimination.18

Next,we investigatedifthere wereanypatternsin thedatathat wereconsistentwithtaste-baseddiscrimination.Wedidthisby split-tingthesampleintotwosubsamples,onewithoccupationswhich re-quiredagreatdealofface-to-facecustomerinteraction(FFCI)and an-otherwithlowFFCIoccupations(Table1).Weobtainedsometentative evidencethatemployersmayhaveactedontheircustomers’tastesas discriminationwas onlystatistically significantin highFFCI occupa-tions,𝜒2(1,𝑁=1,283)=7.65,𝑝=.006,andnotinlowFFCI

occupa-tions,𝜒2(1,𝑁=941)=2.36,𝑝=.125.19However,whenweestimated alinearprobabilitymodel(LPM)ofaspecificationsimilartoModel2

18SeeSupplementaryMaterial,TableA3,forregressionestimates,TableA4for 𝜒2-tests,andFigureA10foragraphicalillustrationofpositiveresponseratesfor

allfourgendercategoriesacrossworkexperience.Allinlinewiththeconclusion thatoveralldiscriminationwasnotstatistical.

19TableA5(PanelA)intheSupplementaryMaterialshowsthese𝜒2-testsmore

(8)

Table5

Neumark’sMethod,AddressingtheHeckman-SiegelmanCritique

(1) Transgender and Cisgender (2) FF control included (3) MM control included

Reference category MM and FF MM FF

Panel A: Probit estimates

Trans -0.0707 ∗∗∗ -0.0555 -0.0865 ∗∗∗

(0.0211) (0.0257) (0.0260)

Panel B: Heteroskedastic probit estimates

Trans -0.0770 ∗∗∗ -0.0594 -0.0953 ∗∗∗ (0.0215) (0.0273) (0.0280) Trans-level -0.0375 -0.0199 -0.0558 (0.0277) (0.0333) (0.0322) Trans-variance -0.0395 ∗ -0.0395 -0.0395 ∗ (0.0201) (0.0201) (0.0201) Panel C: Tests

Ratio S.D of unobservables (Trans/cis) 0.7439 ∗ 0.7440 0.7440

Diagnostic tests (p-values)

Test S.D. ratio = 1 0.0230 ∗ 0.0232 0.0232

Overidentification test 0.6531 0.6512 0.6512

LR test: Probit vs. Heteroskedastic Probit 0.0460 ∗ 0.0462 0.0462

Controls

Skills and vacancy Yes Yes Yes

Month FE Yes Yes Yes

Occupation FE Yes Yes Yes

Observations 2,224 2,224 2,224

Note.—InpanelAandBmarginaleffects(atmeans)arereported.PanelBreportstheestimatesofdiscriminationagainst transgenderpeopleusingNeumark’smethodofaddressingtheHeckman-Siegelmancritique.Theincludedskill,vacancy,and FEcontrolsarethesameinallmodelsandequivalenttothemodelspecificationinModel4ofTable4.Models2and3inthis tableshowthattheNeumarkresultsarerobusttoincludingcontrolsforeitherMMorFFapplicants,i.e.changingthereference group.Standarderrorsinparentheses.“FE” isshortforfixedeffects,“LR” isshortforLikelihoodRatio,and“FFCI” isshortfor face-to-facecustomerinteraction.VariablesareexplainedinmoredetailinTable3.

p<.05. ∗∗∗p< .001.

inTable4wefoundnosupportforaninteractioneffectbetweenthe transgenderdummyandtheFFCIdummy(𝛽 =.023,𝑝=.579).20,21

