• No results found

David J. T. Sumpter Uppsala, 2012

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2022

Share "David J. T. Sumpter Uppsala, 2012"

Copied!
25
0
0

Loading.... (view fulltext now)

Full text

(1)

David J. T. Sumpter

Uppsala, 2012

(2)
(3)

ž 

Morning lectures by David Sumpter.

ž 

Afternoon practical sessions.

1. 

Differential equation models (Stam Nicolis)

2. 

Self-propelled particles (Daniel Strömbom)

3. 

Data analysis (Andrea Perna)

4. 

Model fitting (Richard Mann)

ž 

Wednesday guest talks in the morning (Mario Romero, Jens Krause, Peter

Hedström) then free afternoon.

(4)

1, Modelling animal behaviour (1).

2, Functional explanations (2, 10).

3, Information transfer and synergy (3, 10).

4, Information transfer in humans.

5, Group decision-making (4).

6, Collective motion (5).

7, Quantifying individual interactions.

8, Collective structures (7).

9, Negative feedback and regulation (8).

10, Complicated individuals (9).

(5)

ž 

Please ask questions during the lectures (and afterwards).

ž 

Balance between mathematics and biology.

ž 

Ask me if you want me to cover

something in particular later during the week.

ž 

I will put up pdf’s of the talks in a

Dropbox I will share with you.

(6)

Modelling Animal Behaviour

(7)

A way of travelling securely from A to B.

A: Assumptions about the world.

B: Consequences of those assumptions

Mathematics is rigorous thinking.

(8)

1, Explain data as simply as possible.

2, Link together levels of explanation.

3, To provide detailed descriptions.

4, To predict future outcomes.

(9)

Provide one or two simple rules from which everything else is explained.

This is qualitative modelling, but

necessarily some comparison to data.

Explanation ratio: Explained/Assumptions

Dawkins: http://richarddawkins.net/articles/2236

(10)

is the number of ‘infected’ individuals;

is the rate at which they contact others;

is the probability that a contact is with an uninfected individual.

dx

dt = px(1− x n ) x

px

(1− x

n )

(11)

Disease spread:

is the number infected;

is the rate of contacts;

is the proportion of individuals that are susceptible.

dx

dt = px(1− x n )

x px

(1− x n )

Nannyonga et al. (2012) PLoS One

(12)

Yeast growth:

is the number of bacteria;

is the rate of dividing;

is the proportion of environment which is unoccupied.

dx

dt = px(1− x n )

x px

(1− x n )

4 × 108

3 × 108

2 × 108

1 × 108

Population size

10 20 30 40 50 60 70

Haploids

Time in hours

10 20 30 40 50 60 70

Time in hours Diploids

Population size

(a)

(b)

4 × 108

3 × 108

2 × 108

1 × 108

(13)

Information:

are the ants foraging at a site;

is the rate of recruitment to a site;

is the proportion of colony who don’t know about the site yet.

dx

dt = px(1− x n )

x px

(1− x n )

Detrain (2001) Self-organisation in biological systems

13

Detrain et a1

Number of ants on the area

800 2 A

60 80

Time (min)

0 20 40 60 80 1 00 120 140

Time (min) Number of ants on the area

Figure 2. Time evolution of the number of workers on the arena during five experiments each on Societies 1 (fig. 2a, large colony) and 2 (fig. 2b, small

800

600

400

2 B

-

-

(14)

Innovation (Diffusion):

is the number adopting a technology;

is the rate of informing about technology;

is the proportion of individuals not yet using the technology.

dx

dt = px(1− x n )

x px

(1− x

n )

(15)

Large aggregates cannot be understood by simple extrapolation from the behaviour of a few particles.

Need mathematical models to integrate our understanding from one level to the next.

Explanation ratio may be lower, but more detailed.

15

(16)
(17)

Couzin et al. (2002) Journal of theoretical biology

17

within this zone:

drðt þ tÞ ¼ %X

nr

jai

rijðtÞ rijðtÞ

!

!

!

!

; ð1Þ

where rij¼ (cj – ci)/|(cj – ci)| is the unit vector in the direction of neighbour j. Note that storr

avoids singularities in eqn (1). This behavioural rule has the highest priority in the model, so that if nr 4 0, the desired direction diðt þ tÞ ¼ drðt þ tÞ: The zone of repulsion can be interpreted as individuals maintaining personal space, or avoiding collisions.

