• No results found

exemplars of the same thing. Note, however, that this is a weak test of iconicity. No interpretation of novel pictures at the expense of other novel pictures is required.

And again, language is an integrated part of the task.

A follow-up study by Preissler and Bloom (2007) address both these issues. In a setup where 2-year-olds were shown two novel objects and two novel drawings, children generalised from drawing to object if the drawing had been named (e.g.

“this is a whisk, can you find another one?”), but from drawing to drawing if no label had been used (i.e. “can you find me another one of this?”). Depending on context the children thus attended to either the drawing or to the picture as a piece of paper. Thus, the dual nature of pictures, as well as interpretation of simple line drawings, can be grasped by 2-year-olds if the problem is properly framed (by lan-guage).

(200 lar d seem diffe

Figur used the “ of the Callag

In th and-draw Matc diffe and 2.5-y when when while infan In when choic no ro cat).

By verba matc olds avail ditio form when T succe othe the y prisin most tion actua guag are o In a comp

03) study. D difference i med to have

erent.

re 3. Picture in Callaghan acrylic colou e stimuli is n ghan (2000).

he second s -white pen wings were

ch and non erent verbal unfamiliar year-olds pe n they wer n labels we e older chi nts need to n a third st n presented

ce objects i ole at all. L

Performan y combinin al labels in ch and non than when lable. In the ons, where e med above c

n labels wer The conclus

eed, both v r. Among t younger ch ng conside tly naming

in the abse ally hinder ge, and inst older does t longitudin petence co

Difference i in apprecia e mediated

and replica n (2000). (No ur painting”

not included.) .

study only ncil persp

used as sa n-match ha

l labels, fa to the chi erformed a re not. 3-y ere unavaila

ildren matc label pictu tudy the 2 d with the in beforeha Labels used nce went up ng visual s n a fourth n-match we n choice ob

e latter case either choic chance. 2.5

re available ion was thu verbal and

these, verba hildren. Ca ering the p g games. Yo ence of ling rs children

ead drills th the iconic ro nal training

uld indeed

in style bet ated iconic a compara

stimuli ote that version ) From

black-pective

ample.

d very amiliar ildren.

above chanc year-olds w

able. The s ch by the ures and per .5 year old e picture sa and. Thus, d were belo p, even whe imilarity o study, it w ere dissimila bjects were e they faile ce objects w

-year-olds o e and choice us that you

perceptual, al informat allaghan (20 practices ad

oung childr guistic scaff from usin hem in repl oute catch

study Call d be improv

tween stimu city. Perspe able amoun

ce when lab were above

second stud visual corr rform on a d children w

amples, an in this con ow basic cat

en choice o of the choic was found t ar, the perf e perceptua ed miserably

were dissim on the othe e objects w ung children

, while old tion seemed

000) specu dults engage

ren are just folding. It m ng pictorial

lacing pictu up.

laghan and ved by play

ulus pictur ective draw nt of releva

bels were av chance in dy, togethe

espondence verbal level were equip nd had bee ndition the tegory leve bjects were ce objects that when formance w ally similar

y. However milar or labe

er hand fail were very dis n needed tw

er children d to be the ulated that

e children t not used t might be th l informati ures with ve

Rankin (2 ying pictur

res might n wing and a ant cues de

vailable but all conditi r with the e in the pi l.

pped with t en given th

pictures po el (e.g. tiger e from the s (e.g. butto labels coul was much b

r and there r, in the in els were ava led in all co ssimilar.

wo types of n could do

most impo this findin

and pictur to using pi hat picture ion indepe

erbal labels 002) showe re games w

not equal a acrylic pai spite being

t at chance ions, but w

first, show ictures, you the verbal l he labels o otentially p r cat vs. leo same catego n vs. coin) ld be used better for

3-e w3-er3-e no l ntermediate

ailable, they onditions e

f informatio with one o ortant facto ng is hardly res in, whi ictorial info book “read endently of

s. Not until ed that pic with young

simi-nting g very

e level worse w that unger labels of the played

opard ory.

