• No results found

Food preferences and nutrient composition in zoo-housed ring-tailed lemurs, Lemur catta

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "Food preferences and nutrient composition in zoo-housed ring-tailed lemurs, Lemur catta"

Copied!
6
0
0

Loading.... (view fulltext now)

Full text

(1)

Contents lists available atScienceDirect

Physiology & Behavior

journal homepage:www.elsevier.com/locate/physbeh

Food preferences and nutrient composition in zoo-housed ring-tailed lemurs,

Lemur catta

Madeleine Hansell

a,b

, Angelica Åsberg

c

, Matthias Laska

a,⁎

aIFM Biology, Linköping University, SE-581 83 Linköping, Sweden bFuruviksparken, SE-814 91 Furuvik, Sweden

cDjurparkszoologen, SE-474 93 Ellös, Sweden

A R T I C L E I N F O Keywords: Food preferences Nutrient composition Correlational analysis Ring-tailed lemur Lemur catta A B S T R A C T

The aim of the present study was to assess the occurrence of spontaneous food preferences in zoo-housed ring-tailed lemurs and to analyze whether these preferences correlate with nutrient composition. Using a two-al-ternative choice test three female and one male Lemur catta were repeatedly presented with all possible binary combinations of 12 types of food which are part of their diet in captivity and found to display the following rank order of preference: apple > sweet potato > melon > beetroot > carrot > egg > eggplant > pumpkin > cucumber > tomato > cabbage > mealworm. Correlational analyses revealed a highly significant positive correlation between this food preference ranking and the total carbohydrate and sucrose contents of the foods (p < 0.01, respectively). No other significant correlations with any other macro- or micronutrient were found. These results suggest that zoo-housed ring-tailed lemurs are not opportunistic, but selective feeders with regard to maximizing their net gain of energy as only the content of carbohydrates, but not the contents of total energy, proteins or lipids significantly correlated with the displayed food preferences. Further, we found that ring-tailed lemurs that were raised on a vegetable-based diet did not significantly differ in their food preferences, and in particular in their predilection for food items high in carbohydrates, from animals that had previously been fed a fruit-based diet. This suggests that the lemurs’ preference for carbohydrate-rich food items may be innate and not affected by experience with different diets.

1. Introduction

The diet of most primate species includes a wide variety of plant foods[1]. Nevertheless, most primates are highly selective feeders and display marked preferences for certain types of plants or plant parts [2, 3]. Field studies on both primates and other herbivores suggest that their food choices can be attributed to two main factors: the nutritional and/or toxic content of the plant part in question [4,5] and its relative spatial and temporal distribution[6]. Additional factors such as body size or gut morphology are thought to also affect food selection in primates but are commonly regarded as evolutionary adaptations to the two main factors mentioned above[7]. Numerous studies have found negative correlations between food selection of primates and the con-tent of plant secondary compounds such as alkaloids, phenolics or tannins that are toxic or inhibit the digestion of proteins and poly-saccharides [e.g.8–10]. In contrast, only few studies so far reported positive correlations between the food preferences displayed by pri-mates and the nutrient content of plants [11,12]. However, according

to optimal foraging theory, natural selection should favor individuals which succeed in maximizing their net intake of energy and other cri-tical nutrients and, accordingly, this should be reflected in their food selection patterns and food preferences [13, 14]. One possible ex-planation for the paucity of data on positive correlations between food preferences of primates and the content of certain nutrients is the highly complex chemical composition of plant foods. Herbivorous species have to permanently make trade-offs between the need to meet their nutrient and energy requirements and the need to avoid ingestion of too high amounts of toxic compounds that plants use to defend themselves against predation[15]. This, in turn, makes it difficult to identify those nutrients that herbivorous animals may actively seek.

One approach to overcome this difficulty and to obtain information on which nutrients may affect primate food selection in a positive manner is to present animals with food items that contain only small and thus presumably negligible amounts of plant secondary compounds and to assess if their choice behavior correlates positively with any nutrient. Cultivated fruits and vegetables conform with this idea as they

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2020.113125

Received 8 July 2020; Received in revised form 5 August 2020; Accepted 5 August 2020

Corresponding author.

E-mail address:malas@ifm.liu.se(M. Laska).

Available online 07 August 2020

0031-9384/ © 2020 The Author(s). Published by Elsevier Inc. This is an open access article under the CC BY license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/BY/4.0/).

(2)

are low in plant secondary compounds and are well-characterized with regard to their nutrient and energy content[16]. Using this approach, previous studies found that pig-tailed macaques (Macaca nemestrina) and white-handed gibbons (Hylobates lar) are selective feeders with regard to maximizing their net gain of energy by preferring foods that are high in carbohydrates [17,18]. Squirrel monkeys (Saimiri sciureus) and spider monkeys (Ateles geoffroyi), in contrast, were found to display food preferences which significantly correlated positively with total energy content, irrespective of the source of energy [17, 19]. Thus, there are different strategies with regard to food choices that catarrhine and platyrrhine primates can adopt to meet their energy requirements. So far, no strepsirrhine primate species has been assessed for possible positive correlations between preferences for certain cultivated fruits and vegetables and their nutrient composition. The present study therefore aimed tofill this gap.

