• No results found

Turn-taking in interspecies vocal communication : A study of dog-human interactions

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "Turn-taking in interspecies vocal communication : A study of dog-human interactions"

Copied!
24
0
0

Loading.... (view fulltext now)

Full text

(1)

Linköping University | Department of Physics, Chemistry and Biology Bachelor thesis, 16 hp | Biology programme: Physics, Chemistry and Biology Spring term 2018 | LITH-IFM-G-EX--18/3516--SE

Turn-taking in interspecies

vocal communication

A study of dog-human interactions

Johanna Axelsson

Examinator, Per Jensen, IFM Biologi, Linköpings universitet Supervisor, Lina Roth, IFM Biologi, Linköpings universitet

(2)
(3)

Date 2018-05-28

Division, Department

Department of Physics, Chemistry and Biology Linköping University

URL för elektronisk version

ISBN

ISRN: LITH-IFM-G-EX--18/3516--SE

_________________________________________________________________

Serietitel och serienummer ISSN

Title of series, numbering ______________________________ Språk Language Svenska/Swedish Engelska/English ________________ Rapporttyp1 Report category Licentiatavhandling Examensarbete C-uppsats D-uppsats Övrig rapport _____________ Title

Turn-taking in interspecies vocal communication: a study of dog-human interactions

Author Johanna Axelsson

Keywords

Interspecies communication; dog-human communication; vocalization; turn-taking. Abstract

Dogs are well adapted to a life close to humans and are capable of understanding several human social cues. Turn-taking is a fundamental phenomenon in human conversations, yet no research of turn-taking in interspecies vocal communication has been done. This pilot study therefore aimed to develop a test method in order to analyze if turn-taking occurs in interspecies vocal

communication between dogs and humans and if turn-taking can be correlated with specific behaviors in dogs. By analyzing a vocal conversation between seven dogs and their owners during a request situation in their home environment, this study found support for turn-taking existence in dog-human vocal communications. In general, owner-dog vocal communication could be linked to positive events, such as tail wagging and eye contact. Meanwhile, a lower frequency of vocal turn-taking could be linked to stress related behaviors, such as lip licking. Owner in dyads where more vocal overlap occurred tended to score the owner-dog relationship and communication abilities with lower grades, suggesting the absence of turn-taking was experienced as unpleasant for the owner as well. In conclusion, findings from current study highlight the unique coevolution of dogs and humans and opens up for future research of turn-taking in interspecies vocal communication.

(4)

Contents

1 Abstract ... 5

2 Introduction ... 6

3 Material & method ... 7

3.1 Participants ... 7

3.2 Testing procedure ... 8

3.2.1 Trials ... 8

3.2.2 Audio and video recordings ... 8

3.3 Analysis ... 10

4 Result ... 11

4.1 Testing procedure ... 11

4.2 Dog vocalization and vocal overlap ... 11

4.3 Behaviors correlated with vocalization and overlap ... 13

5 Discussion ... 14

5.1 Societal and ethical implications ... 17

5.2 Conclusion... 17

6 Acknowledgement... 18

7 References ... 18

(5)

1 Abstract

Dogs are well adapted to a life close to humans and are capable of understanding several human social cues. Turn-taking is a fundamental phenomenon in human conversations, yet no research of turn-taking in interspecies vocal communication has been done. This pilot study

therefore aimed to develop a test method in order to analyze if turn-taking occurs in interspecies vocal communication between dogs and humans and if turn-taking can be correlated with specific behaviors in dogs. By analyzing a vocal conversation between seven dogs and their owners during a request situation in their home environment, this study found support for turn-taking existence in dog-human vocal communications. In general, owner-dog vocal communication could be linked to positive events, such as tail wagging and eye contact. Meanwhile, a lower

frequency of vocal turn-taking could be linked to stress related behaviors, such as lip licking. Owner in dyads where more vocal overlap occurred tended to score the owner-dog relationship and communication abilities with lower grades, suggesting the absence of turn-taking was experienced as unpleasant for the owner as well. In conclusion, findings from current study highlight the unique coevolution of dogs and humans and opens up for future research of turn-taking in interspecies vocal communication.

