• No results found

Prediagnostic and comorbidity factors in preschool children with autism

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "Prediagnostic and comorbidity factors in preschool children with autism"

Copied!
67
0
0

Loading.... (view fulltext now)

Full text

(1)

Lotta Höglund Carlsson Prediagnostic and comorbidity factors in preschool children with autism

Prediagnostic and comorbidity factors in preschool children with autism

Lotta Höglund Carlsson

Institute of Neuroscience and Physiology at Sahlgrenska Academy University of Gothenburg

(2)
(3)

Prediagnostic and comorbidity factors in preschool children with autism

Lotta Höglund Carlsson

Gillberg Neuropsychiatry Centre Institute of Neuroscience and Physiology Sahlgrenska Academy at University of Gothenburg

Gothenburg 2015

(4)

Cover illustration: Tom Carlsson 3.5 years old

Prediagnostic and comorbidity factors in preschool children with autism

© Lotta Höglund Carlsson 2015 lotta.hoglund.carlsson@gnc.gu.se ISBN 978-91-628-9519-8

Printed in Gothenburg, Sweden 2015 Ale Tryckteam AB, Bohus

To my beloved, big family

(5)

Cover illustration: Tom Carlsson 3.5 years old

Prediagnostic and comorbidity factors in preschool children with autism

© Lotta Höglund Carlsson 2015 lotta.hoglund.carlsson@gnc.gu.se ISBN 978-91-628-9519-8

Printed in Gothenburg, Sweden 2015 Ale Tryckteam AB, Bohus

To my beloved, big family

(6)

Prediagnostic and comorbidity factors in preschool children with autism

Lotta Höglund Carlsson

Gillberg Neuropsychiatry Centre, Institute of Neuroscience and Physiology Sahlgrenska Academy at University of Gothenburg

Göteborg, Sweden

ABSTRACT

The aim of the thesis was to investigate Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) diagnosed in the early years from different angles; screening, load of coexisting disorders, outcome at routine developmental surveillance, and to study a possible background factor (prenatal ultrasound).

The objective of Paper I was to investigate if the CHecklist for Autism in Toddlers (CHAT), when added to the routine 18-month developmental surveillance at Child Healthcare Centres (CHC), would result in earlier diag- nosis and intervention for children with ASD. The study was carried out in southern Stockholm, and 18 - month-old children in northern Stockholm who underwent the same routine developmental surveillance at CHC, but not the CHAT- screening, served as a comparison group. Although a helpful tool, the use of CHAT in the investigated area did not lead to earlier diagnosis of ASD.

In the study reported in Paper II, records from the18-month routine sur- veillance at CHC of children later diagnosed with ASD were reviewed.

The study group consisted of 175 of a total of 208 children with ASD who had been referred to the Autism Center for Young Children (ACYC) in Stockholm for intervention. More than a third of the total group of children with ASD and half of the group with ASD and concomitant intellectual disability (ID) had failed the 18-month routine developmental surveillance, compared to one in fifty in the general child population. When the pres- ence of regulatory problems also was taken into consideration, the differ- ence between ASD and the general child population was even more marked.

(7)

Prediagnostic and comorbidity factors in preschool children with autism

Lotta Höglund Carlsson

Gillberg Neuropsychiatry Centre, Institute of Neuroscience and Physiology Sahlgrenska Academy at University of Gothenburg

Göteborg, Sweden

ABSTRACT

The aim of the thesis was to investigate Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) diagnosed in the early years from different angles; screening, load of coexisting disorders, outcome at routine developmental surveillance, and to study a possible background factor (prenatal ultrasound).

The objective of Paper I was to investigate if the CHecklist for Autism in Toddlers (CHAT), when added to the routine 18-month developmental surveillance at Child Healthcare Centres (CHC), would result in earlier diag- nosis and intervention for children with ASD. The study was carried out in southern Stockholm, and 18 - month-old children in northern Stockholm who underwent the same routine developmental surveillance at CHC, but not the CHAT- screening, served as a comparison group. Although a helpful tool, the use of CHAT in the investigated area did not lead to earlier diagnosis of ASD.

In the study reported in Paper II, records from the18-month routine sur- veillance at CHC of children later diagnosed with ASD were reviewed.

The study group consisted of 175 of a total of 208 children with ASD who had been referred to the Autism Center for Young Children (ACYC) in Stockholm for intervention. More than a third of the total group of children with ASD and half of the group with ASD and concomitant intellectual disability (ID) had failed the 18-month routine developmental surveillance, compared to one in fifty in the general child population. When the pres- ence of regulatory problems also was taken into consideration, the differ- ence between ASD and the general child population was even more marked.

(8)

The aim of Paper III was to examine different coexisting disorders in chil- dren with ASD. From the total group of 208 preschool children referred to in Paper II, 198 had been followed over a two-year period and were the subject of this study. At this follow-up, including broad clinical examina- tions, 91% of the children were found to have at least one coexisting devel- opmental disorder or problem; language disorder being the most com- mon, followed by ID, motor control problems, and severe hyperactivity.

In the fourth study, reported in Paper IV, the research question was whether early (gestational week 12) or later (gestational week 18) prenatal ultrasound would be associated with an increased risk for ASD in the child. The popula- tion under study comprised approximately 29.000 pregnant women, random- ized to early or later ultrasound. The proportion of their children with ASD (with and without ID) was found to be identical in the two groups, 1.2%.

Conclusion Pre-school children with ASD usually have a complex clinical presentation with many more problems than those subsumed under the ASD label. Many of these children, particularly those who also have ID, can be identified at 18 - month routine health surveillance. Adding the CHAT to such surveillance did not, in itself, appear to increase the uptake rate. The fre- quency of ASD was similar in the early and later ultrasound groups.

