• No results found

Quantitative Trait Evolution in a Changing Environment in a Seed Beetle

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2022

Share "Quantitative Trait Evolution in a Changing Environment in a Seed Beetle"

Copied!
42
0
0

Loading.... (view fulltext now)

Full text

(1)
(2)
(3)

Der Weg ist das Ziel

(4)
(5)

List of Papers

This thesis is based on the following papers, which are referred to in the text by their Roman numerals.

I Hallsson, L.R. and Björklund, M. Sex specific genetic variances in life history and morphological traits of the seed beetle C. maculatus. (In press).

II Hallsson, L.R. and Björklund, M. The colour of noise matters for the response of a population to environmental change. (Manuscript).

III Hallsson, L.R. and Björklund, M. Selection in a fluctuating environment leads to decreased genetic variation and facilitates the evolution of phe- notypic plasticity. (Submitted manuscript).

IV Hallsson, L.R. and Björklund, M. Selection in a fluctuating environment and the evolution of sexual dimorphism in the seed beetle Calloso- bruchus maculatus. (Submitted manuscript).

V Hallsson, L.R, Chenoweth S.F. and Bonduriansky, R. The relative im- portance of genetic and nongenetic inheritance in traits of varying degree of plasticity in Callosobruchus maculatus. (Manuscript).

(6)
(7)

Contents

Introduction ...9

A theoretical model – the starting point ...10

Quantitative genetic framework ...11

Different sexes, traits and the role of parental effects ...14

Aims of the thesis ...15

Methods...16

Study species ...16

Experiments ...17

Before selection ...17

Selection experiment...18

After selection ...19

Cross generational split brood design ...19

Statistical analysis...20

Results and discussion...21

Paper I...21

Paper II ...21

Paper III ...22

Paper IV...23

Paper V ...24

Conclusion and future directions...26

Implications and future research...26

Fluctuations in experimental studies – some thoughts ...27

Sammanfattning på svenska ...29

Zusammenfassung auf Deutsch...31

Acknowledgements ...33

References ...35

(8)
(9)

Introduction

It has become increasingly clear that during the last decades the climate is changing more rapidly than in the preceding periods. The change is for in- stance characterized by an increase in temperature of about 0.6 ˚C during the past 100 years. This is the largest increase in temperature compared to any century during the past 1000 years (Cox et al. 2000, Houghton et al. 2001, Jones & Moberg 2003).

Furthermore the environment is not constant in time, but it is fluctuating.

Fluctuations in the environment are probably the most common and realistic way in how our environment is changing (e.g. Grant & Grant 2002, Lund- berg et al., 2000, Boyce et al. 2006, Schreiber 2010). Many environments are thought to be positively autocorrelated (Halley 1996), that is when con- secutive observations in a time series are expected to be similar (Lawton 1988). An example is the North Atlantic Oscillations (NAO) which affects many species in Europe (Ranta et al. 2006).

A changing environment posses a major challenge to all organisms. Or- ganisms may respond to changing environments by evading the new condi- tions or by adapting to them. A body of evidence has been collected indicat- ing that phenotypic adaptation to climate change is widespread (Pulito &

Berthold 2004). It has been shown that many species respond in their phe- nologies and/or distributions in the direction expected from regional changes in climate (Parmesan & Yohe 2003). For example in birds (e.g. Jonzén et al.

2006) butterflies (e.g. Sparcs & Yates 1997) and amphibians (Beebee 1995), it has been documented that an alteration in the timing of life-history events (e.g. starting of breeding) is a response to climate change. Also, meta- analyses of studies containing information about species and global warming show that an impact of global warming is already discernible in animal and plant populations (Root et al. 2003) and an increased extinction risk from climate change is expected (Thomas et al. 2004, Root et al. 2003).

A species’ response to climate change is highly unique. The response is depending on the species, the environmental- and population-parameters considered, and it is likely to be a result of both plastic behavioural and life history responses as well as evolutionary ones (Jonzén et al. 2006, Gienapp et al. 2008). Plastic responses might be sufficient to cope with an environ- mental change in short term (Przybylo 2000, Chevin & Lande, 2010) but there are limits to plastic responses (Pigliucci 1996, DeWitt et al. 1998; De Jong 2005) and adaptive trait evolution is probably the more likely response

(10)

in long term (Davis & Shaw 2001, Réale et al. 2003, Berteaux 2004, Pulido

& Berthold, 2004, Davis et al., 2005).

The question of how environmental fluctuations impact populations has been investigated using theoretical modelling as well as simulation ap- proaches (Pease et al. 1989, Lynch & Lande 1993, Burger & Lynch 1995, Lande & Shannon 1996) and it has become progressively clear, that fluctua- tions in the environment in general and their pattern of autocorrelation in particular are important when studying the adaptive response of a population to a changing environment (Halley 1996, Ripa & Lundberg 1996, Inchausti

& Halley 2003).

This raises the question of what is a population’s possibility to adapt to a changing climate and how does the population respond to a likely rapid, gradual change in the environment including fluctuations? The way I ap- proached this undoubtedly very big question on an even bigger topic was to use an already existing theoretical model as a starting point. I then conducted an empirical investigation by creating environmental changes in the labora- tory using the bean weevil (Callosobruchus maculatus) as a model species. I imitated environmental change and included environmental stochasticity by exposing different populations with an increase in temperature with or with- out stochasticity added upon the trend of temperature increase (for details see section ‘the selection experiment’).

A theoretical model – the starting point

The model I used as a starting point for my empirical investigations explores the ability of a population to adapt to changing conditions in the environ- ment, such as global warming. It is based on the Breeder’s equation (for details see section ‘quantitative genetic framework’) and the concept of a moving optimum -- where the optimal phenotype of a population is changing due to environmental changes and the population is tracking behind it. Indi- vidual based simulations were performed to study the evolution of a single quantitative trait. Key parameters of the model are strength of selection, heritability of the trait under observation and density compensation. Fur- thermore, this model incorporates not only an environmental trend (increase in temperature over time) (as in models of e.g. Pease et al. 1989, Lynch &

Lande 1993, Burger & Lynch, 1995, Lande & Shannon 1996), but also dif- ferent forms of environmental fluctuations that are added upon the trend of temperature increase. The form of environmental fluctuation depends on the level of serial autocorrelation between years; examples are ‘white noise’, meaning that there is no autocorrelation between successive years and ‘red noise’ implying that there is a positive autocorrelation between successive

(11)

years. This incorporation makes the model more detailed than other models in the same field regarding how this important factor, namely stochasticity, is influencing a population’s response to a changing environment. Simula- tion runs show that levels of selection, heritability, level of density compen- sation and the type of environmental noise matter for a population’s response to a changing environment (the magnitude of evolutionary load; Björklund et al. 2009).

The simulation results by Björklund et al. (2009) suggest that the actual out- come of a selection along a trend is much more dependent on the details of environmental changes when there is ‘red noise’, than when there is ‘white noise’. Furthermore the model suggests that long-term selection with some stochasticity often leads to mal-adaption and even extinction (Björklund et al., 2009). These interesting results strongly suggested that fluctuations in the environment and also their kind of noise are highly important for a popu- lation’s response to environmental change. However, they were ‘only’ theo- retical, and thus an empirical test, in the laboratory with real animals, was greatly needed.