Table5presentstheresultsofapplyingNeumark’smethodtoour datafocusingonthemostcompletespecificationinTable4(Model4). Neumark’smethodusesaheteroskedasticprobitmodelanda decompo-sitiontoestimatehowmuchoftheheteroskedasticprobitestimateis at-tributabletodifferencesinthevarianceofunobservablesandhowmuch isnot(“level”).FormoredetailsonthemethodseeNeumark(2012). PanelAreiteratestheestimatesshowninTable4(PanelA).Inpanel BweshowtheestimatedmarginaleffectswhenemployingNeumark’s method.Theestimatesoftheeffectofvariancewerestatistically signif-icantatthe5percentlevel,andunaffectedbywhetherwecompared tocisgendermaleandfemaleapplicants,cisgendermaleapplicants,or cisgenderfemaleapplicants.Becausethelevelestimateswerenot sig-nificant,we concludethatourestimate ofoveralldiscriminationwas notrobusttotheHeckman-Siegelmancritique.Wefoundthatthe vari-anceofunobservableswasconsistentlysmallerfortransgender appli-cants(PanelC,firstrow),whichsuggestedthattheestimatesofoverall discriminationwerebiased.Indeed,PanelCofTable5showsthatthe likelihoodratiotestrejectedtheconventionalprobitmodelinfavorof theheteroskedasticprobitmodelatthe5percentlevelofsignificance. WeconcludethatourestimatesofdiscriminationinTable4werebiased.

20 Followingareviewercomment,weperformedsimilaranalysiscategorizing

occupationssubjectivelybywhatlevelofteamworktheytendtorequiretesting forco-workerdiscrimination.Theresultsof𝜒2-testsindicatedsignificant

dis-criminationinbothtypesofoccupationswhenclassifiedbyteamworkandthe interactioninalinearprobabilitymodelwasnotsignificant(𝛽 =.032,𝑝=.440). Weconcludethatwefoundnosupportforthisexploratoryhypothesis,which couldbebecausetheoccupationswerenotaseasilydividedbyteamworkasthey werebyFFCI(FFCIwaspartofourpre-registrationandaconsiderationwhen pickingoccupations,teamworkwasnot).Moreextensiveresultsareavailable uponrequest.

21 AllresultsbasedonLPMsinthisstudyareavailablefromthecorresponding

authoruponrequest.

Therobustmarginalchangeintheprobabilityofreceivingapositive employerresponsewas3.75percentagepointslowerforatransgender applicant thanacisgenderapplicant. Thiseffectwas notstatistically significant.

3.2. Discriminationagainsttransgenderapplicantsinoccupationswith differentgenderratios

Wenowcontinueinaccordancewithourpreregistrationplanand explore any heterogeneityin discrimination across occupationswith differentgenderratios.Wedivideoursampleintothreecategoriesby occupationalgenderratio,maledominated,mixed,andfemale domi-nated(categorizationbasedondatafromStatisticsSwedenandshown in Table1).Wedidnotfindasignificant differencebetween cisgen-derandtransgenderapplicantsinpositiveemployerresponseratesin female-dominatedoccupations,𝜒2(1,𝑁=669)=.61,𝑝=.435,norin

mixed-gender occupations,𝜒2(1,𝑁=935) =2.35,𝑝=.126).The

dif-ferenceinmale-dominatedoccupationswas,however,clear. Transgen-der applicantswere12.3percentagepointslesslikely thancisgender applicantstoreceiveapositiveemployerresponseinmale-dominated occupations,𝜒2(1,𝑁=620)=10.15,𝑝=.001.22 Inotherwords,this differencesuggeststhatcisgenderapplicantswereabout41.3percent morelikelythantransgenderapplicantstoreceiveapositiveemployer responseinmale-dominatedoccupations.Theheterogeneityin discrim-inationacrossoccupationswithdifferentgenderratioswasevidenteven whenwecontrolledforotherfactors.Thisisshowngraphicallyforthe cisgender/transgender specificationusing probit modelsin Figure3. Figure3showsthatthepredictedprobability ofapositive employer responseforcisgenderandtransgenderapplicantsconvergedasthe fe-maleproportionacrossoccupationsmovedtowardsunity.