(6)

2 Introduction

Dogs (Canis familiaris) are descended from wolves (Canis lupus) and were domesticated thousands of years ago (Vilá et al., 1997), and is today one of our most common pet animals. During the domestication process, dogs have evolved a unique ability to use human social cues as an adaptation to live side by side with humans (Hare et al., 2002). For example, dogs seem to understand when human speech is directed towards them (Ben-Aderet et al., 2017). They can also receive information, for example where food is hidden, by using human gazing and pointing gestures (Kaminski et al., 2012; Lakatos et al., 2012). Dogs are even able to discriminate emotional expression of human faces, assumable by remembering earlier experience of real emotional human faces (Müller et al., 2015). However, it is not only dogs that are able to perceive and act on human communication cues – the interspecies communication understanding is mutual. Humans are for example, independently of prior dog experience, perfectly capable to determine the emotional state of a dog by listening to his or her barks (Pongrácz et al., 2005). Furthermore, both pet owners and non-owners can with an agreement of over 80 %, interpret the facial expression of “fear” and

“curiosity” in dogs (Bahlig-Pieren & Turner, 1999). In addition, communication between dogs and humans can be facilitated by certain behaviors, such as eye contact; gazing into each other’s eyes triggers an oxytocin-mediated positive loop in both dog and dog owner, improving the social bonding between these two (Nagasawa et al., 2015). These examples of obtained interspecies

communicative skills, including vision, vocalization and gestures, strongly highlight the consequences of dog-human coevolution. Current study will be focusing on vocal communication between dogs and humans.

In humans, the majority of languages involves a phenomenon called turn-taking, which can be described as a rapid exchange of short turns at talking (Stivers et al., 2009). Turn-taking in humans is established as early as in mother-infant relationship (Gratier, 2015) and is believed to require highly cognitive complexity (Levinson, 2016). Since turn-taking has also been found in non-human primates, the phenomenon is thought of as being partly genetic and partly learned (Chow et al., 2015). Another aspect of human language is the use of frequency, F0, in speech. In humans, a high F0 (a high vocal pitch) is

interpreted as ‘friendly’ or ‘polite’, and mediate a nonthreatening attitude (Gussenhoven, 2002; Ohala, 1984), whereas low F0 indicate threats and self-confidence (Ohala, 1984). Analyzing the vocal pitch can in other words give information of the emotional state of the speaker.

To my knowledge there is no study that address the question if turn-taking occurs in interspecies vocal communication, such as that between dogs and humans. Therefore, the first of two aims of this pilot study was to develop a method that can test if turn-taking occurs in dog-human

(7)

vocal communication. The method should also be usable to address the question if dog’s vocal pitch differs between and/or follows the human pitch in two different events; 1) refuse condition, where the dog is being refused of a treat, and 2) agree condition, where the dog is told that he or she will get the treat. Though vocal pitch is not further investigated here, the method designed for this study can be used to address the topic in future research. This experiment’s second aim was to analyze if turn-taking occurs in dog-human vocal communication. The study further intended to investigate if vocalization can be correlated with any specific behavior, since behaviors can give an indication of how the dog

experience the situation.

As mentioned, dogs have adapted to understand and use several human communicative cues. It is therefore reasonable to hypothesize that dogs also have adapted to fundamental phenomenon of human language, such as turn-taking. Furthermore, since female dogs seek more human physical contacts than male dogs (Roth & Jensen, 2015; Persson et al., 2015), there might be a difference between the genders in other ways of communicating as well. Hence, the hypothesis is that female and male dogs will differ in vocalization and/or skill at turn-taking in vocal conversations.

3 Material & method 3.1 Participants

Seven dogs (Nmales = 3, Nfemales = 4) and dog owners were audio and video recorded. The dogs where of following breeds: Pumi, Pharaoh dog, Finnish Lapphund, Nova Scotia Duck Tolling Retriever, Shetland Sheepdog, Laponian Herder and Siberian Husky. In order to investigate if age was an affecting variable regarding vocal communication, dogs with the large age range of 9 years were used (6.5 ± 1.52), were the youngest dog was 2 years old and the oldest dog was 11 years old. Exclusively female dog owners were used in this study to reduce redundant variation between the test trials. Dog owners

voluntarily applied to the study through an online questionnaire (appendix 1: Online questionnaire) and all participating owners signed a written consent before performing the trial (appendix 2: Written consent). The dog owners were chosen by their availability during the test period, of what breed their dog was and the age as well as the sex of the dog. To mimic an everyday situation for the owner and the dog, each trial was performed in the home of the dog owner. Therefore, only dog-owner dyads living in Linköping were chosen for this study.