Quantitative genetic framework

The variance in phenotypic traits is a consequence of genetic and environ- mental influences. The quantitative genetic approach aims to partition these sources of phenotypic variance (VP) into its genetic variance (VG) and envi- ronmental variance (VE). The genetic variance can further be partitioned into the additive genetic variance (VA) and non additive genetic variance (includ- ing dominance and epistatitc variance). Heritability (h2) is the most widely estimated and discussed quantity in quantitative genetics. Heritability meas- ures the relative importance of genetic variance in determining the pheno- typic variance and is, in its narrow sense, calculated as the ration VA/VP, which expresses the extent to which phenotypes are determined by the genes transmitted from the parents (Falconer & Mackay 1996). The additive ge- netic variance is an important component since it is the essential cause of resemblance between relatives and therefore the most important determent of the observable genetic properties of a population. Importantly the VA or h2 are necessary for the response to selection and thus key parameters in the study of a population’s response to environmental change. The response to selection can be summarized in the so-called Breeder’s equation, a funda- mental equation for phenotypic evolution, which states that response to se- lection (R) is determined by the heritability of the trait under observation and the strength of selection, s (R = h2s; Falconer & Mackay 1996, Lynch &

Walsh 1998).

(12)

In my study I was interested in the phenotypic response to selection and phenotypic plasticity as well as a set of quantitative genetic parameters such as gene by environment interactions, genetic correlations and genetic and environmental variances. Phenotypic plasticity is the phenomenon where the same genotype produces different phenotypes in different environments (Falconer & Mackay 1996, Lynch & Walsh 1998). Gene by environment interaction (GxE) is the phenomenon in which different genotypes respond differently to environmental variation (Falconer & Mackay 1996, Lynch &

Walsh 1998).

It is extremely useful to think about phenotypic plasticity and GxE (and also the genetic variance) in a reaction norm framework (Fig. 1A, B). A re- action norm is describing the functional relationship between the phenotype and the environment. In a reaction norm plot the environment (two or more) is shown on the x- and the phenotype on the y-axis, each line is representing a genotype and is connecting the genotypic value in one environment with the genotypic value in the other environment and thus shows the different phenotype a genotype produces across environments (Schmalhausen 1949, Via & Lande 1985). The steepness of the slope of the reaction norm is repre- senting the amount of phenotypic plasticity and the spread of points along the vertical axis is describing the genetic variance (in the respective envi- ronment; Fig 1A). GxE measures how much genotypes vary in their reaction norms (Via 1984, Via & Lande 1985). If reaction norms of different geno- types are crossing each other, this is an indication of a GxE (crossing GxE;

Fig. 1B). If the spread of points along the vertical axis differs in one com- pared to the other environment (i.e. the amount of genetic variance differ between the environments) it is an indication of variance GxE (Fig. 1B).

The genetic correlation between environments (rG) indicates the extent to which the phenotypic expressions of genotypes in different environments are related (e.g. Falconer & Mackay 1996, Via & Lande 1985) and thus the in- terdependency of trait expression across environments. Finally, I investi- gated the environmental variance, which in my case is measured as the varia- tion between members of each family and gives an idea of environmental sensitivity/canalization (Stearns et al. 1995).

Plasticity can play a great role in evolution. It has been shown to acceler- ate and/or decelerate (Behera & Nanjundiah, 1995, Papaj 1994, Ancel 2000, Borenstein et al. 2006) genetically based evolution-ary change and can it help populations to cross ‘adaptive valleys’, and there-fore facilitate adapta- tion (e.g. West-Eberhard 2003, Schlichting 2004, Whitlock 1997, Price et al.

2003; Borenstein et al. 2006). Also, adaptation to new environments can be achieved by the conversion of nonheritable environmentally induced varia- tion to heritable variation (e.g. Baldwin 1896, Waddington 1942, 1953, Schmalhausen 1949, Price et al. 2003, West-Eberhard 2003, Schlichting 2004). Phenotypic plasticity has commonly been observed to exhibit genetic variation (Scheiner 1993, 2002) and the evolution of plasticity itself has

(13)

]

]

]

Figure 1 Reaction norms for two hypothetical situations, with the mean pheno- type for each genotype or family on the y-axis and the two environments on the x-axis, i.e. graphical illustration of phenotypic plasticity (PP), genetic variance and gene by environment interaction (GxE) in a reaction norm framework. A) PP, no GxE B) PP and crossing and variance GxE.

(14)

been subject to a vigorous debate (reviewed in Via et al. 1995, Sarkar, 2004, Windig et al. 2004, Lande 2009). Also GxE and rG are important in an evo- lutionary context. Significant GxE and any correlation between -1 and 1 indicate that there is genetic variation for phenotypic plasticity which is needed for its evolution (Via & Lande 1985).

Different sexes, traits and the role of parental effects

A population’s response to environmental change is complex, and although the parameters covered in the Breeder’s equation can be considered the most important ones, there are various factors that might influence or alter this response. While the outlined theoretical background serves as the basis for all Papers included in the thesis and the mentioned parameters are discussed in detailed fashion in Paper III, I investigated several additional aspects to get a more complete picture. The aspects considered are all known to impact a population’s response to selection, such as potential sex differences, differ- ences between traits and parental effects (Paper I, IV and V).

Homologous traits in males and females often differ in their means, a phenomenon called sexual dimorphism. Sexual dimorphism is thought to arise due to sexes having different optima for a (given) trait, because selec- tion is acting differently on males and females, and/or sexes differ in their plasticity in response to environmental change. Furthermore, the specific genetic architecture of a trait (the magnitude/sign of the genetic correlation between sexes) determines how correlated their response to selection is, and how constrainted sexes are in reaching their respective optima. Sex differ- ences have important implications for evolution of sexual dimorphism and sexual conflicts, and also for general evolutionary dynamics (Bonduriansky

& Rowe 2005, Fairbairn & Roff 2006, Fairbairn 2007, Ellgren & Parsch 2007, Rankin & Arnqvist 2008, Poissant et al. 2010). Thus, I measured sexes separately in all experiments in my thesis and addressed the issue of poten- tial sex differences in phenotype, phenotypic plasticity and genetic variances in Paper I and IV in particular.

Parental effects -- that occur when the phenotype of an individual is af- fected by the phenotype or environment of its parents (Mousseau & Fox 1998a) play an important role in evolution (e.g. Mousseau & Fox 1998b, for reviews see Qvarnström & Price 2001, Badyaev & Uller 2009). While I dis- cussed parental effect as one potential explanation to my findings in paper I, I explicitly investigated transgenerational parental effects, as an example of non-genetic inheritance in Paper V, where I looked at their relative impor- tance in determining the response to selection of traits varying in their de- gree of plasticity.