22TableA5(PanelB)intheSupplementaryMaterialshowsthese𝜒2-testsmore

(9)

Figure3.PositiveEmployerResponseRatesAcross OccupationalGenderRatio.Note.—GraphAshowsthe predictedprobabilityofapositiveemployerresponse forcisgenderandtransgenderapplicantsgiventhe dif-ferentpercentagesofwomenintheoccupation.The underlyingprobitmodelincludesthefullsetof con-trolsdetailedinTable3,includingasquaretermfor femaleworkforcebutexcludingoccupationfixed ef-fects.Graph Bshowsdifferencesinmarginaleffects betweencisgenderandtransgenderapplicantsandthe 95percentconfidenceintervalforthatdifference.Note thedifferencesiny-axisscales.Onthex-axesthe dif-ferentoccupationsinthe studyare indicated.Refer toTable1forwhattheseabbreviationsmean.Three occupationsstartwiththeletterCandtherefore war-rantmorecarefulexplanationhere,theyareChef(C), Cleaner(Cl),andChildcare(Chi).SeeSupplementary MaterialTableA3forprobitestimates.

Figure4.PositiveEmployerResponseRatesforDifferent GenderIdentities AcrossOccupationalGenderRatio.Note.—Thisgraphshowsthepredicted prob-abilityofapositiveemployerresponseforapplicantsgiventhedifferent percent-agesofwomeninanoccupation.Theunderlyingprobitmodelincludesthefull setofcontrolsdetailedinTable3,includingasquaretermforfemaleworkforce butexcludingoccupationfixedeffects.Onthex-axisthedifferentoccupations inthestudyareindicated.RefertoTable1forwhattheseabbreviationsmean. ThreeoccupationsstartwiththeletterCandthereforewarrantmorecareful explanationhere,theyareChef(C),Cleaner(Cl),andChildcare(Chi).See Sup-plementaryMaterialTableA3forprobitestimates.

Based on Figure 3 we may conclude that employers in female-dominatedoccupationsdidnotdiscriminateagainsttransgender appli-cants.Therealitywas,however,more nuanced.Whenwe examined thefour-categoryspecification,wefoundconsiderableheterogeneityin howemployerstreatedtheapplicantsacrossoccupationsdependingon whethertheyweremale-orfemale-dominated.Figure4displaysthe pre-dictedprobabilitiesofapositiveemployerresponseforallfourtypesof applicantsacrossoccupationalgenderratios.Inmale-dominated occu-pations,cisgendermaleapplicantsstoodoutasthemostpreferred. Cis-genderfemaleandtransgendermaleapplicantswerelesslikelythan cis-gendermaleapplicantstoreceiveapositiveemployerresponse. Trans-genderfemaleapplicantswereleastlikelytoreceiveapositiveemployer response.Infemale-dominatedoccupations,discriminationseemedto bemostlybasedonapplicants’gender.Femaleapplicants(FFandMF)

didbetterintermsofpositiveemployerresponsesthandidmale appli-cants(MMandFM).

We also estimated separate probit models for male-dominated, mixed, and female-dominated occupations. Panel A of Table 6 cor-responds to what was visually shown in Figure 3, and Panel B of Table6correspondstoFigure4.Table6showsstatisticallysignificant discriminationagainsttransgenderfemaleapplicantsinmale-dominated occupationsandagainstcisgenderandtransgendermaleapplicantsin female-dominatedoccupations.Notethatthetypeofapplicantwechose tosetasareferencepointintheregressionspresentedinTable6was theapplicantthathadhadthehighestprobabilityofreceivinga posi-tiveemployerresponseinagiventypeofoccupation(i.e.,MMin male-dominatedandFFinfemale-dominatedoccupations).

Next, we examined whether there were any signs of statistical andcustomer discriminationin male-andfemale-dominated occupa-tions. Mixedoccupationswerenotexaminedsinceno discrimination wasfoundintheseoccupationsintheprecedingstepof theanalysis. Wefoundnoevidenceofstatisticaldiscriminationinmale-dominated occupations as disparate treatment did not seem to diminish with increased experience. There was no statistically significant discrimi-nation among less experiencedapplicants, 𝜒2(1,𝑁=206)=1.74,𝑝= .187, while therewas discriminationamongapplicantswithmedium andhigh amountsof experience, 𝜒2(1,𝑁=210)=4.31,𝑝=.038, and 𝜒2(1,𝑁=204)=5.31,𝑝=.021, respectively. The interactionbetween

the transgender dummy and experience was not significant in an LPMspecification(𝛽 =.007,𝑝=.634).Similarresults werefoundin female-dominatedoccupations.Theinteractionbetweenthe transgen-derdummyandlevelofexperiencewasnotsignificantinanLPM spec-ification(𝛽 =.010,𝑝=.471).Foranintuitivegraphicalillustrationof theseresults,seeFigureA11intheSupplementaryMaterial.Insum,we foundthatthedisparatetreatmentfacedbytransgenderapplicantsdoes notdiminishasreportedworkexperienceincreased,eitherinmale-or infemale-dominatedoccupations.