(8)

3.2 Testing procedure 3.2.1 Trials

Each test trial started with a short introduction of the study. The audio

equipment was set up and a microphone was attached to the dog. The owners answered a questionnaire regarding their experience of dogs and how they experience their relationship and communication abilities with their dog on a scale from 1-5, 1 being ‘not close’ or ‘poor’ and 5 being ‘very close’ or ‘very well’ (appendix 3: Relationship). The audio and video recording started when the dog seemed used to the equipment, approximately after 20 minutes.

In order to provide a standardized method for each test dyad, a manuscript regarding a request for a treat or a toy was designed (appendix 4: Manuscript). The manuscript provided guidelines for the owner-dog dialogue but was not designed to fully control the choice of words nor formulation. The manuscript was divided into three steps; 1) owner happily asking the dog if she or he wants a treat or a toy, 2) the owner neutrally telling the dog that she or he has to wait for the treat or the toy (refuse condition), and 3) the owner happily telling the dog that she or he is going to get the treat or the candy (agree condition). All dyads performed one trial each, which consisted of five sessions that were performed continuously after one another (Table 1). Every session included a treat or a toy request. Owner and dog was placed in the same room during a trial but were allowed to move freely around. The whole trial was audio and video recorded.

Table 1. The order of sessions in one trial. All dyads performed one trial each. Session Event

1 Owner and dog speak naturally with each other for 1-2 minutes. 2 Treat request from manuscript.

3 Toy request from manuscript. 4 Treat request from manuscript.

5 Owner and dog speak naturally with each other for 1-2 minutes.

3.2.2 Audio and video recordings

To be able to analyze vocalization of owner and dog separately, both parts were individually audio recorded with a wireless microphone (Sennheiser, ew 100 G3). To optimize the dog’s audio recording, the dog’s microphone was attached to the tip of two twisted pipe-cleansers, which were attached to an adjustable collar (Nobby; Figure 1A). The audio transmitter was placed in a customized made folder, which was attached to an adjustable harness (Metizo Design AB,

(9)

size medium or large; Figure 1A). The owner’s microphone was attached to a headset. To reduce background noise and optimize audio uptake of the owner’s microphone, a horn shape piece of fabric was placed around the owner’s

microphone (Figure 1B). The microphone receivers were connected to a USB audio interface (Focusrite, Scarlett 2i2 2nd Generation) which was plugged into a laptop. The audio recordings were registered in Audacity, version 2.2.2.

(Copyright © 1999-2018 by Dominic Mazzoni). Each trial was also video recorded (Canon, Legria HF G25).

1A) 1B)

Figure 1. Microphone set up of the dog (A) and of the owner (B). The dog’s microphone was attached to a collar, and the audio transmitter was attached to a harness. The owner’s microphone was attached to a headset.

For each dog, the session (treat or toy request) involving the highest duration of vocalization was chosen for analysis. Dog vocalization and vocal overlapping communication were obtained using Audacity, version 2.2.2. Video recordings were edited using iMovie, version 10.1.9

(Copyright © 2001-2013 by Apple Inc).

A behavior study of the chosen session for each dog was conducted using The Observer XT, version 10.5 (Copyright © 1990-2011 by Noldus Information Technology BV). Behaviors of interest in relation to dog vocalization was registered according to the ethogram in Table 2.

(10)

Table 2. Ethogram used to analyze behaviors related to vocalization and vocal overlap in the observed dogs.

Functional term Descriptive term

Vocalization The dog is growling, howling or whining.

Eye seeking contact The dog’s head and eyes are directed towards the owner’s face. Physical contact

Some body part of the dog touches some body part of the owner. Does not include dog’s attempt to take treat or toy from owner’s hand.