(15)

Aims of the thesis

In my thesis I was interested in the potential for evolutionary response of life-history and morphological traits in a changing climate and I aimed to obtain an understanding of the role of environmental stochasticity in the evolutionary process. With this I would like to contribute to a better under- standing of the potential and patterns of evolutionary change in response to environmental change. The specific questions I addressed were:

• Is there a sufficient amount of additive genetic variance to respond to selection in my population? (Paper I)

• Does environmental noise play a role for a population’s adaptive re- sponse to environmental change? (Paper II)

• How do populations respond to rapid and long term changes in the tem- perature environment and is their response dependent on the presence of fluctuations in the selective past? (Paper III, companion to Paper IV)

• Do sexes respond differently to changes in the environment? (Paper IV, companion to Paper III)

• What is the relative importance of genetic and nongenetic inheritance in traits that differ in their degree of plasticity? (Paper V)

(16)

Methods

Study species

I used the seed beetle Callosobruchus maculatus, which is a cosmopolitan pest of stored legumes (Fabaceae) as my study organisms. This species was chosen because its biology is well known (e.g. Moller et al. 1989, Smith et al. 1989), particularly regarding life history parameters and their relation to temperature.

Mated female seed beetles cement their eggs to the surface of host bean (Messina & Mitchell 1989) and newly-hatched larvae burrow into the seed.

The larval development and pupation are completed entirely within a single host seed. Adults emerging from the bean are well-adapted to storage condi- tions, requiring neither food nor water to reproduce. They live for an average of 10 days (without food or water supply); their entire life cycle from egg to egg is completed in 21-24 days at 30°C (Fricke 2006).

I used a mixed strain (Nigerian Mix strain) of C. maculatus for the ex- periments in my thesis (except experiment in Paper V). The Nigerian mixed strain was established in our laboratory at Uppsala University in 2002 by mixing three beetle populations. The three beetle populations were received from Dr. Peter Credland (University of London). Populations had been col- lected in large numbers from three locations in Nigeria (Oyo, Zaria and Lossa), Africa and had been kept in the laboratory prior to their transfer to our laboratory for 2 years (approx. 24-30 generations). Beetles of the Nige- rian mixed strain served as the stock population for experiments of my thesis and were kept on black-eyed cowpeas (Vigna unguiculata) as a host with 250-350 randomly chosen adult beetles transferred to 120-140 g of host me- dium every new generation in incubators under constant conditions at 30 °C and 45 % RH ± 10 % prior to the selection experiment.

The seed beetles used in the cross generational split brood experiment, were obtained from the Department of Primary Industries and Fisheries (DPIF), Queensland, from an Australian population collected in Kingaroy in 2003. The population was initiated with 357 individuals grown on mung beans (Vigna radiata) and continued at 250-300 individuals per generation since then. A sample of 600 beetles were obtained from this population and continued in the laboratory with ~500 individuals per 200 grams of organic mung beans per generation. Beetles were kept at room temperature for ap- proximately 18 generations prior to the experiment.

(17)

Throughout the experiments included in my thesis I was interested in morphological and life history traits. Traits investigated were weight at emergence (dry weight; Paper I-IV), elytra length (Paper V), length of the first abdominal segment (Paper V), developmental time (egg-to-adult, Paper I-IV), longevity (Paper I), number of individuals hatching (Paper IV) and fecundity (number of offspring; Paper IV).

In C. maculatus a large body size/weight is generally associated with a longer developmental time, increased fecundity and longer life (Moller et al.

1989). However, these relationships have also been shown to be less straightforward (Messina & Fry 2003). Furthermore, C. maculatus exhibits sexual dimorphism in body size (e.g. Bandara & Saxena 1995); with females being larger than males, due to substantial fecundity selection on body size of females (Fox 1993). Fecundity selection is much weaker in males com- pared to females (Savalli & Fox 1999). Moreover, early emerging males have a competitive advantage of getting access to females (Gundtrip et al.

1997, Savalli & Fox 1999), but earlier emergence (shorter developmental time) goes along with a smaller body size (Moller et al. 1989, Guntrip et al.

1997). There are also differences in mortality patterns between the sexes in C. maculatus, such that females live longer than males (Tatar & Carey 1994).

Experiments

I conducted a suite of experiments; one selection experiment that lasted over 18 generations (approximately 1.5 years) and three breeding designs, of which two were associated with the selection experiment (one before and one after selection). The experiments are presented in chronological order.

Before selection

I used the stock population (Nigerian mixed strain) to create my selection lines. In order to verify that there was a significant amount of additive ge- netic variance that makes a response to selection possible, I made a heritabil- ity estimate for the relevant morphological and life history traits in this population.

Heritability was estimated using a half-sib full-sib breeding design. A set of males was mated to several females each, creating full-sibs from each female and half-sibs between females mated with the same male. The vari- ance among half-sib families was used to estimate heritability and additive genetic variance (VA). The sire variance component is assumed to be an un- biased estimator of the additive genetic variance and equals 1/4 VA (Lynch

& Walsh 1998). The advantage of this design is that maternal effects (off-

(18)

spring had different mothers but the same father) and common environ- mental effects (offspring were randomized in the environment they were reared in) were eliminated. Possible paternal effects are assumed to be ab- sent (discussed in Paper I).

Selection experiment

The experimental lines were created from the stock population and used in a selection experiment. This experiment lasted over 18 generations with the temperature changing every generation, i.e. emerging adults of the current generation were transferred to new beans in a new temperature environment, ensuring egg-to adult development of the next generation to be restricted to the new temperature environment. A control (called Control) was kept at a constant temperature of 30°C. The rest of the lines were treated with a suc- cessive increase in temperature from 30°C to 36°C. The difference between selection regimes was how the temperature increased. The first treatment was exposed to this linear increase in temperature only, called Trend. In the other two selection regimes, environmental fluctuations (noise) were added on top of the trend of temperature increase. One treatment had no temporal autocorrelation in the noise, called White treatment, and one treatment had high positive temporal autocorrelation in the noise; called Red treatment.

Red and White treatment lines are collectively referred to as Fluctuation lines.

The trajectories of the temperature changes the Fluctuation lines were ex- posed to, were generated using computer simulations. Red or white noise fluctuations were simulated around a trend of temperature increase. In each generation the temperature increased 0.333°C (as in Trend lines), and noise (red or white respectively) was added upon this increase, with a maximum amplitude of one degree. The simulated temperature value (T) of the previ- ous generation (t-1) served as a starting point for the temperature simulation for the next generation (t) (Eq.1). Therefore, both temperature trend and noise (Φ) of the previous generation served as a baseline for the temperature next generation.

T(t)=T(t-1) + 0.333+ Φ (Eq.1)

where Φ is a first-order time dependent process

Φt+1= τ Φt + β εt+1 (Eq.2)

where ε is a random normal deviate with mean zero and unit variance, β determining the amplitude of the fluctuations, and τ is the parameter deter- mining the degree of autocorrelation (Ripa & Lundberg 1996). For white noise we used τ = 0, and for red noise τ = 0.8.

(19)

Trend, Red and White treatment lines were reared in 36 °C in their 18th and last generation of the selection experiment. Thus all populations origi- nated from the same temperature (30°C) and ended up in the same tempera- ture (36°C), but their ways there differ. Four replicates were formed per treatment including Control, resulting in 16 lines in total.

With this setup I could investigate the effect of a temperature increase on the populations, by comparing Trend and Control lines, and I was able to separate the effect of environmental fluctuations by comparing Fluctuation and Trend lines.

After selection

After 18 generations of selection I conducted a split brood experiment. Pairs of beetles were allowed mate and lay eggs on beans, which were subse- quently split up into two temperature environments. The first environment (30°C) was the temperature all lines had been adapted to before the selection experiment; the second environment (36 °C) was the temperature the selec- tion lines had been selected towards during the selection experiment. The split brood design ensured that full-siblings of each family were raised in both environments. I investigated 10 families per treatment line resulting in 160 families in total.