Ring-tailed lemurs (Lemur catta) are known to consume a wide variety of food items of both plant and animal origin [20–22]. Never-theless, they are considered as opportunistic frugivores/folivores and are highly adaptive to seasonal changes in their diet[23]. Free-ranging Lemur catta have been reported to select foods containing high levels of carbohydrates when those are seasonally available[24]. They have also been observed to spend a considerable proportion of their feeding time foraging on arthropods such as cicadas and caterpillars, and thus on protein-rich foods of animal origin[25]. Additionally, Lemur catta in the wild have been reported to regularly drink water from open sources such as streams or springs[26]. Finally, ring-tailed lemurs have also been observed to consume soil, presumably to meet their sodium re-quirements[27].

It was therefore the aim of the present study to assess food pre-ferences in a group of zoo-housed ring-tailed lemurs for a variety of cultivated fruits and vegetables as well as foods of animal origin, and to analyse whether these preferences correlate with the abundance of macro- or micronutrients. We hypothesized that ring-tailed lemurs 1. should show marked preferences for specific foods and that these pre-ferences are based on their nutrient composition, 2. should prefer food items high in carbohydrate content, 3. should prefer food items high in protein content, 4. should be indifferent to differences in water content of food items, and 5. should prefer food items high in sodium content. The opportunity to include both animals that grew up on a vege-table-based diet and animals that switched from a fruit-based to a ve-getable-based diet two years prior to the present study allowed us to additionally assess possible effects of experience with a given diet (or lack thereof) on food preferences. Here, we hypothesized that ring-tailed lemurs 6. should differ in their food preferences based on ex-perience with their captive diet.

2. Materials and methods 2.1. Animals

Four ring-tailed lemurs (Lemur catta), maintained at Furuviksparken (Sweden), participated in the study. They comprised two adult females of 13 (Ester) and 17 (Bi) years of age and two infant siblings of one year (Lily) and three months (Vide) of age, respectively. All lemurs except the oldest female were born at Furuviksparken. The lemurs were housed in an indoor enclosure of 200 m3, with access to a 5,000 m2 outdoor enclosure. A small corridor connected the indoor and outdoor enclosure. The lemurs were fed a variety of vegetables and two kinds of commercial primate pellets (Zoo primate high fiber pellets and leaf-eating primate pellets, from Granovit, Kaiseraugust, Switzerland) twice per day. Fresh leaves, grass and other plant matter as well as arthropods were available from the natural vegetation outdoors. Water was always available ad libitum. Due to problems with obesity and reproduction the lemurs had been switched from a diet based on a high proportion of fruits to a diet based on vegetables which now includes only small amounts of fruit as rewards during behavioral training. The switch of

diet occurred 2 years prior to the start of the study. 2.2. Procedures

Food preferences were assessed using a two-alternative choice test. The animals were presented with pairs of equally-sized food items, and their choice behavior, i.e. which of the two food items was consumed first, was recorded. Animals were tested singly in order to avoid com-petition or distraction affecting a lemur's choice behavior. Testing took place in the morning, between 07:30 and 08:30, before the lemurs were fed theirfirst meal of the day.

During the sessions two of the lemurs approached a wooden shelf mounted on the outside of the mesh of a connecting tunnel, chose one of a pair of simultaneously presented food items and then retreated, so that the rejected food item could be removed. The other two lemurs approached a metal tray held by the experimenter instead of the wooden shelf. In order to prevent an animal from taking both food items at the same time, the food items were placed 20 cm apart. All foods were cut into cubes of approximately 1 cm3to avoid choice

be-havior being affected by size differences. (In the case of mealworms, an amount approximating a volume of 1 cm3was presented.) Each session consisted of eight pairwise presentations and the position of the food items (e.g. cucumber presented left and carrot presented right) was pseudorandomized in order to counterbalance possible side pre-ferences. Each pair of food items was presented to an animal for a total of 10 times and care was taken to never present a food item that had been part of the previous pair.

The following 12 types of food were employed: apple (Malus pu-mila), beetroot (Beta vulgaris), carrot (Daucus carota, subsp. sativus), cucumber (Cucumis sativus), eggplant (Solanum melongea), sweet potato (Ipomoea batatas), butternut pumpkin (Cucurbita moschata), napa cab-bage (Brassica rapa subsp. pekinesis), honey melon (Cucumis melo), to-mato (Solanum lycopersicum), mealworm (larva of Tenebrio molitor) and hardboiled egg (egg from Gallus gallus domesticus). The rationale for choosing these types of food was a) that all of them were part of the animals’ diet in the zoo and thus familiar to the lemurs, b) that data for the contents of macro- and micronutrients in these types of food are available, allowing us to assess possible correlations between food preferences and nutrient contents [16, 28], and c) that they differ markedly in their contents of macronutrients. With regard to the latter point it should be mentioned that the contents of total energy, carbo-hydrates, lipids, protein, dietaryfiber, and water differed by up to a factor of 22, 21, 219, 45, 24, and 1.6, respectively, between the types of food used here. The exact values of the contents of the macro- und micronutrients of all twelve food items are summarized in Supple-mentary Table 1.

In an attempt to minimize the inevitable intraspecific variation in nutrient composition, care was taken to always present food items of a given type with the same degree of ripeness.