Tail wagging Repetitive wagging movement of the tail. The position of the tail can be high or low. Lip licking Dog is snout licking. The dog’s tongue is visible.

Immobile

The dog does not perform any movement. The dog can be standing, sitting or lying down.

3.3 Analysis

The obtained data was analyzed using IBM SPSS Statistics, version

24.0.0.0 (Copyright © IBM Corporation and its licensors 1989, 2016). By performing a Kolmogorov-Smirnov test, one could not assume the data to be normally distributed. Therefore, the data was analyzed using two-tailed non-parametrical tests (α = 0.05). Since this study included a small sample size, mean (instead of median) and standard error of mean (SEM) was calculated to describe the results. To analyze if vocalization, vocal overlap or observed behavior items differed between younger or older dogs, the dogs were divided into two groups: younger (younger than five years; N = 4) and older dogs (older than five years; N = 3). To analyze if vocalization, vocal overlap or behavior related to these factors differed between dogs that vocalized different amounts, the dogs were divided into two groups: dogs vocalizing less than ten seconds (N = 4) and dogs vocalized more than ten seconds (N = 3). The divisions were made at younger or older than five years and less or more than ten seconds of vocalization, since the groups were obvious parted at these intervals. To analyze differences between these groups, as well as gender differences, Mann-Whitney U-tests were performed. To analyze if there were any correlations between behavior, vocalization, vocal overlap and how the owner ranked her and her dog’s relationship and communication abilities, Spearman’s rank correlations were performed.

(11)

4 Result

4.1 Testing procedure

All dogs vocalized in at least one recording session (treat or toy request). By using the method designed for this study, it was possible to measure dog vocalization duration and to detect vocal overlap between owner and dog (Figure 2). Owner and dog were placed near one another during the recording session, resulting in vocalization from one part being registered in both microphones. Therefore, vocal overlapping could not be

determined by exclusively visionally analyze the recordings but needed to be supplemented by audio analyzing as well.

2A)

2B)

Figure 2. Demonstration of 40 seconds audio recordings in Audacity. Example of no overlapping vocalization (A) and overlapping vocalization (B).

4.2 Dog vocalization and vocal overlap

In all test dyads, vocal overlap occurred in less than 35 % of total dog vocalization duration. The duration of vocalization, vocal overlap and who initiated the overlap varied between the dogs (Table 3). The great SEM within each test variable can be explained by the small sample size.

(12)

Table 3. Vocalization and vocal overlap for individual dogs (N = 7). ID Sex Vocalization duration (sec) Vocalization (frequency) Vocal overlap (frequency) Owner initiates overlap (frequency) Dog initiates overlap (frequency) 1 F 12.68 0.00 0.33 0.00 0.00 2 M 0.32 0.00 0.01 0.00 0.00 3 M 1.19 0.38 0.04 1.00 0.00 4 M 20.19 6.09 0.30 0.46 0.54 5 F 1.94 0.51 0.04 0.00 1.00 6 F 23.38 5.04 0.39 0.52 0.48 7 F 1.24 0.00 0.02 0.00 0.00 Mean: 11.07 0.16 0.16 0.28 0.29 SEM: 5.67 0.06 0.06 0.15 0.15

Male and female dogs performed similar vocalization duration (U = 5, N1 = 4, N2 = 3, P > 0.05), vocalization frequency (U = 2, N1 = 4, N2 = 3, P > 0.05; Figure 3) and owner-dog vocal overlapping frequency (U = 5, N1 = 4, N2 = 3, P > 0.05; Figure 3). Neither was there a difference between dogs younger and older than five years regarding vocalization duration (U = 5, N1 = 4, N2 = 3, P > 0.05), vocalization frequency (U = 4.5, N1 = 4, N2 = 3, P > 0.05), or owner-dog vocal overlapping frequency U = 6, N1 = 4, N2 = 3, P > 0.05).

Figure 3. Frequency of vocalization and vocal overlap in female (0.17 ± 0.08; 0.12 ± 0.07; N = 4) and male dogs (0.15 ± 0.12; 0.21 ± 0.10; N = 3).