In this way I could estimate all the relevant parameters and could also in- vestigate the response of a population to a rapid change in temperature envi- ronment by investigating the Control as well as the response to long term changes (including fluctuations) by examining at the selection lines.

Cross generational split brood design

In a separate study I used the Australian population of seed beetles and a cross generational split brood design to investigate the relative roles of ge- netic and nongenetic inheritance of traits that differ in their degree of plastic- ity (Paper V). Mung bean quality was manipulated over two generations. I formed ten virgin male – female pairs (F0 families) and allowed them to mate and lay eggs both low and high quality beans. This resulted in a split- brood design, with full siblings from each family raised in contrasting envi- ronments. Emerging F1 individuals were collected and a subset of the F1

individuals emerging from each type of bean was likewise paired and pro- vided with low and high quality beans to create F2 progeny reared in con- trasting environments. Two traits that differ in their degree of plasticity were measured for a set of individuals of the F2 generation.

This experimental design allowed for estimates of offspring condition and sex as well as effects of parental condition and sex on the trait of interest.

(20)

Furthermore the genetic architecture of the trait in different environments and sexes as well as the relations between them could be assessed.

Statistical analysis

All statistical analysis were conducted in R 1.10.1 (or later versions; R de- velopment Core Team, 2009, 2010, 2011). In several papers of my thesis I used the MCMCglmm package (Hadfield 2010). MCMCglmm is designed to allow the fitting of mixed models to non-Gaussian data within a Bayesian MCMC framework. The advantage of Bayesian methods is their great flexi- bility in dealing with complex data (O’Hara et al. 2008). Further advantages are that MCMC measures of confidence are exact and more accurate than REML estimates, which is especially true when obtaining measures of con- fidence on other derived statistics such as ratios of variances (e.g. variance components), correlations and predictions. The MCMCglmm package allows various residual and random-effect variance structures to be specified, in- cluding heterogeneous variances, unstructured covariance matrices and ran- dom regression (e.g. random slope models). These features of the package were extremely useful for the analysis of the split brood experiment (Paper III, IV) and for the estimation of genetic correlations (between traits and sexes, Paper I, II-V).

(21)

Results and discussion

Paper I

The aim of Paper I was to investigate the genetic architecture of weight at emergence, developmental time and longevity, while examining sexes sepa- rately. I conducted a heritability estimate of the stock population using a paternal half sib full sib breeding design. This population was subsequently used to create my selection lines (Paper II – IV).

First, I found heritability to be significant for all traits under observation.

These results partly contradict results of previous studies made on the same species and traits. This emphasizes the fact that estimates of quantitative genetic parameters are strictly population-, time- and environment-specific and results from one population should not be extrapolated to other popula- tions, and certainly not other species. Second, there were real sex differences in heritability estimates and low genetic correlations between sexes (rMF) for life history traits that are only moderately (weight at emergence) to slightly (longevity) sexually dimorphic. These interesting results contradict findings of previous studies. The most parsimonious explanation to my findings are genetic differences between the sexes and/or sex specific responses to the environment. Importantly the findings highlight that sexes should not be assumed to be the same. Moreover they have great implications for the study of sexual dimorphism and its evolutionary dynamics.

Third, I found sire variance components to be significantly higher than dam variance components for weight at emergence for males and for longev- ity for females. Also this result is interesting, as it is suggesting that non additive genetic and/or environmental effects are present in the sire variance component. These effects are assumed to be absent in a breeding design of this type and thus an assumption of it is violated. This raises the issue of the presence of unnoticed environmental effects in estimates of heritability.

Paper II

Aim of Paper II was to empirically test theoretical model predictions of Björklund et al.’s model (2009; for details see section ‘A theoretical model’). I was particularly interested in whether environmental noise plays a role for a population’s response to environmental change. I exposed the bee-

(22)

tles to temperature changes over 18 generation, with the temperature chang- ing every generation with or without fluctuations (for details see section

‘Selection experiment’).

I could generally confirm theoretical model predictions with my empirical results. The most interesting finding is that the colour of environmental noise seems to be important for a population’s response to environmental change as predicted by Björklund et al. (2009). This is evidenced by an increase in replicate variation in the Red compared to the Trend and White treatment lines.

The results suggest that environmental stochasticity and its pattern matter and have major consequences for the evolutionary response of populations.

Environmental noise is common; many natural time series are positively autocorrelated and both biotic and abiotic time series tend to be red shifted (e.g. Pimm & Redfearn 1988, Cuddington & Yodzis 1999; Inchausti & Hal- ley 2002; Vasseur & Yodzis 2004). Thus a consideration of environmental uncertainties is greatly needed in future research, including empirical and theoretical investigations.

To conclude, in this paper I provide empirical evidence for theoretical model predictions, namely that the pattern of environmental stochasticity is important for the evolutionary response of a population to a changing envi- ronment. I would like to remark that although the computer generated tem- perature changes incorporate differently coloured noise in their time series (i.e. the fluctuation around the trend is either red or white noise, Eq. 2), all resulting noise temperature increases (both red and white) are strongly red- dened (i.e. brown) in their colour of noise. This issue is discussed throughout the paper and potentially implies that the results of this study should be treated with caution.

Paper III

The aim of Paper III was threefold: first, I investigated the short-term re- sponse of a population to a rapid change in the environment; second, I ana- lyzed the long-term response to selection in a changing environment; and third, I was interested in whether the response of a population depends on experienced fluctuations in the environment during selection.

I approached these aims using a quantitative genetic framework, realized by applying a split brood experiment after 18 generations of selection. This breeding design enabled me to assess the, in this context, relevant parameters and I could draw conclusions about a population’s potential to respond to selection, environmental sensitivity, phenotypic plasticity and the potential for phenotypic plasticity to evolve, after both rapid and long term changes in the environment (including fluctuations).

(23)

First, I found a strong response after a rapid shift in the environment. Bee- tles had approximately 6% lower mass at emergence, 25% lower fecundity and 42% lower hatching success. Developmental time was largely unaf- fected (3% change, not significant). This strongly suggests that the new envi- ronment was stressful and not optimal for the beetles. I found the beetles to respond in a plastic way and the genetic variance to be increased in the novel environment. The results were in line with expectations (Lande 2009, Hol- loway et al. 1990, Guntrip et al. 1997, Sgró & Hoffmann 1998).

Second, main phenotypic responses due to selection could be detected in the Trend line, i.e. the line that was exposed to a successive linear increase in temperature. Phenotypic plasticity did not increase due to selection, which was unexpected (Lande 2009). My results suggest that selection in an in- creasing temperature environment is maintaining rather than increasing the existing amount of plasticity.

The third and major aim of this study was to investigate the role of envi- ronmental fluctuation in the evolutionary process of a population. I found a population’s response to environmental change to be dependent on whether or not it experienced fluctuations in its selective past. Selection in a fluctuat- ing environment can lead to: (i) a decreased genetic variance that indicates a reduced potential to respond to selection; (ii) an increased genetic variance that makes sex-specific evolution of plasticity possible; (iii) a reversed rela- tionship of character expression across environments; (iv) a change that fa- cilitates independent trait evolution by decreasing genetic correlations across environments; and (v) a more environmentally canalized response of indi- viduals.