Keywords: Autism Spectrum Disorder, Intellectual Disability, ESSENCE, Chil- dren, Screening, CHAT, Surveillance, Child Healthcare Centre, Prenatal ultrasound ISBN: 978-91-628-9519-8

SAMMANFATTNING PÅ SVENSKA

Syftet med avhandlingen har varit att belysa frågor rörande tidig upptäckt och riskfaktorer vid autism (Autism Spectrum Disorder, ASD) hos barn ur olika aspekter. Specifikt har frågeställningarna varit (1) om en screeningmetod på BVC skulle kunna bidra till att upptäcka barn med ASD redan vid 18 måna- ders ålder; (2) i vad mån den ”vanliga” 18- månadskontrollen på BVC skulle kunna identifiera barn som senare utvecklar ASD; (3) hur vanligt är det att förskolebarn med ASD också har andra funktionsnedsättningar och svårig- heter; samt (4) om ultraljudsundersökning under graviditeten skulle kunna vara en riskfaktor för senare utveckling av ASD hos barnet.

Det finns idag stöd för att det är viktigt med tidig ASD diagnos – och tidiga insatser – i form av utbildning till föräldrar och till barnets förskolepersonal om funktionsnedsättningen och om metoder som främjar barnets utveckling. I den första delstudien prövades om man genom att addera en screeningmetod till BVC:s rutinkontroll för barn vid 18 månaders ålder skulle kunna spåra ASD tidigare. Screeninginstrumentet omfattade 9 frågor till föräldrarna och 4 lekmoment som BVC – sjuksköterskan genomförde med barnet. Efter positiv screening, och om misstanke på ASD kvarstod efter ytterligare bedömning av barnläkare på BVC, remitterades barnet för utredning med frågeställning ASD. Screeningmetoden användes i den södra delen av Stockholms län och barnen i den norra delen – där screeninginstrumentet inte användes – utgjorde en jämförelsegrupp. Det visade sig att screeningmetoden i sig själv inte bi- drog till att fler barn med ASD kunde spåras.

I den andra delstudien ingick 208 barn som hade remitterats till ett habilite- ringscenter i Stockholm, specialiserat för förskolebarn med ASD, för att få tidiga insatser och stöd. Gruppen var populationsbaserad, det vill säga repre- sentativ för små barn som tidigt utretts och fått diagnos ASD i den studerade regionen. För att avgöra i vad mån BVC:s ”vanliga” 18-månaderskontroll skulle ha kunnat spåra svårigheter som kunde indicera ASD analyserades barnens BVC – journaler. För 175 av de 208 barnen kunde BVC – journalen granskas. Studien visade att en tredjedel av barnen som senare fått diagnos ASD inte hade klarat det antal uppgifter som 98 % av barn i allmänhet klarar vid 18 månaders ålder. I den grupp av barn med ASD som också hade en intellektuell funktionsnedsättning hade hälften inte klarat 18 – månadskon- trollen. Om man vid denna utvecklingskontroll även skulle ha beaktat andra problem hos barnet i form av sömnsvårigheter, skrikighet och mat/upp- födningssvårigheter skulle ännu fler barn med ASD ha kunnat spåras enbart med den nu tillgängliga utvecklingskontrollen vid 18 månaders ålder.

(9)

The aim of Paper III was to examine different coexisting disorders in chil- dren with ASD. From the total group of 208 preschool children referred to in Paper II, 198 had been followed over a two-year period and were the subject of this study. At this follow-up, including broad clinical examina- tions, 91% of the children were found to have at least one coexisting devel- opmental disorder or problem; language disorder being the most com- mon, followed by ID, motor control problems, and severe hyperactivity.

In the fourth study, reported in Paper IV, the research question was whether early (gestational week 12) or later (gestational week 18) prenatal ultrasound would be associated with an increased risk for ASD in the child. The popula- tion under study comprised approximately 29.000 pregnant women, random- ized to early or later ultrasound. The proportion of their children with ASD (with and without ID) was found to be identical in the two groups, 1.2%.

Conclusion Pre-school children with ASD usually have a complex clinical presentation with many more problems than those subsumed under the ASD label. Many of these children, particularly those who also have ID, can be identified at 18 - month routine health surveillance. Adding the CHAT to such surveillance did not, in itself, appear to increase the uptake rate. The fre- quency of ASD was similar in the early and later ultrasound groups.

Keywords: Autism Spectrum Disorder, Intellectual Disability, ESSENCE, Chil- dren, Screening, CHAT, Surveillance, Child Healthcare Centre, Prenatal ultrasound ISBN: 978-91-628-9519-8

SAMMANFATTNING PÅ SVENSKA

Syftet med avhandlingen har varit att belysa frågor rörande tidig upptäckt och riskfaktorer vid autism (Autism Spectrum Disorder, ASD) hos barn ur olika aspekter. Specifikt har frågeställningarna varit (1) om en screeningmetod på BVC skulle kunna bidra till att upptäcka barn med ASD redan vid 18 måna- ders ålder; (2) i vad mån den ”vanliga” 18- månadskontrollen på BVC skulle kunna identifiera barn som senare utvecklar ASD; (3) hur vanligt är det att förskolebarn med ASD också har andra funktionsnedsättningar och svårig- heter; samt (4) om ultraljudsundersökning under graviditeten skulle kunna vara en riskfaktor för senare utveckling av ASD hos barnet.

Det finns idag stöd för att det är viktigt med tidig ASD diagnos – och tidiga insatser – i form av utbildning till föräldrar och till barnets förskolepersonal om funktionsnedsättningen och om metoder som främjar barnets utveckling. I den första delstudien prövades om man genom att addera en screeningmetod till BVC:s rutinkontroll för barn vid 18 månaders ålder skulle kunna spåra ASD tidigare. Screeninginstrumentet omfattade 9 frågor till föräldrarna och 4 lekmoment som BVC – sjuksköterskan genomförde med barnet. Efter positiv screening, och om misstanke på ASD kvarstod efter ytterligare bedömning av barnläkare på BVC, remitterades barnet för utredning med frågeställning ASD. Screeningmetoden användes i den södra delen av Stockholms län och barnen i den norra delen – där screeninginstrumentet inte användes – utgjorde en jämförelsegrupp. Det visade sig att screeningmetoden i sig själv inte bi- drog till att fler barn med ASD kunde spåras.