(13)

Owners with dogs that vocalized less than ten seconds in one session tended to rank the owner-dog relationship higher than owners with dogs that vocalized more than ten seconds during one session (U = 2, N1 = 4, N2 = 3, P = 0.08). In addition, the frequency of overlapping vocalization between owner and dog was negatively correlated with how the owner had scored their relationship (rs = -0.79, N = 7, P < 0.05). Hence, a relationship scored as ‘less close’, showed a higher frequency of vocal overlap (Figure 4A). Likewise, a longer duration of overlap (in seconds) negatively correlated with the owner’s grading of communication abilities between the dog and the owner (rs = -0.83, N = 7, P < 0.05). Hence, dyads where the owner had scored the owner-dog communication abilities with lower grades had a longer duration of overlap (Figure 4A).

4A) 4B)

Figure 4. Correlation between A) the relationship score and vocal overlap frequency, and B) the communication score and duration of vocal overlap in seconds (N = 7).

Overlap frequency was positively correlated with owner initiating the vocal overlap (rs = 0.76, N = 7, P < 0.05), while a longer duration of overlaps in seconds positively correlated with the dog initiating the vocal overlap (rs = 0.82, N = 7, P < 0.05). With a higher vocalization frequency, however, the dog initiated less overlaps (rs = -0.865, N = 7, P < 0.05). The more dogs the owner had have in her life, the less overlapping vocalization tended to occur (rs = -0.69, N = 7, P = 0.09).

4.3 Behaviors correlated with vocalization and overlap

Vocalization and vocal overlap showed correlations with several behavior items, such as eye seeking contact, tail wagging and lip licking (Table 4). Eye seeking contact and tail wagging correlated positively with

(14)

vocalization frequency. In addition, there was a tendency of positive correlation between total duration of tail wagging and total duration of eye seeking contact (rs = 0.77, N = 7, P = 0.07). Lip licking correlated positively with vocal overlap (Table 4). The total duration of immobility could not be correlated with vocalization or vocal overlap (Table 4). Physical contact between dog and owner was displayed in only one session, resulting in no correlation could be found.

Table 4. Correlation between behavior and vocalization. Correlation coefficient, rs, and P-value is represented for each behavior. Significant correlation coefficients (P < 0.05) are underlined.

Behavior N Dog vocalization

(frequency) Vocal overlap (sec) Vocal overlap (frequency) rs P rs P rs P Vocalization1 7 0,13 0.79 0.37 0.41 0.63 0.13

Eye seeking contact1 7 0.81 0.03 -0.63 0.13 -0.15 0.75

Physical contact1 1 - - - - - -

Tail wagging1 6 0.81 0.05 -0.52 0.30 0.30 0.95

Lip licking2 7 0.19 0,68 0.43 0.33 0,81 0.03

Immobile1 6 -0.26 0.62 -0.15 0.77 0.09 0.86

1 Measured in total duration (sec) during a session.

2 Measured in total number of times during a session.

5 Discussion

To my knowledge, this is the first study investigating turn-taking in interspecies vocal communication. The method designed for this

experiment fulfilled its aim and made it possible to analyze vocalization and turn-taking in interspecies vocal communication between dogs and their owners. Vocal overlap occurred in less than 35 % of the total duration of dog-human vocal communication, compared to human languages (Dutch, Swedish and Scottish English) where vocal overlap represents about 40 % of all between-speaker communication (Heldner & Edlund, 2010). These findings suggest that turn taking does exist in the interspecies vocal communication between dog and human in this study. Furthermore, several behaviors could be correlated to dog vocalization and vocal overlap by using the method designed for this experiment. Dogs with a higher vocalization frequency performed a longer duration of eye contact seeking as well as tail wagging behavior. Eye contact is one of several behaviors that is characteristic for dog-human communication (e.g. Nagasawa et al., 2015). In addition, dogs, but not wolves, look into a

(15)

human’s eyes when faced with an unsolvable task (Miklósi et al., 2003). These findings suggest that eye contact plays an important role in dog-human interactions. Tail wagging can be seen as a sign of general excitement (Bradshaw and Brown 1990, through Tami and Gallagher, 2009). Furthermore, dogs wag their tail asymmetrical as a response to different emotional stimuli; the tails wag more to the right when exposed to a picture of the owner, and more to the left when exposed to a picture of a stranger (Quaranta et al., 2007). However, this study did not

approach the symmetry of tail wagging and can therefore not address the topic with regard to this. Nevertheless, since current study demonstrate that eye seeking contact as well as tail wagging increased with dog vocalization frequency, this could indicate that the dog experiences positive arousal or wellbeing when vocally communicating with his or her owner.