2.3. Data analysis

A total of 2640 choices (66 binary combinations x 10 presentations per animal x 4 animals) were recorded, and food preference rankings were established using the following criteria:

Criterion 1 (individual level): The sum total of choices for each of the 12 types of food across all binary combinations was built for each individual animal. The theoretical maximum score for any type of food with this criterion is 110 (11 combinations x 10 presenta-tions per animal x 1 animal). In cases when a lemur failed to make a choice between two food items within 10 seconds, each of the two items were assigned 0.5 points.

Criterion 2 (group level): This criterion adopts the same procedure of building the sum total of choices as for criterion 1, although, here, the data for all four animals were collapsed. Thus, the theoretical

(3)

maximum score for any type of food with this criterion is 440 (11 combinations x 10 presentations per animal x 4 animals).

Additionally, two-tailed binomial tests using the sum total of choices for each member of a given binary combination were performed to assess significant preferences both at the individual level and at the group level (p < 0.05). Correlations between the food preference rankings and the contents of nutrients were evaluated by calculating Spearman rank-order correlation coefficients rswhich were tested for

significance by computing z-scores. The same test was also used to as-sess whether the food preference rankings of the four lemurs correlated with each other.

3. Results

3.1. Food preferences

Table 1summarizes the choice behavior of the lemurs in the food preference tests, i.e. the number of choices made by the lemurs in fa-vour of each member of a given pair of food items. With 46 of the 66 binary combinations, the lemurs displayed a statistically significant preference for one of the options (two-tailed binomial test, p < 0.05). Apple was clearly the most attractive food and, accordingly, was sig-nificantly preferred over all 11 other food items (p < 0.05). The high attractiveness of apple is further illustrated by the fact that 93.6% of all possible choices were in favour of this food item (Table 2). Sweet po-tato, melon, beetroot, carrot, and egg were significantly preferred over 9, 8, 6, 6, and 6 other food items, respectively (p < 0.05). Eggplant, pumpkin, cabbage, cucumber, tomato and mealworm were never

significantly preferred over any other food item. Mealworm was the least preferred food and, accordingly, only 23.1% of all choices, and thus the lowest percentage of all options, were in favour of mealworm. 3.2. Rankings derived from the food preferences

The food preference rankings derived from the lemurs’ choice be-havior are summarized inTable 2. All four lemurs displayed similar rankings of preference for the 12 food items. Accordingly, their rank-ings all significantly correlated with each other (Spearman rs≥ 0.62, p

< 0.05, with all six comparisons). Similarly, the food preference rankings of the two animals that had previously been fed a fruit-based diet and of the two animals that had been raised on a vegetable-based diet, respectively, significantly correlated with each other (Spearman rs =0.84, p < 0.01), suggesting that experience with a different diet (or a

lack thereof) did not have a significant effect on the lemurs’ food choices.

3.3. Food preference rankings and nutritional content

The Spearman rank-order correlation statistics between the food preference ranking and the nutritional content of the food items are summarized inTable 3. The food preference rankings displayed by the lemurs correlated highly significantly with total carbohydrate content and with the content of sucrose (Spearman, p < 0.01). This was true both when the four lemurs were considered separately and when they were considered as a group. However, the lemurs’ food preference rankings did not significantly correlate with total energy content (Spearman, p > 0.05). Similarly, the food preference rankings did not correlate significantly with any other macro- or micronutrient (Spearman, p > 0.05). Here, too, this was true both for the individual lemurs and for the group.

4. Discussion

The results of the present study demonstrate that zoo-housed ring-tailed lemurs display marked food preferences in a two-alternative choice test using cultivated fruits and vegetables as well as foods of animal origin. Further, the results show that these preferences sig-nificantly correlated positively with the contents of total carbohydrates and sucrose of the food items used, but not with total energy content. These findings are consistent with our first two hypotheses. Interestingly, the lemurs did not prefer food items that are high in protein content, which is in contrast with our third hypothesis. Taken together, thesefindings suggest that ring-tailed lemurs are not oppor-tunistic feeders with regard to energy gain but rather seek to meet their Table 1

Choice behavior of the ring-tailed lemurs in the food preference tests

Apple S.potato Melon Beetroot Carrot Egg Eggplant Pumpkin Cucumber Tomato Cabbage Mealworm

Apple X ← ← ← ← ← ← ← ← ← ← ← Sweet potato 11:29 X ↖ ↖ ← ← ← ← ← ← ← ← Melon 3:37 14:26 X ↖ ← ← ← ← ← ← ← ← Beetroot 9:31 15:25 15:25 X ↖ ← ← ← ← ← ← ← Carrot 2:38 6:34 6:34 12.5:27.5 X ↖ ← ← ← ← ← ← Egg 2:38 6:34 7:33 11:29 16:24 X ← ← ← ← ← ← Eggplant 1:39 0:40 0.5:39.5 2.5:37.5 2.5:37.5 7.5:32.5 X ↖ ↖ ↖ ↖ ↖ Pumpkin 0:40 0:40 0.5:39.5 3.5:36.5 2.5:37.5 9:31 19:21 X ↖ ↖ ↖ ↖ Cucumber 0:40 0:40 1:39 4:36 2:38 6:34 17:23 20:20 X ↖ ↖ ↖ Tomato 0:40 0:40 0:40 5:35 1:39 8.5:31.5 16:24 18:22 19.5:20.5 X ↖ ↖ Cabbage 0:40 1:39 0:40 4:36 3:37 8.5:31.5 17:23 19:21 18.5:21.5 18.5:21.5 X ↖ Mealworm 0:40 0:40 0:40 3:37 0:40 6:34 16.5:23.5 17:23 20.5:19.5 18.5:21.5 20:20 X

The table indicates the number of choices (from n = 4 animals) for each member of a given pair of food items. Thefirst value applies to the food item to the left and the second to the food item on the top.