I den andra delstudien ingick 208 barn som hade remitterats till ett habilite- ringscenter i Stockholm, specialiserat för förskolebarn med ASD, för att få tidiga insatser och stöd. Gruppen var populationsbaserad, det vill säga repre- sentativ för små barn som tidigt utretts och fått diagnos ASD i den studerade regionen. För att avgöra i vad mån BVC:s ”vanliga” 18-månaderskontroll skulle ha kunnat spåra svårigheter som kunde indicera ASD analyserades barnens BVC – journaler. För 175 av de 208 barnen kunde BVC – journalen granskas. Studien visade att en tredjedel av barnen som senare fått diagnos ASD inte hade klarat det antal uppgifter som 98 % av barn i allmänhet klarar vid 18 månaders ålder. I den grupp av barn med ASD som också hade en intellektuell funktionsnedsättning hade hälften inte klarat 18 – månadskon- trollen. Om man vid denna utvecklingskontroll även skulle ha beaktat andra problem hos barnet i form av sömnsvårigheter, skrikighet och mat/upp- födningssvårigheter skulle ännu fler barn med ASD ha kunnat spåras enbart med den nu tillgängliga utvecklingskontrollen vid 18 månaders ålder.

(10)

I den tredje delstudien följdes förskolebarnen med ASD från den just nämnda delstudien upp då de hade haft insatser från det specialiserade habiliterings- centret under två år. Vid uppföljningen, då 198 av de 208 barnen deltog, be- dömdes i vad mån andra funktionsnedsättningar, utöver ASD, förelåg. Det visade sig att 91% av barnen hade ytterligare minst en ytterligare funktions- nedsättning, av vilka de vanligaste var språkstörning, intellektuell funktions- nedsättning och hyperaktivitet. Barn med ”autistic disorder” hade högre andel ytterligare funktionsnedsättningar jämfört med de barn som hade lindrigare former av ASD. Studien visar vikten av att alltid utreda barn med misstänkt ASD i ett brett perspektiv.

Vid ASD föreligger många kända orsaksfaktorer och kunskapen om viktiga genetiska faktorer har ökat starkt under senare år. Exempel på icke-genetiska faktorer är vissa infektioner, toxiner och läkemedel som kan skada fostret under graviditet. Forskare har också diskuterat om ultraljud under graviditet skulle kunna innebära en risk för fostret. Studier har t.ex. visat att barn som blivit undersökta med ultraljud under fostertiden oftare än förväntat varit vänsterhänta jämfört med barn som inte blivit undersökta med ultraljud. I den fjärde delstudien undersöktes om barn födda efter en tidig ultraljudsunder- sökning, i graviditetsvecka 12, jämfört med barn födda efter ett senare ult- raljud under graviditeten, i vecka 18, hade ökad risk för att utveckla autism.

Studien baserades på data rörande barn till ca 29 000 kvinnor som under gra- viditeten deltagit i en studie och randomiserats till antingen tidigare eller senare ultraljud. Barnen (11-15 år) följdes upp via uppgifter från Försäk- ringskassans register angående beviljade vårdbidrag. I både ”tidig” och ”se- nare” ultraljudsgrupp hade 1.2% av barnen fått diagnos autism, vilket innebär att ingen skillnad kunde påvisas.

LIST OF PAPERS

This thesis is based on the following studies, referred to in the text by their Roman numerals.

I. Höglund Carlsson L, Gillberg C, Lannerö E, Blennow M.A.

(2010). Autism: screening toddlers with CHAT in a child health care programme did not improve early identification.

Acta Pædiatrica. 99, 1897–1899.

II. Höglund Carlsson L, Westerlund J, Barnevik Olsson M, Gillberg C, Fernell E. (2015). Autism spectrum disorders be- fore diagnosis - Developmental assessment at Child

Healthcare Centres at 18 months. Submitted.

III. Höglund Carlsson L, Norrelgen F, Kjellmer L, Westerlund J, Gillberg C, Fernell E. (2013). Coexisting disorders and problems in preschool children with autism spectrum disor- ders. Scientific World Journal. 2013: 213979.

IV. Höglund Carlsson L, Saltvedt S, Anderlid B-M, Westerlund J, Gillberg C, Westgren M, Fernell E. (2015). Ultrasound in the first and second trimester and autism; a prospective ran- domized study. In manuscript.

(11)

I den tredje delstudien följdes förskolebarnen med ASD från den just nämnda delstudien upp då de hade haft insatser från det specialiserade habiliterings- centret under två år. Vid uppföljningen, då 198 av de 208 barnen deltog, be- dömdes i vad mån andra funktionsnedsättningar, utöver ASD, förelåg. Det visade sig att 91% av barnen hade ytterligare minst en ytterligare funktions- nedsättning, av vilka de vanligaste var språkstörning, intellektuell funktions- nedsättning och hyperaktivitet. Barn med ”autistic disorder” hade högre andel ytterligare funktionsnedsättningar jämfört med de barn som hade lindrigare former av ASD. Studien visar vikten av att alltid utreda barn med misstänkt ASD i ett brett perspektiv.

Vid ASD föreligger många kända orsaksfaktorer och kunskapen om viktiga genetiska faktorer har ökat starkt under senare år. Exempel på icke-genetiska faktorer är vissa infektioner, toxiner och läkemedel som kan skada fostret under graviditet. Forskare har också diskuterat om ultraljud under graviditet skulle kunna innebära en risk för fostret. Studier har t.ex. visat att barn som blivit undersökta med ultraljud under fostertiden oftare än förväntat varit vänsterhänta jämfört med barn som inte blivit undersökta med ultraljud. I den fjärde delstudien undersöktes om barn födda efter en tidig ultraljudsunder- sökning, i graviditetsvecka 12, jämfört med barn födda efter ett senare ult- raljud under graviditeten, i vecka 18, hade ökad risk för att utveckla autism.

Studien baserades på data rörande barn till ca 29 000 kvinnor som under gra- viditeten deltagit i en studie och randomiserats till antingen tidigare eller senare ultraljud. Barnen (11-15 år) följdes upp via uppgifter från Försäk- ringskassans register angående beviljade vårdbidrag. I både ”tidig” och ”se- nare” ultraljudsgrupp hade 1.2% av barnen fått diagnos autism, vilket innebär att ingen skillnad kunde påvisas.