Even though turn-taking took place in the majority of vocal

communication, vocal overlap occurred as well. A higher frequency of vocal overlap correlated with more intensive lick lipping of the dog. Lip licking has been suggested to act as an appeasement signal in dog-human communication (Firnkes et al., 2017). The behavior can be related to positive arousal of the dog (Rehn & Keeling, 2011), but can also occur in stressful situations (Beerda et al., 1998, 2000). Nevertheless, since vocal overlap did not correlate with pleasant events (tail wagging or eye

seeking contact) in this study, it is suggested that lip licking in this case increase due to stress. This indicate that dogs could experience the absence of turn-taking in dog-human conversations as stressful and unpleasant. In addition, the frequency, as well as the duration, of vocal overlap was more common in dyads where the owner had ranked the owner-dog relationship and/or communication abilities with lower grades. This suggests that the absence of turn-taking between owner and dog can be experienced as poor communication skills by the owner. Both dog and owner initiated the vocal overlap, indicating that both parties were

responsible for the vocal miscommunication.

Dogs that vocalized more with his or her owner in general initiated less vocal overlap. This indicate that high vocalizing dogs are more skilled in vocal turn-taking. In contrast, older dogs did not perform a higher

frequency of taking than younger dogs. Even though vocal turn-taking seems to be partly based on instinct, the learning process of how to use it appears to be a great component as well (Levinson, 2016).

However, this study found no support that older dogs have learned to wait for their turn to speak compared to younger dogs, suggesting turn-taking is not due to a learning process in this case. In marmoset infants, the development of vocal turn-taking is shaped by the parental feedback

(16)

(Chow et al., 2015). Owner-dog relationship is similar to the relationship between a child and its parents (Topál et al., 1998), and it is therefore reasonable to assume that dogs can receive parental-like feedback from its owner. A dog that use vocalization to communicate with its owner to a high extend receive more communicative feedback, which could result in increased turn-taking capability. For the owner, on the other hand, it seemed to be of relevance how many dogs she had owned in her life, indicating that humans in general vary in their dog communication skills. This could be explained by difference selection pressures of dogs and humans; dogs have been strongly selected for their skill of communicate with humans (Hare et al., 2002; Miklósi et al., 2003; Hare & Tomasello, 2005), whereas humans not have been equally selected for their skill of communicating with dogs.

The gender of the dog did not seem to be of relevance regarding

vocalization duration, vocalization frequency nor the duration of vocal overlap. Hence, vocal communication cannot be compared to physical communication, where female dogs are suggested to be more physical with humans (Roth & Jensen, 2015; Persson et al., 2015).

In general, owners tended to score their relationship with their dog higher if the dog vocalized less. However, one must keep in mind that a

relationship is complex and cannot be explained by a few variables. It has been suggested that dogs mainly use visual communication (Beaver, 1999, through Tami & Gallagher, 2009) and that vocalization is used to attract attention to the visual signals, rather than being a communication method by itself (Shepherd, 2002, through Tami & Gallagher, 2009). It is possible that dogs that use a moderate amount of vocalization to

communicate with its owner have developed a more efficient method to attract attention, for example by touch, or might even use eye contact as a communicative tool in a higher extend than dogs that vocalize more. As mentioned, this pilot study aimed to develop and examine the use of the test procedure, and the number of participants was therefore low, hence the results should be interpreted with caution. Variables that tended to correlate with each other (e.g. eye seeking contact and tail wagging) could have been significant correlated with a larger sample size. For further development of the test procedure, I suggest clarifying the manuscript to reduce variation between the test dyads and to give the owner instructions of where to hold the treat during the treat session, since the owner’s hand placement tended to be in the same line as the dog’s eye seeking contact. In the current study, the owner initiated the talk. Since it in some trials were difficult to trigger the dog to vocalize, I believe the method could be improved by the dog initiating the

(17)

vocalization. This would further make it possible to analyze latency to first vocalization, which could give information about how eager the dog is to speak during refuse and agree condition.