Intermsofcustomerdiscrimination,resultsformale-dominated oc-cupationsweresimilartothosewefoundearlierforoverall discrimi-nation.DiscriminationwassignificantonlyinhighFFCIoccupations,

𝜒2(1,𝑁=230)=7.67,𝑝=.006,andnotinlowFFCIoccupations,𝜒2(1, 𝑁=390)=3.250,𝑝=.071.Yet,usinganLPM,wefoundnosupportfor aninteractionbetweenthetransgenderdummyandtheFFCIdummy (𝛽 =.083,𝑝=.294).Similarly,therewasnointeractioneffectbetween thetransgenderdummyandtheFFCIdummyinfemale-dominated oc-cupations(𝛽 =.051,𝑝=.626).

Lastly,weusedNeumark’smethodtocheckiftheestimatesof dis-criminationagainsttransgenderpeople inoccupations withdifferent genderratioswererobusttotheHeckman-Siegelmancritique.Table7 showsthatthevarianceofunobservablesdidnotbiastheestimatesin male-dominatedoccupations.Infemale-dominatedoccupationswecan

(10)

Table6

ProbitEstimatesofDiscriminationAcrossOccupationswithDifferentGenderRatios (1) Male-dominated (2) Mixed (3) Female-dominated

Reference category MM MM FF

Panel A: Cisgender and transgender

Trans -0.1629 ∗∗∗ -0.0472 -0.1313 ∗∗

(0.0474) (0.0400) (0.0466)

Panel B: Four gender identities

FF -0.0726 -0.0190 REF (0.0561) (0.0469) MM REF REF -0.1905 ∗∗∗ (0.0566) MF -0.2339 ∗∗∗ -0.0318 -0.1036 (0.0575) (0.0460) (0.0530) FM -0.1051 -0.0638 -0.1614 ∗∗ (0.0537) (0.0470) (0.0543) Controls

Skills and vacancy Yes Yes Yes

FFCI and gender ratio No No No

Month FE Yes Yes Yes

Occupation FE Yes Yes Yes

Observations 620 935 669

Note.—Probitmarginaleffects(atmeans).Thistablereportstheestimatesof dis-criminationagainsttransgenderpeopleinoccupationswithdifferentgenderratios. Allmodelsusethesamecontrolspecificationasthefullmodelin(Model4)inTable4. Standarderrorsinparentheses.“FE” isshortforfixedeffectsand“FFCI” isshortfor face-to-facecustomerinteraction.VariablesareexplainedinmoredetailinTable3.

∗∗p<.01. ∗∗∗p< .001.

Table7

Neumark’sMethodforOccupationswithDifferentGenderRatios

(1) Male-dominated (2) Mixed (3) Female-dominated

Reference category MM MM FF

Panel A: Probit estimates

Trans -0.1629 ∗∗∗ -0.0472 -0.1313 ∗∗

(0.0474) (0.0400) (0.0466)

Panel B: Heteroskedastic probit estimates

Trans -0.1618 ∗∗ -0.0632 -0.1799 ∗∗ (0.0493) (0.0437) (0.0634) Trans-level -0.1651 ∗∗ -0.0055 -0.0828 (0.0550) (0.0485) (0.0652) Trans-variance 0.0032 -0.0577 ∗ -0.0971 (0.0424) (0.0277) (0.0612) Panel C: Tests

Ratio S.D of unobservables (Trans/cis) 1.0213 0.6471 ∗∗ 0.4848 ∗ Diagnostic tests (p-values)