LIST OF PAPERS

This thesis is based on the following studies, referred to in the text by their Roman numerals.

I. Höglund Carlsson L, Gillberg C, Lannerö E, Blennow M.A.

(2010). Autism: screening toddlers with CHAT in a child health care programme did not improve early identification.

Acta Pædiatrica. 99, 1897–1899.

II. Höglund Carlsson L, Westerlund J, Barnevik Olsson M, Gillberg C, Fernell E. (2015). Autism spectrum disorders be- fore diagnosis - Developmental assessment at Child

Healthcare Centres at 18 months. Submitted.

III. Höglund Carlsson L, Norrelgen F, Kjellmer L, Westerlund J, Gillberg C, Fernell E. (2013). Coexisting disorders and problems in preschool children with autism spectrum disor- ders. Scientific World Journal. 2013: 213979.

IV. Höglund Carlsson L, Saltvedt S, Anderlid B-M, Westerlund J, Gillberg C, Westgren M, Fernell E. (2015). Ultrasound in the first and second trimester and autism; a prospective ran- domized study. In manuscript.

(12)

3.2.2 Nurse survey ... 23

3.2.3 Outcome measure ... 23

3.2.4 Study II ... 23

3.2.5 Registered regulatory problems ... 24

3.2.6 A history of developmental regression ... 24

3.2.7 Study III ... 24

3.2.8 Cognitive assessment ... 24

3.2.9 Speech and language assessment ... 24

3.2.10 ASD assessment ... 25

3.2.11 Motor function assessment ... 25

3.2.12 Vision and Hearing assessment ... 25

3.2.13 Activity and behavioural assessments ... 25

3.2.14 Study IV ... 25

3.2.15 Identification of children with ASD – validation study for the US study ...………..26

3.2.16 Identification of children with ASD in the US study ... 26

3.3 Statistical analyses ... 27

3.4 Ethics ... 27

4 RESULTS ... 28

4.1 Study I ... 28

4.1.1 Overall findings ... 28

4.1.2 Numbers of children with ASD identified with the CHAT ... 28

4.1.3 CHAT-screening not performed at CHC as planned ... 28

4.1.4 The CHC nurse’s attitude to the CHAT- screening ... 29

4.2 Study II ... 29

4.2.1 Overall findings ... 29

4.2.2 Results in the group of children later diagnosed with ASD ... 29

4.2.3 Results in the group with ASD and ID who had passed the surveillance ... 30

4.2.4 Measures taken at CHC after positive screening ... 30

4.3 Study III ... 31

CONTENT ABBREVIATIONS ... V 1 INTRODUCTION ... 1

1.1 Definitions and classifications of ASD ... 1

1.1.1 DSM-IV (1994) and DSM-IV-TR (2000), diagnostic criteria ... 2

1.1.2 DSM-5 (2013) Autism Spectrum Disorder ... 4

1.2 Prevalence ... 4

1.3 Coexisting disorders/”comorbidity”... 5

1.4 Background factors ... 7

1.5 Intervention and support ... 9

1.6 Outcome ... 10

1.7 Human brain development ... 10

1.8 Ultrasound for imaging ... 11

1.8.1 Ultrasound safety in obstetrics ... 12

1.8.2 The ultrasound machines ... 12

1.8.3 Prenatal ultrasound ... 12

1.9 Screening and surveillance ... 13

1.9.1 Screening ... 14

1.9.2 CHAT-screening ... 14

1.9.3 CHC/BVC in Sweden ... 15

2 AIM ... 17

3 PARTICIPANTS AND METHODS ... 18

3.1 Participants ... 18

3.1.1 Study I ... 18

3.1.2 Study II ... 19

3.1.3 Study III... 20

3.1.4 Study IV ... 20

3.2 Methods ... 21

3.2.1 Study I ... 21

(13)

3.2.2 Nurse survey ... 23

3.2.3 Outcome measure ... 23

3.2.4 Study II ... 23

3.2.5 Registered regulatory problems ... 24

3.2.6 A history of developmental regression ... 24

3.2.7 Study III ... 24

3.2.8 Cognitive assessment ... 24

3.2.9 Speech and language assessment ... 24

3.2.10 ASD assessment ... 25

3.2.11 Motor function assessment ... 25

3.2.12 Vision and Hearing assessment ... 25

3.2.13 Activity and behavioural assessments ... 25

3.2.14 Study IV ... 25

3.2.15 Identification of children with ASD – validation study for the US study ...………..26

3.2.16 Identification of children with ASD in the US study ... 26

3.3 Statistical analyses ... 27

3.4 Ethics ... 27

4 RESULTS ... 28

4.1 Study I ... 28

4.1.1 Overall findings ... 28

4.1.2 Numbers of children with ASD identified with the CHAT ... 28

4.1.3 CHAT-screening not performed at CHC as planned ... 28

4.1.4 The CHC nurse’s attitude to the CHAT- screening ... 29

4.2 Study II ... 29

4.2.1 Overall findings ... 29

4.2.2 Results in the group of children later diagnosed with ASD ... 29

4.2.3 Results in the group with ASD and ID who had passed the surveillance ... 30

4.2.4 Measures taken at CHC after positive screening ... 30

4.3 Study III ... 31

CONTENT ABBREVIATIONS ... V 1 INTRODUCTION ... 1

1.1 Definitions and classifications of ASD ... 1

1.1.1 DSM-IV (1994) and DSM-IV-TR (2000), diagnostic criteria ... 2

1.1.2 DSM-5 (2013) Autism Spectrum Disorder ... 4

1.2 Prevalence ... 4

1.3 Coexisting disorders/”comorbidity”... 5

1.4 Background factors ... 7

1.5 Intervention and support ... 9

1.6 Outcome ... 10

1.7 Human brain development ... 10

1.8 Ultrasound for imaging ... 11

1.8.1 Ultrasound safety in obstetrics ... 12

1.8.2 The ultrasound machines ... 12

1.8.3 Prenatal ultrasound ... 12

1.9 Screening and surveillance ... 13

1.9.1 Screening ... 14

1.9.2 CHAT-screening ... 14

1.9.3 CHC/BVC in Sweden ... 15

2 AIM ... 17

3 PARTICIPANTS AND METHODS ... 18

3.1 Participants ... 18

3.1.1 Study I ... 18

3.1.2 Study II ... 19

3.1.3 Study III... 20

3.1.4 Study IV ... 20

3.2 Methods ... 21

3.2.1 Study I ... 21

(14)