5.1 Societal and ethical implications

Dogs have for a long time been part of our everyday life and is often considered a family member. Dog have been bred for different purposes, and they all have a broad function in the society; dogs are held as company, are used when hunting and in different types of sport events. Nowadays, dogs also work side by side with humans in several professions, for example in the military force and in human health care. One can assume that there are several fields of dog capabilities yet to be discovered.

Findings from this pilot study leads to an increased knowledge of the dogs’ unique coevolution with humans. Since dogs are one of our most common pet animals, it is of high importance to understand dog ethology in order improve dog welfare.

Since this study suggest that turn-taking exists in interspecies vocal

communication between dogs and humans, this could motivate further research of the topic in other species as well.

According to Swedish legislation of pet animals, an ethical permit was not needed to perform this study.

5.2 Conclusion

The method designed for this study made it possible to analyze

vocalization and turn-taking in dog-human communication. Current study suggest that turn-taking do exist in dog-human vocal communication. Furthermore, vocal communication seems to be related to positive

behaviors of the dog, such as eye seeking contact and tail wagging, while vocal overlap correlates with stressful events, such as lip licking. In addition, owners in dyads where more overlap occurred scored the owner-dog communication abilities poorer, indicating the

miscommunication is notable for humans as well. These findings highlight the consequences of dog-human coevolution and might also encourage research of turn-taking in interspecies vocal communication in other species as well. Finally, further improvement of how to measure turn-taking in interspecies vocal communication in future studies is suggested.

(18)

6 Acknowledgement

I want to thank my supervisor Lina Roth for all her help and support, and my examinator, Per Jensen. I also want to thank Ann-Sofie Sundman for the help with testing the recording material. I am very thankful to all the participating owners and their dogs, as well as to my opponents, Maria Edstam and Johanna Holmqvist. Thanks to Fanny Edenborg and Kim Johansson for giving their opinions on my text. I also want to thank Caroline Ivarsson and Matilda Stridh for helping me with photographs and analyze of the video recordings. Finally, I want to thank Metizo Design AB for providing customized made material to the study.

7 References

Bahlig-Pieren, Z., & Turner, D. C. (1999). Anthropomorphic

interpretations and ethological despricptions of dog and cat behavior by lay people. Anthrozoos 12; 205-210.

Ben-Aderet, T., Gallego, M., Reby, D., & Mathevon, N. (2017). Dog-directed speech: why do we use it and do dogs pay attention to it? Proc. R. Soc. B. 284: 20162429.

Beerda, B., Schilder, M. B. H., van Hooff., J. A. R. A. M., de Vries, H. W., & Mol, J. A. (1998). Behavioural, salivia cortisol and heart rate responses to different types of stimuli in dogs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci.58, 365-381.

Beerda, B., Schilder, M. B. H., van Hooff., J. A. R. A. M., de Vries, H. W., & Mol, J. A. (2000). Behavioural and hormonal indicators of enduring environmental stress in dogs. Anim. Welf. 9; 49-62.

Chow, C. P., Mitchell, J. F., & Miller, C. T. (2015). Vocal turn-taking in a non-human primate is learned during ontogeny. Proc. R. Soc. B., 282; 20150069.

Firnkes, A., Bartels, A., Bidoli, E., & Erhard, M. (2017). Appeasement signal used by dogs during dog-human communication. J. Vet. Behav. 19; 35–44.

Gratier, M., Devouche, E., Guellai, B., Infanti, R., Yilmaz, E., & Parlato-Oliveira, E. (2015). Early developmental of turn-taking in vocal

(19)

Gussenhoven, C. (2002). Intonation and interpretation: phonetics and phonology. Centre for Language Studies, University of Nijmegen, The Netherlands.

Hare, B., & Tomasello, M. (2005). Human-like social skills in dogs? Trends. Cogn. Sci. 9; 439-444.

Hare, B., Brown, M., Williamson, C., & Tomasello, M. (2002). The domestication of social cognition in dogs. Sci. 198: 1634-1636, Heldner, M., & Edlund, J. (2010). Pauses, gaps and overlap in conversations. J. Phon. 38; 555-568.