If no neighbours are within the zone of repulsion (nr ¼ 0), the individual responds to others within the ‘‘zone of orientation’’ (zoo) and the ‘‘zone of attraction’’ (zoa). These zones are spherical, except for a volume behind the individual within which neighbours are unde- tectable. This ‘‘blind volume’’ is defined as a cone with interior angle (360%a)1, where a is defined as the field of perception (see Fig. 1). An individual with a ¼ 3601 can respond to others in any direction within the behavioural zones.

The zone of orientation contains no detectable neighbours with rrpjðcj2ciÞjoro and the zone of attraction no detectable neighbours with ropjðcj2ciÞjpra: The widths of these zones are defined as Dr ¼ r % r and Dr ¼ r % r :

An individual will attempt to align itself with neighbours within the zone of orientation, giving

doðt þ tÞ ¼ X

no

j¼1

vjðtÞ vjðtÞ

!

!

!

!

ð2Þ

and towards the positions of individuals within the zone of attraction

daðt þ tÞ ¼ X

na jai

rijðtÞ rijðtÞ

!

!

!

!

: ð3Þ

The attraction represents the tendency of organ- isms to join groups and to avoid being on the periphery, whereas the orientation allows collec- tive movement by minimizing the number of collisions between individuals. If neighbours are only found in the zoo (n ¼ no), then diðt þ tÞ ¼ doðt þ tÞ; likewise if all neighbours are in the zoa (n ¼ na), then diðt þ tÞ ¼ daðt þ tÞ: If neighbours are found in both zones, then diðt þ tÞ ¼

1

2½doðt þ tÞ þ daðt þ tÞ': In the eventuality that the social forces result in a zero vector, or if no individuals are detected, then diðt þ tÞ ¼ viðtÞ:

Decision making in animals is subject to stochastic effects (e.g. sensory error, movement error). This is simulated by modifying diðt þ tÞ by rotating it by an angle taken at random from a spherically wrapped Gaussian distribution with standard deviation, s (Table 1).

After the above process has been performed for every individual they turn towards the direction vector diðt þ tÞ by the turning rate y:

Provided the angle between vi(t) and diðt þ tÞ is less than the maximum turning angle yt; then viðt þ tÞ ¼ diðt þ tÞ; if not, the individual rotates by yt towards the desired direction. To simplify the analysis of parameter space initially we assume that individuals move at a constant speed of s units per second (we investigate the importance of differences in individual speed below). Following these rules individual trajec- tories can be integrated over time to explore how the behavioural responses influence collective behaviour.

ANALYSIS OF THE MODEL

To analyse the collective behaviour of the model, we explore the consequences of changing

z x

y

zoa

zor zoo

α° (360 - α

Fig. 1. Representation of an individual in the model centred at the origin: zor ¼ zone of repulsion, zoo ¼ zone of orientation, zoa ¼ zone of attraction. The possible ‘‘blind volume’’ behind an individual is also shown. a ¼ field of perception.

COLLECTIVE BEHAVIOUR OF ANIMAL GROUPS 3

(18)

Couzin et al. (2002) Journal of theoretical biology

18

Fig. 3(E) and (F) show the group polarization p

group

and angular momentum m

group

, respec- tively, as Dr

o

and Dr

a

vary. The area of zero values when Dr

o

and Dr

a

are relatively low [Fig. 3(E) and (F), region e] corresponds to the area of parameter space, where groups have a greater than 50% chance of fragmenting. Since the collective behaviour is dependent on group

exist for all group sizes analysed, although the range over which the torus and dynamic parallel groups form tends to decrease as the group size decreases. The field of perception also influences the collective behaviour. The range in which groups form a torus is diminished to a very small range of Dr

o

and Dr

a

when the field of perception, a; is 3601, but increases as a

0 2

4 6

8 10

12 14

Δra

0 2 4 6 8 10 12 14

Δro

0 0.2

0.4 0.6

0.8 1

0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8

1

pgroup

0 2

4 6

8 10

12 14

Δra

0 2 4 6 8 10 12 14

Δro

0 0.2

0.4 0.6

0.8 1

0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8

1

mgroup

a a

b c b

c d

d

e z

(A)

(C) (D)

(B)

(E) (F)

Fig. 3. The collective behaviours exhibited by the model: (A) swarm, (B) torus, (C) dynamic parallel group, (D) highly parallel group. Also shown are the group polarization p

group

(E) and angular momentum m

group

(F) as a function of changes in the size of the zone of orientation Dr

o

and zone of attraction Dr

a

: The areas denoted as (a–d), correspond to the area of parameter space in which the collective behaviours (A–D), respectively, are found. Area (e) corresponds to the region in parameter space, where groups have a greater than 50% chance of fragmenting. N ¼ 100, r

r

¼ 1, a ¼ 270, y ¼ 40, s ¼ 3, s ¼ 0.05. Data shown in (E) and (F) are the mean of 30 replicates per parameter combination.