In summary the results of this paper suggest that the potential to respond to selection, environmental sensitivity and the evolution of phenotypic plas- ticity is strongly dependent on the selective past, and on whether selection involved fluctuations in the environment or not.

Paper IV

The aim of Paper IV was to investigate sex differences in response to envi- ronmental change (including fluctuations) in general, and to test two hy- potheses on differential plasticity across sexes in a quantitative genetic framework in particular. I used the same experimental setup as in Paper III.

First, I found that a fluctuating environment can lead to a sex-specific higher potential to respond to selection, trait dependent changes in patterns of sex specific environmental sensitivity (reversal, induction, disappearance) and finally that it facilitates an independent response of the sexes to selec- tion, due to a reduced genetic correlation between the sexes.

(24)

Second, the hypothesis on differential plasticity across the sexes in re- sponse to a changing environment can broadly be defined in two categories with opposite predictions. The adaptive canalization hypothesis (Fairbairn 2005) predicts that traits closely related to fitness will be less plastic because of strong selection acting on them. The condition dependence hypothesis, on the other hand, predicts that traits under strong selection are more likely to capture genetic and environmental effects affecting condition and therefore these can be more plastic (Bonduriansky 2007a,b). I investigated sex differ- ences in plasticity by applying these hypotheses in a quantitative genetic framework. I made use of the fact that the hypotheses are based on different types of selection (stabilizing and directional selection respectively, Bon- duriansky 2007a,b, Stillwell et al. 2010) and combined it with knowledge on expected changes in genetic and environmental variation under stabilizing versus directional selection. Then I made inferences about effects on genetic and environmental variation under the different hypotheses and finally tested the hypotheses empirically.

While I generally found support for the condition dependence hypothesis for body mass at emergence, evidenced by a greater plastic response in fe- males compared to males, that was accompanied by an increase in genetic and environmental variance, the results also suggested that selection in a fluctuating environment can lead to increased stabilizing selection on traits that are most important for fitness in the respective sex, supporting the adap- tive canalization hypothesis.

In summary I emphasize that the selective past of a population and espe- cially fluctuations in the environment are of general importance when study- ing sexual dimorphism and that environmental variation in terms of tempera- ture differences, can create variation in sexual size dimorphism due to sexes differing in the degree of canalization against environmental changes.

Paper V

The aim of Paper V was to investigate the relative role of genetic and non- genetic inheritance in traits that differ in their degree of plasticity.

Both genetic and nongenetic inheritance are important for the evolution of a trait (Jablonka & Lamb 2005, Day & Bonduriansky 2011). In addition, different traits within the same individual can differ in their degree of plas- ticity (Arnqvist & Thornhill 1998, Bonduriansky & Rowe 2005, David et al.

2000), where the degree of trait plasticity reflects the extent to which their expression is influenced by environmental variation (including nongenetic parental effects).

I predicted that more plastic traits will be more amenable to parental ef- fects, and genetic variation/covariance will be more difficult to detect. My

(25)

results confirmed this prediction. Thus, I can conclude that our ability to predict evolutionary change in highly plastic traits may be limited. First, due to difficulties in detecting genetic variance and covariances and second, due to the relative influence of genetic versus nongenetic inheritance that is likely to influence the evolutionary response of a trait. This in turn is be- cause nongenetic inheritance can impact heritability and because plasticity itself might allow for responses to environmental change that cannot be achieved by genes alone.

(26)

Conclusion and future directions

In my thesis I investigate a population’s response to environmental change and I was particularly interested in the role of environmental fluctuations. I found that the selective past is highly important for a population’s current and future response to selection (Paper II-IV), environmental fluctuations play an important role (Paper III + IV) and also the exact pattern of envi- ronmental fluctuations matters (Paper II) for a populations response to envi- ronmental change. Furthermore the response to changes in the environment (including plastic responses) can be different between sexes (Paper I + IV) and traits (V). Moreover, parental effects are important and they might im- pact estimates of quantitative genetic parameters (Paper I). In addition, ac- quired environmental conditions might be transferred across generations via parental effects (Paper V). Both effects can be trait and/or sex specific them- selves (Paper I + V).

Implications and future research

Fluctuations in the environment are common and probably the most realistic and biologically relevant way in how our climate is changing and affecting populations (Steele 1985, Inchausti & Halley 2001). To date the vast major- ity of studies investigating fluctuations are theoretical modelling and simula- tion approaches (Halley 1996, Ripa & Lundberg 1996, Inchausti & Halley 2003). Thus empirical work was and still is greatly needed. With my thesis I create a link between theoretical and empirical work and show empirical evidence for the importance of environmental fluctuations in the evolution- ary process. I see my thesis as a starting point and step in the right direction and suggest that future studies of both theoretical and empirical nature should consider that the environment is fluctuating, especially those studies that are investigating how populations respond to a changing environment, such as climate change.

I show in several papers that estimates of quantitative genetic parameters are highly population-, time-, environment- and sex-specific and that they can be affected by the selective past, including fluctuations. Thus sexes should not be assumed the same and results from one population should not be extrapolated to other populations, environments and certainly not other

(27)

species. My findings also strongly suggest that it is not sufficient to investi- gate quantitative genetic parameters in a single environment, but it would be more accurate to investigate multiple environments.

My results highlight that the genotypic variance covariance matrix (G- matrix) is not constant in time and across environments, and that slope and variation of reaction norms of traits across environments is affected by the selective past (Paper III). In addition, traits can differ in their degree of plas- ticity per se (Paper V). Consequently, reaction norms and their variation are depending on the degree of plasticity of the trait itself and the exact pattern of the selective past. This in turn has major consequences for the evolution- ary response of the trait, i.e. the potential to respond to selection and the relative roles of genetic and nongenetic inheritance in the evolutionary proc- ess. Thus, future modelling, theoretical and empirical work should take into account that the G-matrix is variable, as well as reaction norm slopes and their variance. One modelling approach would be to incorporate a reaction norm and its variance as a parameter into a theoretical model predicting the response to environmental change. Values for the G-matrix and the charac- teristics of the reaction norms could for example be taken from empirical studies that are based on experiments that are the same or similar to the one I conducted in my thesis.

Fluctuations in experimental studies – some thoughts

First, I would like to point out that the time scale of variation in the envi- ronment is important, i.e. the frequency of temperature change in relation to the generation time of the beetles. In my selection experiment I chose the environment to change every generation.

This choice was mainly made for practical reasons and to assure that I in- vestigate a population’s response on only one scale of fluctuation. More frequent changes could have exposed the beetles to within generation fluc- tuations (different scale) and potentially affected different life stages of the beetles differently. This would have complicated the picture greatly, since within generation fluctuations are likely to influence developmental stability (a pattern I did not investigate directly with my experiments). Surely one could argue that within generation fluctuations are more ‘important’ than fluctuations between generations, and maybe could even overwrite the ef- fects seen in my experiments. However, I am unable to tell this from the experiments conducted here. To investigate this issue a different type of experiment is needed. For instance, increasing and/or decreasing the fre- quency of temperature changes in relation to the generation time of the bee- tle (e.g. changing the temperature 2-3 times per generation or only every 2nd or third generation) and then look at the ratio of within/between genera- tion fluctuation.