← indicates a significant preference for the food item to the left (p < 0.01). ↖ indicates a lack of significant preference for either member of a pair (p > 0.05).

Table 2

Food items and their corresponding rank order of preference

Criterion 1 Vide Lily Bi Ester Criterion 2 ∑V+L+B+E %

Apple 103 102 101 106 Apple 412 (93.6)

Sweet potato 88 85 94 102 Sweet potato 369 (83.9)

Melon 88.5 96 77 85.5 Melon 347 (78.9) Beetroot 89.5 85 89.5 49.5 Beetroot 313.5 (71.3) Carrot 75 61 67 76.5 Carrot 279.5 (63.5) Egg 25 69 78.5 64 Egg 236.5 (53.8) Eggplant 36.5 37 24.5 28.5 Eggplant 126.5 (28.8) Pumpkin 39.5 25.5 26.5 31 Pumpkin 122.5 (27.8) Cucumber 30.5 27.5 25.5 28 Cucumber 111.5 (25.3) Tomato 34 21.5 25.5 30 Tomato 111 (25.2) Cabbage 25 27 26 31.5 Cabbage 109.5 (24.9) Mealworm 25.5 23.5 25 27.5 Mealworm 101.5 (23.1) Numbers in parentheses indicate the percentage of choices in favour of a given type of food (relative to the theoretical maximum of 440 choices / type of food).

(4)

requirements of metabolic energy by preferring foods that are high in soluble carbohydrates. This is remarkable considering that in the nat-ural habitat of primates the availability of fruits that are high in car-bohydrates is usually more variable and dependent on the season of the year than the availability of arthropods that are high in protein[29]. However, ring-tailed lemurs may be an exception from this rule as their native habitat of Madagascar provides at least two plant species (Melia azedarach and Ficus spp.) which produce carbohydrate-rich fruits year-round and are heavily exploited by Lemur catta [21]. Further, field studies suggest that, although ring-tailed lemurs are considered as op-portunistic frugivores/folivores that are highly flexible in their diet [23], they spend a markedly higher proportion of time feeding on fruit (31%,[24]) compared to feeding on arthropods (15.7%,[25]). As so-luble carbohydrates and proteins provide the same amount of energy (4 calories) per gram, it seems plausible that the expenditure of time and energy that ring-tailed lemurs have to invest to procure equal amounts of calories from these two macronutrients favors foraging for fruits over foraging for arthropods.

In order to assess whether the marked preferences of the ring-tailed

lemurs for food items high in solube carbohydrates are based on the carbohydrates’ taste property of sweetness or on their physiological property of being an easily metabolizeable source of energy, studies determining taste preference thresholds as well as on relative taste preferences would be needed. Whereas a variety of nonhuman primate species have been studied in this respect [30,31], corresponding data for Lemur catta are so far largely lacking.

Ourfinding that ring-tailed lemurs display a marked preference for foods that are high in soluble carbohydrates is in line with studies in pig-tailed macaques (Macaca nemestrina) and white-handed gibbons (Hylobates lar) which employed the same method as the present study [17, 18]. However, the food preferences of squirrel monkeys (Saimiri sciureus) and spider monkeys (Ateles geoffroyi), which were also tested using the same method as in the present study, significantly correlated positively with total energy content, irrespective of the source of energy [17,19]. Thus, there are clearly different strategies with regard to food choices that primates can adopt to meet their energy requirements. Several possible explanations for these between-species differences in food preferences with regard to macronutrients have been put forward and include differences in the degree of frugivory[17], the degree of food competition with sympatric species[32], and the abundance of certain plant species that serve as staple foods[33]. As these possible explanations are not mutually exclusive, further studies including ad-ditional primate species are needed to draw reliable conclusions re-garding the causes underlying the food selection strategies of non-human primates to maximize the net gain of metabolic energy.

In contrast to our third hypothesis, the lemurs of the present study did not prefer food items high in protein content. Thisfinding deserves some discussion as the protein leverage hypothesis states that human subjects will prioritize the consumption of protein in food over other macronutrients and will eat until their protein needs have been met, regardless of total energy content [e.g.34–36]. Studies in free-ranging nonhuman primates lend some support to the notion of a pivotal role of protein in food selection [37,38]. However, it should be mentioned that no study on food preferences in captive nonhuman primates ever reported a positive correlation with protein content [e.g.17–19]. This might be explained by the fact that the diets fed to primates in human care provide a sufficient amount of protein, possibly due to the pellets that often form part of their daily provisioning, so that the animals can meet their protein requirements without the need to display significant preferences for food items containing high amounts of this macro-nutrient. Studies on captive primates kept on a protein-deficient, but otherwise nutritionally complete diet would be needed to test whether the protein leverage hypothesis may contribute to the food preferences observed in e.g. zoo settings.