4.3.1 Overall findings ... 31

4.3.2 Types and frequencies of coexisting disorders ... 31

4.4 Study IV ... 33

4.4.1 Overall findings ... 33

4.4.2 Frequencies of children with ASD in the group with early vs later prenatal ultrasound... 33

5 DISCUSSION ... 34

5.1 General Discussion ... 34

5.2 Study I ... 34

5.2.1 Strengths and Limitations ... 35

5.2.2 Conclusion ... 35

5.3 Study II ... 36

5.3.1 Strengths and Limitations ... 36

5.3.2 Conclusion ... 37

5.4 Study III ... 37

5.4.1 Strengths and limitations ... 38

5.4.2 Conclusion ... 38

5.5 Study IV ... 39

5.5.1 Strengths and limitations ... 40

5.5.2 Conclusion ... 40

6 SOME CLINICAL CONSIDERATIONS AND A FUTURE PERSPECTIVE... 41

ACKNOWLEDGEMENT ... 42

REFERENCES ... 43

ABBREVIATIONS

ABA Applied Behavior Analysis

ACYC Autism Centre for Young Children ADHD Attention-Deficit / Hyperactivity Disorder ALC Autistic Like Condition

APA American Psychiatric Association ASD Autism Spectrum Disorder

ASSQ Autism Spectrum Screening Questionnaire BVC Barnavårdscentral [in Swedish]

CHAT Checklist for Autism in Toddlers CHC Child Healthcare Centre

CMV Cytomegalovirus CTG Cardiotochography

DISCO The Diagnostic Interview for Social and Communication Disorders

DSM Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders DSM-IV DSM, 4th edition

DSM-5 DSM, 5th edition

EIBI Early Intensive Behavioral Intervention

ESSENCE Early Symptomatic Syndromes Eliciting Neurodevelopmen- tal Clinical Examinations

FDA Food and Drug Administration DNA Deoxyribonucleic acid

(15)

4.3.1 Overall findings ... 31

4.3.2 Types and frequencies of coexisting disorders ... 31

4.4 Study IV ... 33

4.4.1 Overall findings ... 33

4.4.2 Frequencies of children with ASD in the group with early vs later prenatal ultrasound... 33

5 DISCUSSION ... 34

5.1 General Discussion ... 34

5.2 Study I ... 34

5.2.1 Strengths and Limitations ... 35

5.2.2 Conclusion ... 35

5.3 Study II ... 36

5.3.1 Strengths and Limitations ... 36

5.3.2 Conclusion ... 37

5.4 Study III ... 37

5.4.1 Strengths and limitations ... 38

5.4.2 Conclusion ... 38

5.5 Study IV ... 39

5.5.1 Strengths and limitations ... 40

5.5.2 Conclusion ... 40

6 SOME CLINICAL CONSIDERATIONS AND A FUTURE PERSPECTIVE... 41

ACKNOWLEDGEMENT ... 42

REFERENCES ... 43

ABBREVIATIONS

ABA Applied Behavior Analysis

ACYC Autism Centre for Young Children ADHD Attention-Deficit / Hyperactivity Disorder ALC Autistic Like Condition

APA American Psychiatric Association ASD Autism Spectrum Disorder

ASSQ Autism Spectrum Screening Questionnaire BVC Barnavårdscentral [in Swedish]

CHAT Checklist for Autism in Toddlers CHC Child Healthcare Centre

CMV Cytomegalovirus CTG Cardiotochography

DISCO The Diagnostic Interview for Social and Communication Disorders

DSM Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders DSM-IV DSM, 4th edition

DSM-5 DSM, 5th edition

EIBI Early Intensive Behavioral Intervention

ESSENCE Early Symptomatic Syndromes Eliciting Neurodevelopmen- tal Clinical Examinations

FDA Food and Drug Administration DNA Deoxyribonucleic acid

(16)

DCD Developmental Coordination Disorder DQ Developmental Quotient

Hz Hertz = cycles per second

ICD International Classification of Diseases ID Intellectual Disability

IQ Intelligence Quotient

JA-OBS Joint Attention OBervation Schedule MI Mechanical Index

M-CHAT Modified CHecklist for Autism in Toddlers M-CHAT

R/F Modified CHecklist for Autism in Toddlers Revised and with Follow-up

PDD-NOS Pervasive Developmental Disorders-Not Otherwise Specified PPV Positive Predictive Value

PUS Prenatal Ultrasound SD Standard Deviation

SLI Specific Language Impairment SSIA Swedish Social Insurance Agency

TEACCH Treatment and Education of Autistic and related Communi- cation handicapped Children

TI Thermal Index

US Ultrasound

VABS Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales WHO World Health Organization

Lotta Höglund Carlsson

1 INTRODUCTION

Autism spectrum disorders (ASD) represent early-onset neurodevelopmental disorders, characterized by impairments in reciprocal social interac- tion/communication and a restricted range of interests and behaviours. The symptoms are usually associated with cognitive deficits in “theory of mind”, central coherence and executive functions (1). The consequences of these cognitive impairments in daily life vary widely between individuals, depending on the severity of the ASD per se, on coexisting disorders and problems, and on underlying medical/aetiological factors.

Children with ASD may have exhibited symptoms already in infancy, in- cluding abnormalities of joint attention, eye contact, strange reactions to sound and regulatory problems, i.e., sleep problems, excessive crying and feeding difficulties (1-3). Common symptoms during preschool age are de- layed and aberrant language development, peer relationship problems, hyper- activity or passivity, and insistence on sameness with associated temper tan- trums.

A subgroup of children with autism has a different onset of symptoms. After apparently normal early development up to the age of approximately 18 to 24 months, sometimes up to 30 months, there is a loss of verbal and/or non-verbal skills, i.e., a regression in development (4). This regressive trajectory has to be carefully investigated and several medical disorders may underlie this clinical presentation.