(28)

Second, I could only detect a phenotypic response in the selection lines that did not include fluctuations. This suggests that if the environment is fluctuating, 18 generations of selection might not have been enough to detect signs of phenotypic adaptation. Thus, future studies should aim for more generations of selection.

Third, in the long term selection experiment I added fluctuations on the trend of temperature increase. After the experiment I tested for the effect of fluctuation on the populations by comparing the trend with the fluctuation lines and with that I can account for the effect of temperature increase. Al- though this is a good estimate of the effect of fluctuations, it always includes the interaction of temperature increase with the fluctuation. Thus, it would have been beneficial to design the experiment with an additional selection line. A temperature selection line with fluctuations only, i.e. without a tem- perature trend. Also, it would have been interesting to not only look at the effect of temperature increase (with and without fluctuations), but also the effect of a temperature trend downwards (with and without fluctuations).

However, since every treatment line had to be kept in a separate temperature chamber, this proposed experimental setup (including 4 x fluctuations (only), 4 x temperature decrease, 4 x temperature decrease with fluctuation) would have exceeded the laboratory (and money) facilities.

(29)

Sammanfattning på svenska

Klimatförändringen är allestädes närvarande. Denna karakteriseras bland annat av en ökning av temperaturen. Temperaturhöjningen är inte konstant över tiden utan varierar. Därmed kan man beskriva förändringarna i miljön på bästa och mest realistiska sätt som en fluktuation i temperaturmiljön.

Temperaturförändringar påverkar organismer på olika sätt. Den nya omgiv- ningstemperaturen är inte alltid till fördel och optimal för individerna utan de reagerar på förändringarna. Det finns olika typer av reaktioner, till exempel kan de undvika den nya miljön och kolonisera andra områden, eller så kan de anpassa sig till den nya temperaturen. En anpassning kan vara plastiskt eller adaptiv. Om en individ förändrar sig på ett plastiskt sätt förändras inte genotypen till att börja med, en adaptiv anpassning däremot innebär en gene- tisk förändring över tiden.

Det finns många teoretiska och matematiska modeller som försöker att prediktera klimatförändringen och hur den påverka populationer, men bara ett fåtal modeller involverar en fluktuerande omgivningsmiljö. En nyligen utvecklad modell har tagit med fluktuationer och resultaten förutsäger att svängningarna i temperaturmiljön spelar en viktig roll för hur en population reagerar på förändringar i miljön.

Syftet med avhandlingen är att undersöka en populations reaktion på klimatförändringar, som här beskrivs som temperaturförändringar. Särskild tyngdpunkt ligger på hur viktiga fluktuationer i miljön är.

För att nå detta mål testade jag en teoretisk modell på ett empiriskt sätt (dvs. i laboratorium med levande organismer) och med hjälp av kvantitativ genetik. I kvantitativ genetik är man intresserad av fenotypen hos en individ och framförallt vill man förklara hur den beskrivs av miljön och genetiska faktorer. I kvantitativa genetiska experiment undersöker man det genetiska sammanhanget mellan individer (ärftlighet) genom att mäta fenotyper. Med denna metod kan man till exempel undersöka evolutionära processer.

Jag genomförde ett antal experiment i laboratoriet. Som studieorganism valde jag bönvivlar, Callsobruchus maculatus. De är särskilt lämpliga för laboratoriearbete och studier av evolutionära processer eftersom de är lätta att föda upp och de har en ganska kort generationstid (en månad). Jag mätte olika egenskaper, till exempel kroppsstorlek, utvecklingstid och fekunditet. I tre försök var jag intresserad av ärftligheten av olika egenskaper, och poten- tiella effekter av föräldrarnas miljö och skillnader mellan könen. I ett stort selektionsexperiment testade jag den teoretiska modellen. Jag utsatte bönviv-

(30)

larna för simulerade temperaturförändringar. Temperaturen förändrades, på olika sätt (med eller utan fluktuationer i temperaturökningen) över totalt 18 generationer (motsvarande 1 ½ år), på olika sätt (med och utom fluktuatio- nen i temperaturökningen). Efter experimentet undersökte jagförändringar hos olika egenskaper.

Med min avhandling kan jag ge empiriskt stödj åt den teoretiska model- len. Fluktuationer i omgivningstemperaturen spelar en viktig roll för en po- pulations reaktion på förändringar i miljön. Dessutom påverkades den gene- tiska variansen av fluktuationerna. Den genetiska variansen är viktig för att en population ska kunna reagera på förändringen i miljön på ett evolutionärt sätt. Känsligheten hos populationen på miljöförändringarna påverkades av fluktuationen och selektionen hade konsekvenser för skalbaggarnas potential att reagera plastiskt. Dessutom kunde jag hitta signifikanta skillnader mellan könen vad i hur de reagerar på miljön och föräldrarnas miljö hade också en effekt på individernas reaktion på förändringar.

Som slutsats vill jag betona att historisk selektion och särskilt fluktuatio- nen i omgivningsfaktorer är viktiga för hur en population reagerar på miljö- förändringar. Dessutom innehåller denna komplexa process mer än bara genetik och selektion, föräldrarna och skillnad mellan könen har en viktig betydelse. Därför är det nödvändigt att framtida studier (av både empiriskt och teoretiskt natur) som försöker förutsäga hur klimatförändringarna påver- kar populationer involverar dessa faktorer.

(31)

Zusammenfassung auf Deutsch

Klimawandel ist allgegenwärtig. Er ist unter anderem durch eine Erhöhung der Temperatur gekennzeichnet. Diese Temperaturerhöhung ist nicht kon- stant über die Zeit, sondern fluktuiert, und deshalb werden Veränderungen in der Umgebung am besten und realitätsnächsten anhand von Fluktuationen in der Umgebungstemperatur beschrieben.

Organismen werden von den Temperaturveränderungen in ihrer Umge- bung beeinflusst. Die neue Umgebungstemperatur ist nicht immer vorteilhaft und optimal für die Individuen – sie reagieren auf die Veränderungen. Es gibt unterschiedliche Arten der Reaktion, zum Bespiel weichen sie der neuen Temperatur aus und besiedeln andere Gebiete oder sie passen sich der neuen Temperatur an. Eine Anpassung kann plastisch oder auch adaptiv sein.

Während bei einer plastischen Veränderung der Genotyp zunächst einmal gleich bleibt, impliziert eine adaptive Anpassung eine genetische Veränderung.

Es gibt zahlreiche theoretische und mathematische Modelle, die ver- suchen Klimawandel und dessen Einfluss auf Populationen vorherzusagen.

Nur wenige dieser Modelle berücksichtigen jedoch die Tatsache, dass die Umgebungstemperatur fluktuiert. Ein erst kürzlich entwickeltes Model hat genau diesen Faktor einbezogen, und Simulationsresultate prognostizieren, dass Fluktuationen eine wichtige Rolle bei der Reaktion einer Population auf Veränderungen in der Umgebung spielen.

Das Ziel der Doktorarbeit ist, die Reaktion einer Population auf Klima- veränderungen, die durch Temperaturveränderungen gekennzeichnet sind, zu untersuchen. Der Schwerpunkt liegt hierbei auf der Erforschung der Rolle von Fluktuationen in der Umwelt.