Our finding that the ring-tailed lemurs did not base their food preferences on water content of the food items is consistent with our fourth hypothesis. Thisfinding is not trivial considering that squirrel monkeys[17], spider monkeys [19], and also pacas (Agouti paca), a highly frugivorous rodent[39], have been found to display significant correlations between their food preferences and the water content of the tested food items. A plausible explanation for our result is that ring-tailed lemurs, unlike the aforementioned species, are known to reg-ularly drink water from open sources such as streams or springs[26] and thus do not depend on juicy fruits to meet their water requirements. In contrast to ourfifth hypothesis, the ring-tailed lemurs did not prefer food items high in sodium content. Certain minerals such as sodium have frequently been reported to be in short supply in the natural diet of primates. Accordingly, several studies reported that e.g. ring-tailed and brown lemurs (Lemur catta and Lemur fulvus; [27]), black-and-white colobus monkeys (Colobus guereza;[40]), cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus;[41]), red howler monkeys and white-bel-lied spider monkeys (Alouatta seniculus and Ateles belzebuth;[42]), and chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes;[43]) display marked predilections for edible material such as soil or wood which are high in those minerals that are otherwise lacking in their diet.

Table 3

Spearman rank-order correlation statistics (food preference ranking x nutrient content) rs p Macronutrients Energy 0.29 0.35 Water -0.29 0.37 Protein -0.34 0.29 Lipids -0.43 0.17 Carbohydrates 0.73 0.00763 Dietaryfiber 0.28 0.38 Total nitrogen -0.33 0.29 Carbohydrates Sucrose 0.69 0.01257 Fructose 0.26 0.41 Glucose 0.23 0.47 Starch 0.20 0.51 Fatty acids saturated -0.32 0.30 mono-unsaturated -0.49 0.11 poly-unsaturated -0.37 0.23 cholesterol -0.29 0.36 Vitamins Retinol (Vitamin A) -0.29 0.36 Carotene (Vitamin A) 0.11 0.74 Thiamine (Vitamin B1) -0.38 0.22 Riboflavin (Vitamin B2) -0.12 0.69 Vitamin B6 -0.36 0.24 Vitamin B12 -0.29 0.35

Ascorbic acid (Vitamin C) 0.19 0.59

Vitamin D -0.29 0.36 Vitamin E -0.20 0.53 Folate (Vitamin B9) 0.15 0.65 Pantothenate (Vitamin B5) -0.35 0.26 Biotin (Vitamin H) -0.21 0.50 Niacin (Vitamin B3) -0.43 0.16 Minerals Sodium (Na) 0.12 0.70 Potassium (K) -0.17 0.60 Calcium (Ca) -0.17 0.60 Magnesium (Mg) 0.00 1.00 Phosphorous (P) -0.34 0.29 Iron (Fe) -0.26 0.42 Copper (Cu) 0.10 0.76 Zinc (Zn) -0.09 0.78 Chlorine (Cl) -0.15 0.63 Manganese (Mn) -0.10 0.76 Selenium (Se) -0.33 0.30 Iodine (I) -0.41 0.19

Values for the statistical measure rsmay range from +1 (perfect positive

cor-relation) to -1 (perfect negative corcor-relation). Statistically significant correla-tions are shown in bold typeface.

(5)

Ourfinding that the ring-tailed lemurs showed neither significantly positive nor significantly negative correlations between their food preferences and any of the micronutrients (i.e. minerals and vitamins) tested is not self-evident as studies in captive squirrel monkeys[17], spider monkeys[19], and white-handed gibbons[18]– all tested using the same method as in the present study– showed significant positive correlations between food preferences and the contents of copper (in Saimiri sciureus), copper, magnesium and manganese (in Ateles geof-froyi), and selenium (in Hylobates lar), respectively. This suggests that not only diets of primates in the wild, but also diets of zoo-housed primates may lack important micronutrients such as certain minerals that the animals in question may therefore actively seek to consume. This notion is consistent with the well-established phenomenon of an-imals displaying a craving for specific types of food as a result of pro-longed specific nutrient deficiencies[44]. Ourfindings can therefore be interpreted as indicating that the diet fed to the zoo-housed ring-tailed lemurs of the present study does not lack any micronutrients, at least not to a degree so as to induce corresponding food preferences.

In this context, it is also important to note that the diet of zoo-housed primates usually differs markedly in its nutritional composition from the diet of their conspecifics in the wild. Cultivated fruits, for example, which often form the basis of the diet of primates in human care, have been selectively bred to appeal to human tastes [45]. Ac-cordingly, cultivated fruits are generally higher in soluble carbohy-drates, and lower infiber and plant secondary compounds compared to the non-cultivated fruits that primates co-evolved with and feed on in the wild[46]. Since primates in human care are often kept on a diet based on cultivated fruits, health issues such as obesity, diabetes, and problems with reproduction are a frequent consequence [47, 48]. Therefore, a balanced diet for zoo-housed primates needs to avoid presenting the animals with a too high abundance of certain macro- or micronutrients. This is particularly true for captive lemurs which have repeatedly been reported to be vulnerable to hepatic iron storage dis-ease [49,50]. The development of this disease which is characterized by accumulation of iron in the liver and which can be fatal in lemurs is fostered by a fruit-based diet as cultivated fruits are low in iron-binding polyphenols[51].