Children with ASD and Intellectual Disability (ID) are usually assessed and receive their diagnoses during preschool years, while children with ASD and a general cognitive function within the “normal” variation may not present obvious developmental problems until school age when demands on co- operation with peers and school tasks increase.

1.1 Definitions and classifications of ASD

ASDs are usually defined according to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (American Psychiatric Association, APA). The first DSM appeared in 1952 (DSM-I) and autism was at that time considered a childhood type of schizophrenic reaction, and in the next DSM, from 1968, (DSM-II) a similar term, schizophrenia, childhood type was used. In DSM-III

(17)

DCD Developmental Coordination Disorder DQ Developmental Quotient

Hz Hertz = cycles per second

ICD International Classification of Diseases ID Intellectual Disability

IQ Intelligence Quotient

JA-OBS Joint Attention OBervation Schedule MI Mechanical Index

M-CHAT Modified CHecklist for Autism in Toddlers M-CHAT

R/F Modified CHecklist for Autism in Toddlers Revised and with Follow-up

PDD-NOS Pervasive Developmental Disorders-Not Otherwise Specified PPV Positive Predictive Value

PUS Prenatal Ultrasound SD Standard Deviation

SLI Specific Language Impairment SSIA Swedish Social Insurance Agency

TEACCH Treatment and Education of Autistic and related Communi- cation handicapped Children

TI Thermal Index

US Ultrasound

VABS Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales WHO World Health Organization

Lotta Höglund Carlsson

1 INTRODUCTION

Autism spectrum disorders (ASD) represent early-onset neurodevelopmental disorders, characterized by impairments in reciprocal social interac- tion/communication and a restricted range of interests and behaviours. The symptoms are usually associated with cognitive deficits in “theory of mind”, central coherence and executive functions (1). The consequences of these cognitive impairments in daily life vary widely between individuals, depending on the severity of the ASD per se, on coexisting disorders and problems, and on underlying medical/aetiological factors.

Children with ASD may have exhibited symptoms already in infancy, in- cluding abnormalities of joint attention, eye contact, strange reactions to sound and regulatory problems, i.e., sleep problems, excessive crying and feeding difficulties (1-3). Common symptoms during preschool age are de- layed and aberrant language development, peer relationship problems, hyper- activity or passivity, and insistence on sameness with associated temper tan- trums.

A subgroup of children with autism has a different onset of symptoms. After apparently normal early development up to the age of approximately 18 to 24 months, sometimes up to 30 months, there is a loss of verbal and/or non-verbal skills, i.e., a regression in development (4). This regressive trajectory has to be carefully investigated and several medical disorders may underlie this clinical presentation.

Children with ASD and Intellectual Disability (ID) are usually assessed and receive their diagnoses during preschool years, while children with ASD and a general cognitive function within the “normal” variation may not present obvious developmental problems until school age when demands on co- operation with peers and school tasks increase.

1.1 Definitions and classifications of ASD

ASDs are usually defined according to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (American Psychiatric Association, APA). The first DSM appeared in 1952 (DSM-I) and autism was at that time considered a childhood type of schizophrenic reaction, and in the next DSM, from 1968, (DSM-II) a similar term, schizophrenia, childhood type was used. In DSM-III

(18)

Prediagnostic and comorbidity factors in preschool children with autism

(1980) the term infantile autism was used and in DSM III-R (1987) there was a major change, to autistic disorder. During the study period of the present thesis, the DSM-IV (5), collecting different categories of ASDs under the umbrella term Pervasive Developmental Disorders, was used. The DSM-IV distinguishes between Autistic Disorder, Pervasive Developmental Disor- ders-Not Otherwise Specified (PDD-NOS), Asperger syndrome and Disinte- grative disorder (which overlap with the regressive form of autism).

The fifth updated edition, DSM-5 (6), was introduced in 2013 and in this manual the subcategories have been collapsed into one subtype; Autism Spectrum Disorder. This change can be seen partly as a response to a consid- erable literature having found that diagnoses of the different subtypes (partic- ularly Asperger syndrome and PDD-NOS) are not highly reliable across cli- nicians (7, 8).

A parallel system, used in medical diagnostic registers, is the WHO ICD (International Classification of Diseases) classification system, currently the ICD-10 version, 1992 (9).

1.1.1 DSM-IV (1994) and DSM-IV-TR (2000), diagnostic criteria

Autistic Disorder

A total of six (or more) items from (1), (2), and (3), with at least two from (1), and one each from (2) and (3):

(1) Qualitative impairment in social interaction, as manifested by at least two of the following:

Marked impairment in the use of multiple nonverbal behaviors such as eye- to-eye gaze, facial expression, body postures, and gestures to regulate social interaction

Failure to develop peer relationships appropriate to developmental level A lack of spontaneous seeking to share enjoyment, interests, or achievements with other people (e.g., by a lack of showing, bringing, or pointing out ob- jects of interest)

Lack of social or emotional reciprocity

Lotta Höglund Carlsson

(2) Qualitative impairments in communication as manifested by at least one of the following:

Delay in or total lack of, the development of spoken language (not accompa- nied by an attempt to compensate through alternative modes of communica- tion such as gesture or mime)

In individuals with adequate speech, marked impairment in the ability to initiate or sustain a conversation with others

Stereotyped and repetitive use of language or idiosyncratic language

Lack of varied spontaneous make-believe play or social imitative play appro- priate to developmental level

(3) Restricted, repetitive, and stereotyped patterns of behavior, interests, and activities, as manifested by at least of one of the following:

Encompassing preoccupation with one or more stereotyped and restricted patterns of interest that is abnormal either in intensity or focus

Apparently inflexible adherence to specific, nonfunctional routines or rituals Stereotyped and repetitive motor mannerisms (e.g. hand or finger flapping or twisting, or complex whole body movements)

Persistent preoccupation with parts of object

Delays or abnormal functioning in at least one of the following areas, with onset prior to age 3 years: (1) social interaction, (2) language as used in social communication, or (3) symbolic or imaginative play.

The disturbance is not better accounted for by Rett's disorder or childhood disintegrative disorder.