Test S.D. ratio = 1 0.9396 0.0074 ∗∗ 0.0109

Overidentification test 0.9195 0.6615 0.6078

LR test: Probit vs. Heteroskedastic Probit 0.9389 0.0258 ∗ 0.0505

Observations 620 935 669

Note.—InpanelAandBmarginaleffects(atmeans)arereported.Thistablereportstheestimatesof dis-criminationagainsttransgenderpeopleusingNeumark’smethodofaddressingtheHeckman-Siegelman critiqueforeachgenderratiosubsample.Vacancy,occupationfixedeffects,monthfixedeffects,and skillcontrolsareincludedinallmodels.Standarderrorsinparentheses.“LR” isshortforLikelihood Ratio.

p< .05. ∗∗p<.01.

seeinpanelCofTable7,thereisevidenceforadifferenceinvariance be-tweenfemaleandtransgenderapplicants(firstandsecondrowofpanel Cincolumn3).However,inpanelBweseefromthedecomposition thatthevariancetermisnotsignificantandthereforedidnotaffectthe results,leadingustothesameconclusionasinmale-dominated occupa-tions.However,inmixedoccupationsthevarianceestimatewas signif-icant,indicatingthatthereeitherwasdiscriminationonthevarianceof unobservablesinthosesectorsorourCVsmayhavebeenless represen-tativeintermsofskillsinthoseoccupations.Weconcludethatwhilethe levelofourCVspotentiallyintroducedabiasattheaggregatelevel(as showninTable5)theywererepresentativeenoughwithinmale-and

female-dominatedoccupationsnottoinfluencetheestimates.Itseems thatthefailureoftheestimatetowithstandtheHeckman-Siegelman cri-tiquemaybeduetomixedoccupations,wheretherewasless discrimina-tiongenerally.TheLR-testsdonotrejectthehomoskedasticrestriction inthefemale-ormale-dominatedoccupations,sowebaseour conclu-sionsontheestimatesinpanelA.Weconcludethattransgendermale andfemaleapplicantssuffered a16.3percentagepointpenalty com-paredtocisgendermaleapplicantsinmale-dominatedoccupationsanda 13.1percentagepointpenaltycomparedtocisgenderfemaleapplicants infemale-dominatedoccupations.Thismeansthat,inmale-dominated occupations, anapplication froma cisgendermalewas 82.6percent

(11)

morelikelytoreceiveapositiveemployerresponsethanan applica-tionfromatransgendermaleorfemale.Thecorrespondingfigurefor applicationsfromacisgenderfemaleinfemale-dominatedoccupations was52.8comparedtoatransgendermaleorfemale.

3.3. Robustnesschecksandcomplementaryanalyses

IntheSupplementaryMaterialswereportsomeadditional robust-nesschecks. Wetested forbalanceofcovariatestovalidatethe ran-domizationofgenderidentityvariableandfoundthatitwassuccessful (TableA1).Weshowanexampleofanapplicationletter(ExampleA2). WereporttheprobitestimatesunderlyingFigures2–4(TableA3).In TablesA4–A5,weshowmorecompleteoutputformanyofthe𝜒2-tests

wediscussedearlier.Weperformedsurvivalanalysestoexplorewhether positiveemployerresponsetimingdifferedbetweencisgenderand trans-genderapplicantsandfoundnodifferencesinresponsetimes(TableA6 andFigureA7).Further,weverifiedtherobustnessofthe𝜒2-testsby

re-examiningthetestswithanyambiguousresponseallcodedaspositive andthenallasnegative.Wefoundthatthisdidnotaffecttheresults (Ta-bleA8PanelA)nordidsuchrecodingaffecttheprobitestimatesmuch (TableA8PanelB).Weshow estimatesofdiscriminationindifferent workexperiencesubsamples(TableA9),andproducedafiguresimilar toFigure4,butforworkexperience(FigureA10),bothofwhich(Table A9andFigureA10)corroboratethestoryofFigure2.Inotherwords, thereis nostatistical discrimination.FigureA11reaffirmsthatthere wasnosupportforstatisticaldiscriminationwithinmale-dominatedor female-dominatedoccupations.FiguresA12andA13depictthe signif-icanceofdifferencesacrossworkexperienceandgenderratio respec-tively.TableA14showshowestimateswereunaffectedbyusing differ-entspatialcontrolspecifications,andfixedeffectsforapplicantname (currentorpast).