As the commercial varieties of vegetables have been found to be more similar in their nutrient composition to the wild fruits consumed by free-ranging primates than cultivated fruits are, zoos increasingly replace the fruit-based diets fed to their primates with vegetable-based diets [52,53]. This has also been the case with the lemurs of the present study two years prior to the start of our food preference tests. In con-trast to our sixth hypothesis, we found that ring-tailed lemurs that grew up on a vegetable-based diet did not significantly differ in their food preferences, and in particular in their predilection for food items high in carbohydrates, from animals that had previously been fed on a fruit-based diet. Thisfinding suggests that the switch of diet two years prior to the present study had little, if any effect on the animals’ food pre-ferences, and that the preference for foods high in carbohydrates may be innate and is not affected by a diet aiming at a limited access to sweet-tasting carbohydrates. The latter notion is in line with the well-established “sweet tooth” that primates display [54]as all primate species tested so far vividly prefer e.g. sweet-tasting solutions in two-alternative choice tests [e.g.30,31]. Thus, the switch from a fruit-based to a vegetable-based diet, although in most cases clearly beneficial for zoo-housed primates [52, 53], needs to take into consideration that nonhuman primates are likely to keep their predilection for sweet-tasting food items. This, in turn, may cause problems with the process of familiarization with and acceptance of novel and more healthy food items.

Ourfinding that the two infant lemurs included in the present study did not significantly differ in their food preferences from the adults may seem counter-intuitive considering that infant mammals usually differ in their nutritional needs from those of adult conspecifics[51]. These differences in the nutritional needs of infant and adult animals may,

although not necessarily, be accompanied by differences in the food preferences they display. However, it is also well-established that food preferences observed in infant mammals are strongly affected by their mothers’ diet[55]. As the mother of both infant lemurs of the present study has been fed the same vegetable-based diet during pregnancy that the infants were provided with after weaning, this might at least par-tially explain ourfinding of similar food preferences in both infant and adult ring-tailed lemurs.

Taken together, the results of the present study suggest that zoo-housed ring-tailed lemurs are not opportunistic, but selective feeders with regard to maximizing their net gain of energy as only the content of carbohydrates, but not the contents of total energy, proteins or lipids significantly correlated with the displayed food preferences. The lack of significant correlations between food preferences and the content of any of the micronutrients suggests that the diet fed to our lemurs was balanced. Further, we found that ring-tailed lemurs that grew up on a vegetable-based diet did not significantly differ in their food pre-ferences, and in particular in their predilection for food items high in carbohydrates, from animals that had previously been fed a fruit-based diet. This suggests that the lemurs’ preference for carbohydrate-rich food items may be innate and not affected by experience with different diets.

Ethical note

The experiments reported here comply with the American Society of Primatologists’ Principles for the Ethical Treatment of Primates, with the European Union Directive on the Protection of Animals Used for Scientific Purposes (EU Directive 2010/63/EU), and also with current Swedish animal welfare laws.

Funding

This research did not receive any specific grant from funding agencies in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.

Declarations of Competing Interest None.

Supplementary materials

Supplementary material associated with this article can be found, in the online version, atdoi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2020.113125.

References

[1] A.F. Richard, Primate diets: patterns and principles, in: A.F. Richard (Ed.), Primates in nature, W. H. Freeman, New York, 1985, pp. 163–205.

[2] R.N. Hughes, Diet Selection. An Inter-disciplinary Approach To Foraging Behaviour, Blackwell, London, 1993.

[3] D.J. Chivers, Measuring food intake in wild animals: primates, Proc. Nutr. Soc. 57 (1998) 321–332.

[4] R.A. Barton, A. Whiten, Reducing complex diets to simple rules: food selection by olive baboons, Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 35 (1994) 283–293.

[5] A.M. Felton, A. Felton, D. Raubenheimer, S.J. Simpson, W.J. Foley, J.T. Wood, I.R. Wallis, D.B. Lindenmayer, Protein content of diets dictates the daily energy intake of a free-ranging primate, Behav. Ecol. 20 (2009) 685–690.

[6] M. Leighton, Modeling dietary selectivity by Bornean orangutans: evidence for in-tegration of multiple criteria in fruit selection, Int. J. Primatol. 14 (1993) 257–311. [7] A.W. Illius, I.J. Gordon, Constraints on diet selection and foraging behaviour in

mammalian herbivores, in: R.N. Hughes (Ed.), Behavioural Mechanisms of Food Selection, Springer, New York, 1990, pp. 369–392.

[8] K.E. Glander, The impact of plant secondary compounds on primate feeding be-havior, Yearbk. Phys. Anthropol. 25 (1982) 1–18.

[9] P.G. Waterman, Food acquisition and processing as a function of plant leaf chem-istry, in: D.J. Chivers, B.A. Wood, A. Bilsborough (Eds.), Food Acquisition and Processing in Primates, Plenum Press, New York, 1984, pp. 177–211. [10] R. Wrangham, N.L. Conklin-Brittain, K.D. Hunt, Dietary response of chimpanzees

and cercopithecines to seasonal variation in fruit abundance: I. Antifeedants, Int. J. Primatol. 19 (1998) 949–970.

(6)

[11] M.L. Conklin-Brittain, R. Wrangham, K.D. Hunt, Dietary response of chimpanzees and cercopithecines to seasonal variation in fruit abundance: II. Macronutrients, Int. J. Primatol. 19 (1998) 971–998.