Um diesem Ziel nachzukommen testete ich das genannte theoretische Model auf empirischer Ebene (d.h. mit lebenden Organismen im Labor) sowie im Rahmen der Quantitativen Genetik. Letztere befasst sich mit dem Phänotypen eines Individuums und versucht vor allem, diesen Phänotypen durch Umwelteinflüsse und genetische Faktoren zu erklären. In Experimen- ten der Quantitativen Genetik wird anhand von Phänotypen die Erblichkeit von Eigenschaften untersucht. Mit diesem Ansatz lassen sich zum Beispiel evolutionäre Prozesse untersuchen.

Im Rahmen der Doktorarbeit wurden eine Reihe von Experimenten im Labor durchgeführt. Als Studienorganismus diente der Bohnenkäfer, Callso- bruchus maculatus. Dieser eignet sich besonders gut für Laborexperimente

(32)

und die Erforschung der Evolution, da er anspruchslos ist und mit der Länge von einem Monat eine eher kurze Generationszeit aufweist. In den Experi- menten wurden beispielsweise die Merkmale Körpergrösse, Entwicklung- szeit und Fruchtbarkeit untersucht.

Drei der Experimente fokussierten die Erblichkeit von Merkmalen, elter- liche Effekte sowie Unterschiede zwischen den Geschlechtern. In einem großen Selektionsexperiment wurde das theoretische Model getestet. Hierzu wurden die Käfer simulierten Temperaturveränderungen ausgesetzt. Dies geschah über insgesamt 18 Generation, was einem Zeitraum von eineinhalb Jahren entspricht. Die Temperatur wurde in dieser Zeit auf unterschiedliche Art und Weise (Anstieg mit oder ohne Fluktuationen) verändert. Nach ab- geschlossener Selektion wurden die genanntem Merkmale gemessen.

In meiner Doktorarbeit kann ich das theoretische Model empirisch belegen. Fluktuationen in der Umgebungstemperatur sind wichtig für die Reaktion einer Population auf äußere Veränderungen. Zudem beeinflusste die Selektion in einer fluktuierenden Umgebung die genetische Varianz der Population. Diese ist wichtig, um auf weitere Umweltveränderungen zu reagieren. Außerdem änderte sich die Empfindlichkeit der Population in Reaktion auf Umwelteinfüsse und die Selektion trug Konsequenzen für das Potential der Käfer plastisch zu reagieren. Darüber hinaus konnten signifi- kante Unterschiede zwischen den Geschlechtern in der Reaktion auf Um- welteinflüsse festgestellt werden und auch elterliche Effekte hatten Einfluss auf die Reaktion der Individuen.

Als Fazit meiner Doktorarbeit möchte ich hervorheben, dass die his- torische Selektion und insbesondere Fluktuationen in der Umgebung eine wichtige Rolle spielen. Außerdem beinhaltet der komplexe Vorgang der Reaktion einer Population auf Umweltveränderungen mehr als nur die Erblichkeit von Merkmalen und die Selektion dieser; elterliche Effekte und eine Unterscheidung der Geschlechter sind ebenso von Bedeutung. Deshalb sollten zukünftige Studien, ob empirisch oder theoretisch, die den Einfluss von Klimawandel auf Populationen vorherzusagen versuchen, diese Fak- toren in Betracht ziehen.

(33)

Acknowledgements

I want to thank Mats B. my supervisor for his great optimism, always sup- porting my decisions and for his advice to take a break once in a while. Hol- ger S., who turned out to be my unofficial supervisor. Besides being such a good friend, I seriously don’t know what I would have done without your statistical support. Danke!

Russell Bonduriansky for welcoming me so warmly in your lab at the UNSW in Sydney. Thank you, for being such a great supervisor, I really enjoy working with you. Also thanks to the rest of the Bond-lab, especially to Ange & Margo. You made me feel home the day I arrived!

Niclas K. my second supervisor, who took me in as a project student, while I was still on Erasmus exchange in Uppsala in 2006. I think that was the turning point of me staying not only one, but five (or more?) years in Sweden.

Axel for being such a great friend and ‘Tratschtante’ and listening to all my guys stuff in many of the uncountable Fika breaks in the sun J.

Thanks to all the people in and around the EBC, who made it worth com- ing to work, even in the darkest times of the year. To Amber, what should I say…you are simply great and one of the most positive thinking persons I know! To my office girls, Kasia & Jossan and to the green sofa. This sofa combined with our daily laughter seemed to irresistibly attract people who were in great need of a break from starring at their computer screens….Paolo! who I started and will end my PhD-life with - thanks for the good times, Mirjam for being my sweet swedish friend ;-) Pia for drinks and girls nights, Karo, Lauren, Olivia, Alexei, Katja, Lisa, Phillip, Björn, Mårten, Rado (also for being such a good flat-mate), Hanna and Katrine (for help in the lab), Rike, Matt & Fernando for all the dancing. Simone, for or- ganizing the MCMCglmm course and encouraging me to participate. I think during this course I became to enjoy stats and…I still do. Then I of course know, that one should not have any deeper emotional feelings for the work.

However, I really need to say that paper III was: a pain, my little problem child, me trying to find the way through the jungle of way too many parame- ters…the list is long. But well, honestly: how should we expect something to respond in a straightforward way, when it is treated with randomness and noise?!?

I want to thank all my friends in and around Uppsala who gave my PhD- life the balance it needed. Thanks to all my climbing friends: Martin &

(34)

Maria, Axel and Alex with whom I had the greatest weekends/days out in nature, touching some rock, camping and simply enjoying life. Beke, for you being so down to earth, nature-loving, active and happy. Moritz for being such a good listener & climbing partner, Drew for an unforgettable summer on the Lofoten, Silja for brightening the long Swedish winters with climbing, sauna & good conversations and Tony, Max, Henry, Bernardo, Andreas, Shawn, Jarkko, Pablo, Andrea, Håkan, Veera & Christoffer for good times on various trips and climbing adventures. Melanie for brightening my life in Australia! Jesse, for an interesting experience. Thanks to my Flogsta- Corridor-Family: Wanda, for you being such a great girl & very good friend.

Dyl for being just Dyl. Jonno, Ben & Curtis for all the good times partying.

Ina, Tatí, Poppy, Kim, Emma, Anne and the finish girls for the nice times in the beginning of my life in Sweden.

Marlen, the best flatmate ever! - for being a good friend and tolerating all the books & articles that were covering floors and tables in our living room.

Rocio, for all the good conversations and Fikas we had. Judith & Steffi for being good friends and making my first two years of biology studies in Tübingen a memorable time of my life.

And to my gorgeous girls, my truly faithful friends, Kim, Britta & Jana, who always found and hopefully always will find the time to visit me – even if it means a trip to the other side of the world! :-* :-* :-*

Danke, to my parents for always standing behind me and my decisions and finally to my sister Inga, who I know is always there for me, and that is the most important thing there is.

(35)

References

Ancel, L. W. 2000. Undermining the Baldwin expediting effect: does phenotypic plasticity accelerate evolution? Theoretical Population Biology 58: 307-319.

Arnqvist, G, & Thornhill, R. 1998. Evolution of animal genitalia: patterns of pheno- typic and genotypic variation and condition dependence of genital and non- genital morphology in water strider (Heteroptera: Gerridae: Insecta). Genetical Research 71: 193-212.