DSM-IV (1994) Pervasive Developmental Disorder-Not Otherwise Specified (PDD-NOS), diagnostic criteria

This category should be used when there is a severe and pervasive impairment in the development of reciprocal social interaction or verbal and nonverbal communication skills, or when stereotyped behavior, interests, and activities are present, but the criteria are not met for a specific pervasive developmental disorder. Presentations do not meet the criteria for autistic disorder because of

(19)

Prediagnostic and comorbidity factors in preschool children with autism

(1980) the term infantile autism was used and in DSM III-R (1987) there was a major change, to autistic disorder. During the study period of the present thesis, the DSM-IV (5), collecting different categories of ASDs under the umbrella term Pervasive Developmental Disorders, was used. The DSM-IV distinguishes between Autistic Disorder, Pervasive Developmental Disor- ders-Not Otherwise Specified (PDD-NOS), Asperger syndrome and Disinte- grative disorder (which overlap with the regressive form of autism).

The fifth updated edition, DSM-5 (6), was introduced in 2013 and in this manual the subcategories have been collapsed into one subtype; Autism Spectrum Disorder. This change can be seen partly as a response to a consid- erable literature having found that diagnoses of the different subtypes (partic- ularly Asperger syndrome and PDD-NOS) are not highly reliable across cli- nicians (7, 8).

A parallel system, used in medical diagnostic registers, is the WHO ICD (International Classification of Diseases) classification system, currently the ICD-10 version, 1992 (9).

1.1.1 DSM-IV (1994) and DSM-IV-TR (2000), diagnostic criteria

Autistic Disorder

A total of six (or more) items from (1), (2), and (3), with at least two from (1), and one each from (2) and (3):

(1) Qualitative impairment in social interaction, as manifested by at least two of the following:

Marked impairment in the use of multiple nonverbal behaviors such as eye- to-eye gaze, facial expression, body postures, and gestures to regulate social interaction

Failure to develop peer relationships appropriate to developmental level A lack of spontaneous seeking to share enjoyment, interests, or achievements with other people (e.g., by a lack of showing, bringing, or pointing out ob- jects of interest)

Lack of social or emotional reciprocity

Lotta Höglund Carlsson

(2) Qualitative impairments in communication as manifested by at least one of the following:

Delay in or total lack of, the development of spoken language (not accompa- nied by an attempt to compensate through alternative modes of communica- tion such as gesture or mime)

In individuals with adequate speech, marked impairment in the ability to initiate or sustain a conversation with others

Stereotyped and repetitive use of language or idiosyncratic language

Lack of varied spontaneous make-believe play or social imitative play appro- priate to developmental level

(3) Restricted, repetitive, and stereotyped patterns of behavior, interests, and activities, as manifested by at least of one of the following:

Encompassing preoccupation with one or more stereotyped and restricted patterns of interest that is abnormal either in intensity or focus

Apparently inflexible adherence to specific, nonfunctional routines or rituals Stereotyped and repetitive motor mannerisms (e.g. hand or finger flapping or twisting, or complex whole body movements)

Persistent preoccupation with parts of object

Delays or abnormal functioning in at least one of the following areas, with onset prior to age 3 years: (1) social interaction, (2) language as used in social communication, or (3) symbolic or imaginative play.

The disturbance is not better accounted for by Rett's disorder or childhood disintegrative disorder.

DSM-IV (1994) Pervasive Developmental Disorder-Not Otherwise Specified (PDD-NOS), diagnostic criteria

This category should be used when there is a severe and pervasive impairment in the development of reciprocal social interaction or verbal and nonverbal communication skills, or when stereotyped behavior, interests, and activities are present, but the criteria are not met for a specific pervasive developmental disorder. Presentations do not meet the criteria for autistic disorder because of

(20)

Prediagnostic and comorbidity factors in preschool children with autism

late age of onset, atypical symptomatology, or sub threshold symptomatolo- gy, or all of these.

DSM-IV (1994) Asperger syndrome

Asperger syndrome is according to DSM-IV defined with criteria pertaining to impairments in social interaction and restricted repetitive and stereotyped patterns of behaviour, interests and activities, but without qualitative impair- ments in communication.

1.1.2 DSM-5 (2013) Autism Spectrum Disorder

ASD, according to DSM-5, in short, is an umbrella term encompassing per- sistent deficits in social communication and social interaction, across multi- ple contexts, and restricted, repetitive patterns of behaviour, interests or activ- ities. Severities and accompanying impairments should be specified.

1.2 Prevalence

Autism has previously been regarded as a rare disorder. The first prevalence study in England, by Lotter (1966) pointed to a prevalence of 4 in 10,000 children with childhood psychosis/autism (10).

In the late 1970s, the famous Camberwell study in England included children with classical autism as well as with milder variants of autism (11). Wing, Gould and Gillberg, found that more than 20 in 10,000 children had the “triad of social impairment”, today subsumed under the ASD umbrella. However, Gillberg reported that 0.7% of seven-year-olds in Gothenburg already in the mid-1970s had Wing´s triad/ASD (“comorbid” with deficits in attention, motor control and perception) (12).

Prevalence rates of autism or ASD will vary across time due to diagnostic criteria in use. The registered prevalence has increased over time, which could be due to the increased awareness of the condition; awareness of its common association with intellectual disability (ID) and with medically iden- tified syndromes. More attention has also been drawn to the occurrence of ASD in children and adolescents with “normal” intellectual level. In Sweden, increased register rates can probably also be partly accounted for the fact that a diagnosis of autism or ASD opens the door to support and interventions from habilitation and community services, under the act concerning support and services for persons with certain functional impairments (LSS in Swe- dish).

Lotta Höglund Carlsson

Today ASDs are identified in the general population of preschool children at approximately a rate of 0.6%–0.8%, and in school children and young adults at about 1.0% (13-18).

In a Swedish study covering a period of 10 years, a comparison of the annual prevalence of the autism symptom phenotype in children and of registered diagnoses for autism spectrum disorder was carried out. The prevalence of the autism symptom phenotype was found to have remained stable while the official prevalence for registered, clinically diagnosed, autism spectrum dis- order had increased substantially. The authors concluded that the increased prevalence of ASD most likely is attributable to administrative changes, ra- ther than to a “true” increase in the rate of ASD (19).