[12] K. Milton, Physiological ecology of howlers (Alouatta): energetic and digestive considerations and comparison with the Colobinae, Int. J. Primatol. 19 (1998) 513–548.

[13] D.W. Stephens, J.R. Krebs, Foraging Theory, Princeton University Press, Princeton, 1986.

[14] D.W. Stephens, J.S. Brown, R.C. Ydenberg, Foraging Behavior and Ecology, University of Chicago Press, Chicago, 2008.

[15] J.E. Lambert, J.M. Rothman, Fallback foods, optimal diets, and nutritional targets: primate responses to varying food availability and quality, Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 44 (2015) 493–512.

[16] Food Standards Agency, McCance and Widdowson's The composition of Foods, 6th ed., Royal Society of Chemistry, Cambridge, 2002.

[17] M. Laska, A comparison of food preferences and nutrient composition in captive squirrel monkeys, Saimiri sciureus, and pigtail macaques, Macaca nemestrina, Physiol. Behav. 73 (2001) 111–120.

[18] R. Jildmalm, M. Amundin, M. Laska, Food preferences and nutrient composition in captive white-handed gibbons, Hylobates lar, Int. J. Primatol. 29 (2008) 1535–1547. [19] M. Laska, L.T. Hernandez Salazar, E. Rodriguez Luna, Food preferences and nutrient composition in captive spider monkeys, Ateles geoffroyi, Int. J. Primatol. 21 (2000) 671–683.

[20] A. Gemmell, L. Gould, Microhabitat variation and its effects on dietary composition and intragroup feeding interactions between adult female Lemur catta during the dry season at Beza Mahafaly special reserve, Southwestern Madagascar, Int. J. Primatol. 29 (2008) 1511–1533.

[21] L. Gould, D.N. Gabriel, Wet and dry season diets of the endangered Lemur catta (ring-tailed lemur) in two mountainous rocky outcrop forest fragments in south-central Madagascar, Afr. J. Ecol. 53 (2014) 320–330.

[22] M. LaFleur, M.L. Sauther, Seasonal feeding ecology of ring-tailed temurs: A com-parison of spiny and gallery forest habitats, Folia Primatol 86 (2015) 25–34. [23] B. Simmen, S. Peronny, M. Jeanson, A. Hladik, A. Marez, Diet quality and taste

perception of plant secondary metabolites by Lemur catta, in: A. Jolly, N. Koyama, H. Rasamimanana, R.W. Sussman (Eds.), Ring-tailed lemur biology, Springer, New York, 2006, pp. 197–207.

[24] N. Yamashita, Chemical properties of the diets of two lemur species in shouthwestern Madagascar, Int. J. Primatol. 29 (2008) 339–364.

[25] T. Soma, Tradition and novelty: Lemur catta feeding strategy on introduced tree species at Berenty Reserve, in: A. Jolly, R.W. Sussman, N. Koyama,

H. Rasamimanana (Eds.), Ringtailed Lemur Biology, Springer, New York, 2006, pp. 141–159.

[26] D.E. Wilson, E. Hanlon, Lemur catta (Primates: Lemuridae), Mamm. Species 42 (2010) 58–74.

[27] J.U. Ganzhorn, Soil consumption of two groups of semi-free ranging lemurs (Lemur catta and Lemur fulvus), Ethology 74 (1987) 146–154.

[28] J.P. Williams, J.R. Williams, A. Kirabo, D. Chester, M. Peterson, Nutrient content and health benefits of insects, in: A.T. Dossey, J.A. Morales-Ramos, M.G. Rojas (Eds.), Insects as Sustainable Food Ingredients, Elsevier, Amsterdam, 2016, pp. 61–84.

[29] G. Hanya, C.A. Chapman, Linking feeding ecology and population abundance: a review of food resource limitation on primates, Ecol. Res. 28 (2013) 183–190. [30] A. Wielbass, M. Amundin, M. Laska, Gustatory responsiveness of black-and-white

ruffed lemurs (Varecia variegata variegata) to food-associated sugars, Int. J. Primatol. 36 (2015) 460–472.

[31] S. Nicklasson, D. Sjöström, M. Amundin, D. Roth, L.T. Hernandez Salazar, M. Laska, Taste responsiveness to two steviol glycosides in three species of nonhuman pri-mates, Curr. Zool. 64 (2018) 63–68.

[32] J.R. Mackinnon, K.S. Mackinnon, Niche differentiation in a primate community, in:

D.J. Chivers (Ed.), Malayan forest primates, Plenum Press, New York, 1980, pp. 167–190.

[33] R.A. Palombit, Inter- and intraspecific variation in the diets of sympatric siamang (Hylobates syndactylus) and lar gibbons (Hylobates lar), Folia Primatol. 68 (1997) 321–337.

[34] S.J. Simpson, D. Raubenheimer, Obesity: the protein leverage hypothesis, Obesity Rev. 6 (2005) 133–142.

[35] S. Griffioen-Roose, M. Mars, E. Siebelink, G. Finlayson, D. Tomé, C. de Graaf, Protein status elicits compensatory changes in food intake and food preferences, Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 95 (2012) 32–38.

[36] A.K. Gosby, A.D. Conigrave, D. Raubenheimer, S.J. Simpson, Protein leverage and energy intake, Obesity Rev. 15 (2014) 183–191.