Kaminski, J., Schulz, L., & Tomasello, M. (2012). How dogs know when communication is indented for them. Dev. Sci. 15; 222-232.

Lakatos, G., Gácsi, M., & Topál, J. (2012). Comprehension and

utilization of pointing gestures and gazing in dog-human communication in relatively complex situations. Anim. Cogn. 15; 201-213.

Levison, S. C. (2016). Turn-taking in human communication – origins and implications for language processing. Trends Cogn. Sci. 20, 6-14. Miklósi, Á., Kubinyi, E., Topál, J., Gácsi, M., Virányi, & Csányi, V. (2003). A simple reason for big difference: wolves do not look back at humans, but dogs do. Curr. Biol. 13; 763-766.

Müller, C. A., Schmitt, K., Barber, A. L. A., & Huber, L. (2015). Dogs can discriminate emotional expressions of human faces. Curr. Bio. 25; 601–605.

Nagasawa, M., Mitsui, S., En, S., Ohtani, N., Ohta, M., Mogi, K., Kikusui, T., Onaka, T., & Sakuma, Y. (2015). Oxytocin-gaze positive loop and the coevolution of human-dog bonds. Sci. 384; 333-336. Ohala, J. J. (1984). An ethological perspective on common cross-language utilization of F0 of voice. Phonetica 41; 1-16.

Persson, M. E., Roth, L. S. V., Johnsson, M., Wright, D., & Jensen, P. (2015). Human-directed social behavior in dogs shows significant heritability. Genes Brain Behav. 14; 337-344.

Pongrácz, P., Molnár, C., Miklósi, Á., & Csányi. (2005). Human listeners are able to classify dog (Canis familiaris) barks recorded in different situations. Jour. Comp. Psych. 119; 136-144.

(20)

Quaranta, A., Siniscalchi, M., & Vallortigara, G. (2007). Asymmetric tail-wagging responses by dogs to different emotive stimuli. Curr. Biol. 17; R199-R201.

Rehn, T., & Keeling, L. J. (2011). The effect of time left alone at home on dog welfare. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 129; 129-135.

Roth, L. S. V., & Jensen, P. (2015). Assessing companion dog behavior in a social setting. J. Vet. Behav. 10; 315-323.

Stivers, T., et al. (2009). Universals and cultural variation in turn-taking in conversations. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 106; 10587-10592. Tami, G., & Gallagher, A. (2009). Description of the behavior of domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) by experienced and inexperienced people. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 120; 159-169.

Topál, J., Miklósi, À., Csányi, V., & Dóka, A. (1998). Attachment

behavior in dogs (Canis familiaris): A new application of the Ainsworth’s (1969) strange situation test. J. Comp. Psychol. 112; 219-229.

Vilá, C., Savolainen, Maldonado, J. E., Amorim, I. R., Rice, J. E.,

Honeycutt, R. L., Crandall, K. A., Lundeberg, J., & Wayne, R. K. (1997). Multiple and ancient origins of the domestic dog. Sci. 276; 1687-1689.

(21)

8 Appendix

(22)
(23)
(24)

References

Related documents

Francis writes about extraordinary (live donor kidney transplant surgery) and mundane (nausea and dizziness) medical moments and attends to medics and patients, human organs and

The second theme concerns impacts, furthermore questions about daily use of technology and its affect on other parts of life, but also smart environments and

However, most contributions in the literature are limited to a more aggregated view on co- creation and in the language of Oliver (2006) service encounter research has been

The language of dierential and commutative algebra is very appropriate for polynomial systems so we will assume some familiarity with terms such as ideal, dierential ideal etc...

Division of Fluid and Mechatronic Systems Department of Management and Engineering

As part of the EU project SWEEPER, we are working on developing an autonomous robot able to harvest sweet pepper fruits

The definition of in vivo amyloid (Nomenclature Committee of the International Society of Amyloidosis) declares that there has to be tissue deposits, with a

We use our previous results on subdifferentiability and dual character- ization of optimal decomposition for an infimal convolution to establish mathematical properties of