In the past, the girl:boy ratio in ASD was usually reported as 1:3-5 (20).

However, there have been several more recent studies indicating that the male excess is probably less pronounced. Girls with social/communication problems often have a slightly different symptom presentation and therefore have not been considered sufficiently with regard to ASD – clinically or in research (21).

1.3 Coexisting disorders/”comorbidity”

Most developmental disorders occur in combinations. This is the rule not only for ASD, but also for, for example attention-deficit/hyperactivity disor- der (ADHD) and ID (22, 23).

In the Swedish nationwide twin study, the coexistence of eight psychiatric disorders, known to coexist with ASDs, were investigated in nine-year-old children (24). It was demonstrated that 50% of the children with ASD had indications of four or more coexisting disorders, only 4% did not have any indication of concomitant disorder.

ASD and ID often co-occur and the rate of coexisting ID in children with ASD varies considerably between studied groups; 10-90% (2).

The relationship between specific language impairment (SLI) and ASD was systematically reviewed by Tomblin (25). The author discussed that many of the features reported to be characteristic of SLI were also found in other forms of neurodevelopmental disorders. ASD and what has been known as SLI very likely overlap, reflecting a complex mixture of similarities and dif- ferences.

(21)

Prediagnostic and comorbidity factors in preschool children with autism

late age of onset, atypical symptomatology, or sub threshold symptomatolo- gy, or all of these.

DSM-IV (1994) Asperger syndrome

Asperger syndrome is according to DSM-IV defined with criteria pertaining to impairments in social interaction and restricted repetitive and stereotyped patterns of behaviour, interests and activities, but without qualitative impair- ments in communication.

1.1.2 DSM-5 (2013) Autism Spectrum Disorder

ASD, according to DSM-5, in short, is an umbrella term encompassing per- sistent deficits in social communication and social interaction, across multi- ple contexts, and restricted, repetitive patterns of behaviour, interests or activ- ities. Severities and accompanying impairments should be specified.

1.2 Prevalence

Autism has previously been regarded as a rare disorder. The first prevalence study in England, by Lotter (1966) pointed to a prevalence of 4 in 10,000 children with childhood psychosis/autism (10).

In the late 1970s, the famous Camberwell study in England included children with classical autism as well as with milder variants of autism (11). Wing, Gould and Gillberg, found that more than 20 in 10,000 children had the “triad of social impairment”, today subsumed under the ASD umbrella. However, Gillberg reported that 0.7% of seven-year-olds in Gothenburg already in the mid-1970s had Wing´s triad/ASD (“comorbid” with deficits in attention, motor control and perception) (12).

Prevalence rates of autism or ASD will vary across time due to diagnostic criteria in use. The registered prevalence has increased over time, which could be due to the increased awareness of the condition; awareness of its common association with intellectual disability (ID) and with medically iden- tified syndromes. More attention has also been drawn to the occurrence of ASD in children and adolescents with “normal” intellectual level. In Sweden, increased register rates can probably also be partly accounted for the fact that a diagnosis of autism or ASD opens the door to support and interventions from habilitation and community services, under the act concerning support and services for persons with certain functional impairments (LSS in Swe- dish).

Lotta Höglund Carlsson

Today ASDs are identified in the general population of preschool children at approximately a rate of 0.6%–0.8%, and in school children and young adults at about 1.0% (13-18).

In a Swedish study covering a period of 10 years, a comparison of the annual prevalence of the autism symptom phenotype in children and of registered diagnoses for autism spectrum disorder was carried out. The prevalence of the autism symptom phenotype was found to have remained stable while the official prevalence for registered, clinically diagnosed, autism spectrum dis- order had increased substantially. The authors concluded that the increased prevalence of ASD most likely is attributable to administrative changes, ra- ther than to a “true” increase in the rate of ASD (19).

In the past, the girl:boy ratio in ASD was usually reported as 1:3-5 (20).

However, there have been several more recent studies indicating that the male excess is probably less pronounced. Girls with social/communication problems often have a slightly different symptom presentation and therefore have not been considered sufficiently with regard to ASD – clinically or in research (21).

1.3 Coexisting disorders/”comorbidity”

Most developmental disorders occur in combinations. This is the rule not only for ASD, but also for, for example attention-deficit/hyperactivity disor- der (ADHD) and ID (22, 23).

In the Swedish nationwide twin study, the coexistence of eight psychiatric disorders, known to coexist with ASDs, were investigated in nine-year-old children (24). It was demonstrated that 50% of the children with ASD had indications of four or more coexisting disorders, only 4% did not have any indication of concomitant disorder.

ASD and ID often co-occur and the rate of coexisting ID in children with ASD varies considerably between studied groups; 10-90% (2).

The relationship between specific language impairment (SLI) and ASD was systematically reviewed by Tomblin (25). The author discussed that many of the features reported to be characteristic of SLI were also found in other forms of neurodevelopmental disorders. ASD and what has been known as SLI very likely overlap, reflecting a complex mixture of similarities and dif- ferences.

References

Related documents

INDOT has a primary role in addressing transportation infrastructure as it relates to traffic safety, mindful of the Highway Safety Improvement Program (HSIP) in its approach to

The effects of the students ’ working memory capacity, language comprehension, reading comprehension, school grade and gender and the intervention were analyzed as a

The results show superfinished gears have higher mesh efficiency and lower gear tooth and bulk temperatures, hence lower heat flux compared to ground gears in all

BACKGROUND: Autism spectrum disorder (autism) and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) are likely underdiagnosed in children with cerebral palsy (CP).. Early

Screening positive outcome was more common than identified diagnoses of autism and ADHD in all types and functional levels of CP, although the excess was higher in

Results: Considerable changes with regard to ASD type, general cognitive level, adaptive behaviour and expressive speech were found, especially in children with atypical

Instead, children with cognitive disabilities are of interest in the present study, since supposedly they should be able to travel on school transportation utilising the

Tidigare forskning visar att när arbetstagaren är tvingad till att använda digitala verktyg påverkas motivationen av dennes attityd och ålder.. Cheferna har ett ansvar för att få de