) and , and

-year-labels e

con-y per-xcept on to or the or for y

sur-ich is

orma-ding”

f lan-l they ctorial

chil-dren (28 months old at start of study), where visual rather than verbal matching was the method. The pictures in these studies were simple pencil drawings. The control group, who got placebo training, lagged two months behind the experimental group in their comprehension of graphic symbols, and one month behind in their produc-tion. No children in the study had showed any comprehension at the onset of train-ing.

This dependence on naming, and thereby inattention to visual correspondence, may help explain why young children fail in scale-model tasks and when placing stickers on dolls in the absence of instruction.

Language interacts with the use of iconic information in more respects than as labels. It can for example structure a situation so that necessary relations gain sali-ence, and it can make iconicity redundant. As mentioned above, to use pictorial in-formation in model-search tasks one must be able to appreciate the relation between picture and referent in at least three different ways (DeLoache et al., 1998b). Firstly, that there exist a symbol-referent relation (e.g. that a map shows a country). Sec-ondly, how the symbol is related to the referent (e.g. that dots on the maps are cit-ies). Thirdly, one must be able to compute specific relations between symbol and ref-erent in order to use the symbol’s information about the real world (e.g. that specific dots refer to specific cities). By 3 years of age children can do this in a model-search task, but there are interesting interactions between the three levels. If children have been explicitly instructed that there exist a global model-room relation they can find a toy in a room where the furniture does not correspond to the scale model, but with less instruction it is crucial that the internal elements (i.e. the furniture) be-tween the two spaces correspond in order for the stand-for relation to be discovered and the toys be successfully retrieved (Marzolf et al., 1999). Again it seems that lan-guage relieves iconicity from its duties.

There are other means than instruction to heighten correspondence relations be-tween items. DeLoache et al. (2004a) found a robust transfer effect bebe-tween sym-bolic mediums (pictures to scale models), suggesting that training affects a general ability. However, instruction still plays a significant role in these experiments and instructions per se, not only experience with representations, also transfers between tasks (Peralta de Mendoza & Salsa, 2003).

Troseth (2003) attempted to train 2-year-olds to understand the connection be-tween video and reality in an object-retrieval task by showing hiding events simulta-neously live and on a video monitor, or by showing an adult model finding objects through video. The 2-year-olds did not become better at using video information on subsequent trials. When viewing the hiding events through a window, though, they could generally retrieve the objects.

I wonder how much faster it would have taken to teach the referential nature of photographs instead of line drawings in the Callaghan and Rankin (2002) study above. They only address one of the ways into pictorial competence. By using ab-stract drawings the scientists target the pictorial concept from the “referential side,”

while it is fully possible to first use pictures that the child already can decode, i.e.

highly realistic photographs, and then add reference onto that.

However, child researchers argue that iconicity plays a small part in acquiring the symbolic function of pictures. “One reason that iconicity should not be considered criterial in thinking about symbols is that even the most realistic color photograph expresses a point of view regarding its referent” (DeLoache, 2004). That is, there is a symbolic aspect in even highly iconic pictures. This is certainly true, but more from an outside perspective. For the child there is definitely an unambiguous “referent,”

which is the perceived object in the photograph, regardless of “expressed points of views.” Iconicity is of course central in this process and is necessary for learning the referential nature of pictures in terms of similarity and differentiation.

DeLoache et al. (2004a) for example brought differentiation into the picture when they made 2.5-year-olds train on photographs and subsequently perform bet-ter than 3-year-olds on scale-model tasks. This was a very different solution from putting the scale model behind a window to minimise its object properties. How-ever, it served a similar differentiating role. Reference was possible in the photograph condition just because it was sufficiently differentiated from the real world, but still had a striking likeness to it in virtue of being photographic. Would reference have turned up as effectively if drawings had been used instead?

Although I admit that realistic pictures are perhaps in minority in an infant’s up-bringing, and probably not the typical path towards reference, I would not exclude the possibility that iconicity can be a way into reference until it has been tested more thoroughly. Pictorial competence is after all an ability, or abilities, with a cultural foundation, and as such there can be many paths to the same end state. The typical route might not be the most effective one.