Badyaev, A. V., & Uller, T. 2009. Parental effects in ecology and evolution: mecha- nisms, processes and implications. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London - Series B: Biological Sciences 364: 1169-1177.

Baldwin, J. M. 1896. A new factor in evolution. The american naturalist: 441–451.

Bandara, K., & Saxena, R. 1995. A techinique for handling and sexing Calloso- bruchus maculatus (F.) adults (Coleoptera: Bruchidae). Journal of Stored Products Research 31: 97-100.

Beebee, T. J. C. 1995. Amphibian breeding and climate. Nature 374: 219-220.

Behera, N., & Nanjundiah, V. 1995. An investigation into the role of phenotypic plasticity in evolution. Journal of Theoretical Biology 172: 225-234.

Berteaux, D. 2004. Keeping Pace with Fast Climate Change: Can Arctic Life Count on Evolution? Integrative and Comparative Biology 44: 140-151.

Björklund, M., Ranta, E., Kaitala, V., Bach, L. A., Lundberg, P., & Stenseth, N. C.

2009. Quantitative trait evolution and environmental change. PloS one 4:

e4521.

Bonduriansky, R. 2007a. The genetic architecture of sexual dimorphism: the poten- tial roles of genomic imprinting and condition dependence. In: D J Fairbairn, W U Blanckenhorn, & T. Szekely (Eds.), Sex Size And Gender Roles Evolu- tionary Studies of Sexual Size Dimorphism. Oxford University Press (pp. 176- 184).

Bonduriansky, R. 2007b. The evolution of condition-dependent sexual dimorphism.

The American naturalist 169: 9-19.

Bonduriansky, R., & Rowe, L. 2005. Intralocus sexual conflict and the genetic archi- tecture of sexually dimorphic traits in Prochyliza xanthostoma (Diptera: Pio- philidae). Evolution 59: 1965-1975.

Borenstein, E., Meilijson, I., & Ruppin, E. 2006. The effect of phenotypic plasticity on evolution in multipeaked fitness landscapes. Journal of Evolutionary Biol- ogy 19: 1555-1570.

Boyce, M. S., Haridas, C. V., & Lee, C. T. 2006. Demography in an increasingly variable world. Trends in Ecology & Evolution 21: 141-148.

Burger, R., & Lynch, M. 1995. Evolution and Extinction in a Changing Environ- ment: A Quantitative-Genetic Analysis. Evolution 49: 151-163.

(36)

Chevin, L.-M., Lande, Russell, & Mace, G. M. 2010. Adaptation, Plasticity, and Extinction in a Changing Environment: Towards a Predictive Theory (J. G.

Kingsolver, Ed.). PLoS Biology 8: e1000357.

Cox, P. M., Betts, R. A., Jones, C. D., Spall, S. A., & Totterdell, I. J. 2000. Accel- eration of global warming due to carbon-cycle feedbacks in a coupled climate model. Nature 408: 184-187.

Cuddington, K. M., & Yodzis, P. 1999. Black noise and population persistence.

Proceedings of the Royal Society B Biological Sciences 266: 969.

David, P., Bjorksten, T., Fowler, K., & Pomiankowski, A. 2000. Condition- dependent signalling of genetic variation in stalk-eyed flies. Nature 406: 186- 188.

Davis, M B, & Shaw, R G. 2001. Range Shifts and Adaptive Responses to climate change. Science 292: 673-679.

Davis, M. B., Shaw, R. G., & Etterson, J. R. 2005. Evolutionary responses to chang- ing climate. Ecology 86: 1704-1714.

Day, T., & Bonduriansky, R. 2011. A unified approach to the evolutionary conse- quences of genetic and nongenetic inheritance. The American naturalist 178:

E18-36.

De Jong, G. 2005. Evolution of phenotypic plasticity: patterns of plasticity and the emergence of ecotypes. New Phytologist 166: 101-17.

DeWitt, T. J., Sih, A., & Wilson, D. S. 1998. Costs and limits of phenotypic plastic- ity. Trends in Ecology & Evolution 13: 77-81.

Ellegren, H., & Parsch, J. 2007. The evolution of sex-biased genes and sex-biased gene expression. Nature Reviews Genetics 8: 689-698.

Fairbairn, D J, & Roff, D. A. 2006. The quantitative genetics of sexual dimorphism:

assessing the importance of sex-linkage. Heredity 97: 319-328.

Fairbairn, Daphne J. 2005. Allometry for sexual size dimorphism: testing two hy- potheses for Renschʼs rule in the water strider Aquarius remigis. The Ameri- can naturalist 166 : S69-S84.

Fairbairn, D. 2007. Introduction: the enigma of sexual size dimorphism . In D. Fair- bairn, W. Blanckenhorn, & T. Székely (Eds.), Sex, Size and Gender Roles:

Evolutionary Studies of Sexual Size Dimorphism. New York: Oxford Univer- sity Press (p. 1–10 ).

Falconer, D. S., & Mackay, T. F. C. 1996. Introduction to Quantitative Genetics.

Longman Harlow UK.

Fox, C. W. 1993. Multiple Mating, Lifetime Fecundity and Female Mortality of the Bruchid Beetle, Callosobruchus-Maculatus (Coleoptera, Bruchidae). Func- tional Ecology 7: 203-208.

Fricke, C. 2006. Postmating sexual selection and its role in population divergence in beetles. Acta Universitatis Upsaliensis, Digital Comprehensive Summaries of Uppsala Dissertations from the Faculty of Science and Technology 152.

Gienapp, P., Teplitsky, C., Alho, J. S., Mills, J. A., & Merilä, J. 2008. Climate change and evolution: disentangling environmental and genetic responses.

Molecular ecology 17: 167-78.

Grant, P. R., & Grant, B. R. 2002. Unpredictable evolution in a 30-year study of Darwinʼs finches. Science 296: 707-711.

Guntrip, J., Sibly, R M, & Holloway, G J. 1997. The effect of novel environment and sex on the additive genetic variation and covariation in and between

References

Related documents

Risken för köldvågor är konsekvent den typ av risk som svenska branscher är mest exponerade mot i sina leverantörskedjor (denna risk finns även i Sverige). Detta kan mycket

Det andra steget är att analysera om rapporteringen av miljörelaterade risker i leverantörskedjan skiljer sig åt mellan företag av olika storlek (omsättning och antal

46 Konkreta exempel skulle kunna vara främjandeinsatser för affärsänglar/affärsängelnätverk, skapa arenor där aktörer från utbuds- och efterfrågesidan kan mötas eller

In the latter case, these are firms that exhibit relatively low productivity before the acquisition, but where restructuring and organizational changes are assumed to lead

Inkubatorernas roll i det svenska innovationssystemet.” 80 En orsak till det oklara läget har varit bristen på tillgång till långsiktiga tidsserier där man kunnat jämföra de

The increasing availability of data and attention to services has increased the understanding of the contribution of services to innovation and productivity in

 Regarding the utilization of a finite element analysis simulation it was determined that the best correlation between tests methods for the specific analyzed scenario, is

Employees that had set aside time for reflection, e.g., by participating in reflection-sessions over their learning curve with the aim of better understand their behaviour