4. DiscussionandConclusion

Thispaperhaspresentedtheresultsofthefirstcorrespondencetest ofdiscriminationagainsttransgenderpeopleinthelabor market.We havedocumentedseveralimportantfindings.Wedidnotfind signifi-cantevidenceforoveralldiscriminationagainsttransgenderapplicants whenweconsideredtheHeckman-Siegelmancritique.However,when weexaminedoccupationswithdifferentgenderratios,anotherpicture emerged.Wefoundthatbothmale-andfemale-dominatedoccupations exhibited highratesof discriminationagainsttransgenderapplicants whilemixedoccupationsdidnot.Therewasa16.3percentagepoint penaltyfortransgendermaleandfemaleapplicantscomparedto cis-gendermaleapplicantsinmale-dominatedoccupationsandtherewas a13.1percentagepointpenaltyfortransgendermaleandfemale ap-plicantscomparedtocisgenderfemaleapplicantsinfemale-dominated occupationsasregardspositiveemployerresponserate.Theseestimates wererobusttotheHeckman-Siegelmancritique.

Inmale-dominatedoccupations,Neumark’smethodshowedusthat employers act as ifthere was no difference in the variance of un-observablesbetween cisgender menand transgenderapplicants (i.e.

𝜎T/𝜎MM≈1),wealsofindnosupportforstatisticalmotivations leav-ingonlytaste-basedones.Thisfindingisinlinewithsomesurvey evi-dencethathaveshownthatmenmaybemoreprejudicedthanwomen againsttransgenderpeople(Nagoshietal.,2008).Employersin female-dominatedoccupationsinsteadactedasiftherewasadifferenceinthe varianceof unobservables(i.e.𝜎T/𝜎FF ≈.49),butevidentlyour CVs wererepresentativeenoughthatthisdidnotaffectourestimatesof dis-criminationinthissubsample.Thismeansthatwefoundnoevidence thattransgenderapplicantswereseenasparticularlyrisky(ineither male-orfemale-dominatedoccupations),astheywouldhavethenbeen treatedasiftheyhadahighervarianceofunobservablesthan cisgen-derapplicants.Insteaditseemedasifemployersinfemale-dominated occupationsweremorecertainoftheunderlyingproductivityof trans-genderapplicantsthantheywereofthatoffemaleapplicants,butstill

preferredtohirefemaleapplicants.Thissuggeststhatemployersmay havehadstereotypedperceptionsabouttheunobservedproductivityof transgenderapplicants.

The heterogeneouseffectsfordifferent transgender(MF andFM) andcisgender(FFandMM)applicantsacrossoccupationswith differ-entgender ratiosprovided somesuggestiveevidencethat employers infemale-dominatedoccupationsseemedlessacceptingofmale appli-cants,whereasemployersinmale-dominatedoccupationsseemedless acceptingoftransgenderapplicants.Infemale-dominatedoccupations, employersweremorelikelytorespondpositivelytocisgenderand trans-genderfemaleapplicantsthantocisgenderandtransgendermale appli-cants.Inmale-dominatedoccupationsemployersseemedtohave posi-tivelyrespondedtoapplicantsinfollowingdescendingorder:cisgender maleapplicants,cisgenderfemaleapplicants,transgendermale appli-cants,andlastlytransgenderfemaleapplicants.