[37] A.M. Felton, A. Felton, D.B. Lindenmayer, W.J. Foley, Nutritional goals of wild primates, Funct. Ecol. 23 (2009) 70–78.

[38] J.U. Ganzhorn, S.J. Arrigo-Nelson, V. Carrai, M.K. Chalise, G. Donati, I. Droescher, T.M. Eppley, M.T. Irwin, F. Koch, A. Koenig, M.M. Kowalewski, C.B. Mowry, E.R. Patel, C. Pichon, J. Ralison, C. reisdorff, B. Simmen, E. Stalenberg, D. Starrs, J. Terboven, P.C. Wright, W.J. Foley, Importance of protein in leaf selection of folivorous primates, Am. J. Primatol. 79 (2017) e22550.

[39] M. Laska, J.M. Luna Baltazar, E. Rodriguez Luna, Food preferences and nutrient composition in captive pacas, Agouti paca (Rodentia, Dasyproctidae), Mamm. Biol. 68 (2003) 31–41.

[40] J.F. Oates, Water-plant and soil consumption by guereza monekys (Colobus guereza): a relationship with minerals and toxins in the diet? Biotropica 10 (1978) 241–253. [41] P.A. Garber, Proposed nutritional importance of plant exudates in the diet of the

Panamanian tamarin, Saguinus oedipus geoffroyi, Int. J. Primatol. 5 (1984) 1–15. [42] K. Izawa, Soil-eating by Alouatta and Ateles, Int. J. Primatol. 14 (1993) 229–242. [43] E.M. Venable, Z. Machanda, L. Hagberg, J. Lucore, E. Otali, J.M. Rothman,

M. Uwimbabazi, R. Wrangham, Wood and meat as complementary sources of so-dium for Kanyawara chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 172 (2020) 41–47.

[44] S.J. Simpson, R.M. Sibly, K.P. Lee, S.T. Behmer, D. Raubenheimer, Optimal foraging when regulating intake of multiple nutrients, Anim. Behav. 68 (2004) 1299–1311. [45] C. Schwitzer, S.Y. Polowinsky, C. Soman, Fruits as foods– Common misconceptions

about frugivory, in: M. Clauss, J. Hummel, J. Nijboer, A. Plowman (Eds.), Zoo Animal Nutrition IV. Filander, Fürth, Germany, 2009, pp. 131–168.

[46] K. Milton, Back to basics: why foods of wild primates have relevance for modern human health, Nutrition 16 (2000) 481–483.

[47] S. Goodchild, C. Schwitzer, The problem of obesity in captive lemurs, Int. Zoo News 55 (2008) 353–357.

[48] E.N. Videan, J. Fritz, J. Murphy, Development of guidelines for assessing obesity in captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), Zoo Biol. 26 (2007) 93–104.

[49] C. Wood, S.G. Fang, A. Hunt, W.J. Streich, M. Clauss, Increased iron absorption in lemurs: quantitative screening and assessment of dietary prevention, Am. J. Primatol. 61 (2003) 101–110.

[50] A. Caravaggi, A. Plowman, D.J. Wright, C. Bishop, The composition of ruffed lemur (Varecia spp.) diets in six UK zoological collections, with reference to the problems of obesity and iron storage disease, J. Zoo Aquar. Res. 6 (2018) 41–49. [51] Committee on Animal Nutrition, Nutrient Requirements of Nonhuman Primates,

2nd revised ed., National Academies Press, Washington DC, 2000.

[52] A. Plowman, Diet review and change for monkeys at Paignton zoo environmental park, J. Zoo Aquar. Res. 1 (2013) 73–77.

[53] S. Britt, K. Cowlard, K. Baker, A. Plowman, Aggression and self-directed behaviour of captive lemurs is reduced by feeding fruit-free diets, J. Zoo Aquar. Res. 3 (2015) 52–58.

[54] I. Ramirez, Why do sugars taste good? Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 14 (1990) 125–134.

[55] A.K. Ventura, J. Worobey, Early influences on the development of food preferences, Curr. Biol. 23 (2013) R401–R408.

References

Related documents

spårbarhet av resurser i leverantörskedjan, ekonomiskt stöd för att minska miljörelaterade risker, riktlinjer för hur företag kan agera för att minska miljöriskerna,

This project focuses on the possible impact of (collaborative and non-collaborative) R&amp;D grants on technological and industrial diversification in regions, while controlling

Analysen visar också att FoU-bidrag med krav på samverkan i högre grad än när det inte är ett krav, ökar regioners benägenhet att diversifiera till nya branscher och

I många andra länder finns relativt stora skillnader mellan män och kvinnor och det är inte minst därför en ökad förvärvsintensitet för kvinnor förs fram

Parallellmarknader innebär dock inte en drivkraft för en grön omställning Ökad andel direktförsäljning räddar många lokala producenter och kan tyckas utgöra en drivkraft

Accordingly, this paper aims to investigate how three companies operating in the food industry; Max Hamburgare, Innocent and Saltå Kvarn, work with CSR and how this work has

In the end of the novel we see Marian’s eating habits return to ‘normal’ after she consumes the edible woman cake; yet the main part of the novel is taken up by her rejection of

The case of the Porto Metropolitan Area’ (Malcata, 2010), is relevant to my paper because it studies and compares the settlement patterns of immigrants