I. D. COUZIN ET AL.

6

(19)

Put everything we know down in one place.

Quantitative modelling.

Test that this knowledge is self-consistent.

Find out if we really do understand how the

system works.

(20)
(21)

Pratt et al. (2005) Animal behaviour

SearchExplore GetLost

SearchAssess

Accepti

1 – QuorumMet QuorumMet Recruiti

SearchCanvas

Reverse 1 – Reverse Recruiti GetLost ×

(1 – LostTrans) SearchCommitted

Findi, i 1 – Rejecti, j

Rejecti, j

Findj, i

StopTrans 1 – Stop

Trans 1 – Rejecti, j

Rejecti, j

Findj, i GetLost

Findi, i 1 – Rejecti, j Rejecti, j

Find0, 0

Find0, i 1 – Reject0, j Reject0, j

Find0, j

Reject0, i

1 – Reject0, i

Findi, i

1 – GetLost + GetLost × LostTrans

At Nest (i, f)

Carried (i, f)

Transport (i, f) Carried

(i, f) Search

(i, f)

ForwardLead (i, f) Carried

(i, f) Search

(i, f) At Nest

(i, f) Follow

(i, f) Search

(0, —) At Nest

(0, —) Follow

(0, —)

Arrive at Nest j

Arrive at Nest j

Findj, i Arrive at

Nest j

Arrive at Nest j

At Nest (i, f)

Arrive at Nest i

PickedUpCommitted PickedUpCanvas PickedUpAssess PickedUpExplore Carried

(0, —)

Search (i, f) Follow

(i, f) At Nest

(j, f)

Reverse Tandem (i, f) Exploration

Assessment

Canvassing

Committed

Figure 1. Model of the behaviour of active ants responsible for organizing colony emigrations. Boxes represent behavioural states and arrows represent transitions between them. Colours indicate the four major levels of an ant’s commitment to a candidate nest site: Exploration (blue), Assessment (red), Canvassing (amber) and Commitment (green). The first subscript i in each state identifies the nest that the ant is currently assessing or recruiting to. The second subscript f identifies the nest from which the ant recruits (either the old nest or a rejected new site to

PRATT ET AL.: MODEL OF NEST SITE SELECTION BY ANTS 1025

(22)
(23)

1, Explain data as simply as possible.

2, Link together levels of explanation.

3, To provide detailed descriptions.

4, To predict future outcomes.

Decreasing level of

abstraction

Increasing level of

description

23

(24)

1, Explain data as simply as possible.

2, Link together levels of explanation.

3, To provide detailed descriptions.

4, To predict future outcomes.

Qualitative comparison between systems

Quantitative description of particular system

(25)

1, Explain data as simply as possible.

2, Link together levels of explanation.

3, To provide detailed descriptions.

4, To predict future outcomes.

Fun!

Hard work

25

References

Related documents

Both Brazil and Sweden have made bilateral cooperation in areas of technology and innovation a top priority. It has been formalized in a series of agreements and made explicit

För att uppskatta den totala effekten av reformerna måste dock hänsyn tas till såväl samt- liga priseffekter som sammansättningseffekter, till följd av ökad försäljningsandel

The increasing availability of data and attention to services has increased the understanding of the contribution of services to innovation and productivity in

Syftet eller förväntan med denna rapport är inte heller att kunna ”mäta” effekter kvantita- tivt, utan att med huvudsakligt fokus på output och resultat i eller från

Generella styrmedel kan ha varit mindre verksamma än man har trott De generella styrmedlen, till skillnad från de specifika styrmedlen, har kommit att användas i större

I regleringsbrevet för 2014 uppdrog Regeringen åt Tillväxtanalys att ”föreslå mätmetoder och indikatorer som kan användas vid utvärdering av de samhällsekonomiska effekterna av

Parallellmarknader innebär dock inte en drivkraft för en grön omställning Ökad andel direktförsäljning räddar många lokala producenter och kan tyckas utgöra en drivkraft

Närmare 90 procent av de statliga medlen (intäkter och utgifter) för näringslivets klimatomställning går till generella styrmedel, det vill säga styrmedel som påverkar