Wealsofoundthatdiscriminationdidnotdiminishwiththeamount ofexperiencereportedintheCVs,eitherinthefullsampleorinany sub-sample,whichsuggeststhatdiscriminationwaslesslikelytobedriven bystatisticalmotivations.Oneimportantnoteonstatistical discrimina-tionisthatstatisticalmotivescouldstillplayaroleinindividualCV evaluationseventhoughwedidnotdetectthemattheaggregatelevel, becausestatisticalmotivesarenotnecessarilyunidirectional.For exam-ple,VanBormandBaert(2018)andVanBormetal.,2020use hypothet-icalhiringdecisionsofhumanresourcesstudentstoexamine discrim-inatorymotivationsagainsttransgenderapplicants.Theyfoundmixed evidenceforstatisticaldiscrimination,withbothpositiveandnegative effectsbasedonthreestatisticalmotivations:(i)thelikelihoodofsick leave(negativeforbothtransgendermalesandfemales),(ii)the prob-abilityof maternityleave(heterogeneousbetweentransgendermales andfemales),and(iii)perceptionsaboutassertivenessandautonomy (positiveforbothtransgendermalesandfemales).Ourresultthat dis-paratetreatmentwaslikelynotduetostatisticaldiscriminationcould beinlinewiththeseresultsbecausethepositiveandnegativeeffects fromdifferentmotivationsmaycanceleachotheroutattheaggregate level.

Thecontextof ourstudymustbe discussedat thispoint,bothin termsoflocaleandtiming.Thereareseveralreasonswhyestimates re-portedininthispapermightreflectalowerboundofhiring discrimina-tionfacedbytransgenderpeople.First,weconductedthisexperimentin Sweden,aparticularlyprogressiveandtolerantcountrywhenitcomes togenderequality(WorldEconomicForum,2018)andLGBTacceptance (FloresandPark,2018).Second,theexperimentwasconductedwhen theSwedish economywasdoing well,which meansthatthemarket forlow-skilljobsshouldberelativelytight.Reportsofdiscrimination tendtobelowerundersuchconditions(BoulwareandKuttner,2019) whichmayhaveloweredourestimatesofdiscrimination.Third,even thoughwefocusexplicitlyonprivatesectorjobs,somesectorsclearly interactmorewiththepublicsector;thisismostlytrueforthe female-dominatedoccupationsin oursample(e.g.,enrollednurseand child-care)andmayhaveaffectedtheratesofdiscriminationseeninthose sectors(Ahmedetal.,2013b).Fourth,ourexperimentaldesignclearly signalstransgenderstatusandseparatesitfrom namechanges. How-ever,wemayhaveunderstateddiscriminationifthecisgender appli-cantshavereceivedfewerpositiveemployerresponsesbecauseoftheir namechangecomparedtoapplicantswhohavenotgonethroughaname change.Wefindthisunlikelybecausetheresponserateswerehigh(40 percent)andchangingone’sgivennameisfairlycommoninSweden.

Itisalsoimportanttonotethatourmethodcapturesdiscrimination onlyintheinitialstepofthehiringprocessandusesonlyofficialjob searchchannels.Obtainingajobthroughsocialcontactsandnonofficial channelsiscommoninSweden,andtheuseofsocialchannelsismore commonamongsmallfirms(Eliasonetal.,2017).Ourstudy, unfortu-nately,didnotcapturethisinformalsideofthemarket.Evenifthereare fewreasonstobelievethatthesechannelsshouldexhibitless discrimi-nationthantraditionalchannels,moreresearchisclearlyneededhere. Itisalsolikelythatotheravenuesfordiscriminationsuchasworkplace

References

Related documents

There are several possible explanations for the effect, but it suggests that researchers had limited information about these potential collaborations – either about who else

This study investigates how a group of children of incarcerated parents in India make meaning in their lives and how India Vision Foundation affects their meaning-making process..

Therefore, as fertility decreased over time, female lifespan increased, while male lifespan remained largely stable, supporting the theory that differential costs of reproduction

Is mathematics considered to be a male, female, or gender neutral domain by Swedish pupils in compulsory and upper secondary school.. One main motive for the study is

In order to fulfill the purpose of the study, the case study object needed to be an organization within the ICT industry that today is experiencing lack of women on managerial

The aim of this study is twofold: firstly, to analyze students’ texts to see if the students, with the help of scaffolding, can develop their writing skill and get higher

Many young girls in developing countries experience early pregnancy and  lifelong dependence upon family and 

The purpose of this thesis is to analyse and compare different transgender characters in order to get a better understanding of how the transgender population has been represented