• No results found

The effects of artificial illumination on invertebrate drift

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "The effects of artificial illumination on invertebrate drift"

Copied!
12
0
0

Loading.... (view fulltext now)

Full text

(1)

The effects of artificial illumination

on invertebrate drift

Effekten av artificiellt ljus på evertebratdrift

Sandra Andersson

Faculty of Health, Science and Technology Biology

15 hp

Supervisor: Olle Calles Examiner: Larry Greenberg 2015-02-18

(2)

1

Abstract

For the past century, humans have drastically increased the use of artificial light all over the world. This is causing many problems for other organisms. Daytime feeders extend their activity into the night, which causes an increase in predation pressure on their prey. This study focused on macroinvertebrate drift and how it is affected by artificial light. A street light was placed at a Welsh river, and drift nets were staggered across the stream. The stream was then exposed to three different light treatments: (1) the lights were on all night, (2) the lights were off all night (3) the lights were on from 20.30 to 24.00 and then turned off. The results showed that species richness was lower in the net nearest the street light when the light was on for the first part of the night. This indicated a light sensitivity in some invertebrate species in the stream. Drift abundance was lower when the light was on throughout the whole night and when the light was on for the first part of the night than when the lights were never on. This difference was found in the net furthest away from the street light. Two possible explanations for this are: (1) the statistical significance was spurious, (2) There was a local difference in species composition. Some invertebrate species are especially vulnerable to predatory fish, and the difference in drift abundance for one of the nets could have been an indication of the presence of predatory fish in the stream. Further studies of invertebrate drift and artificial might benefit if the abundance of predatory fish is also estimated.

Abstrakt

Under det senaste århundradet har människan drastiskt ökat användandet av artificiellt ljus över hela jorden. Detta skapar problem hos många organismer. Organismer som äter dagtid är aktiva en bit in på natten vilket kan leda till ökat predationstryck på deras byten. Denna studie fokuserade på drift hos makroevertebrater och hur den påverkas av artificiellt ljus. Ett gatljus placerades i en flod i Wales och driftnät spreds ut i floden. Därefter utsattes denna del av floden för tre olika ljusbehandlingar: (1) ljuset var på hela natten, (2) ljuset var av hela natten, (3) ljuset var på från 20.30 till 24.00 och stängdes sedan av. Resultaten visade att artrikedomen var lägre i det nät närmast lampan när den var på under första delen av natten. Detta kan ha betytt att ljuskänsliga evertebrater var närvarande. Driftabundansen var lägre när ljuset var på antingen hela natten eller under första delen av natten i ett av näten. Skillnaden var i nätet längst bort från lampan. Det fanns två trovärdiga förklaringar till detta: (1) den statistiska signifikansen var falsk, (2) det fanns en lokal variation i artsammansättningen. Vissa evertebratarter är särskilt utsatta för rovdjursfisk. Denna skillnad driftabundans i ett av

(3)

2

näten kunde vara på grund av att rovdjursfisk var närvarande. I fortsatta studier där effekten av artificiellt ljus på evertebrater undersöks kan det vara av fördel att även uppskatta abundansen av rovdjursfisk.

Introduction

Human activities are causing many different changes to ecosystems world-wide (Vitousek, 1997). One of these activities is the use of artificial light, which has increased around the world since the beginning of the 20th century (Holden, 1992). This increased use of artificial light has changed nightscapes all over the world both in terms of light intensity and light spectrum. It has been shown that artificial light can have negative effects on human health and social well-being (Hölker et al, 2010a).However, when biodiversity conservation is discussed, light pollution has not been regarded as a major problem compared with for example, invasive species or microplastic pollution as light has been used to increase human safety (Hölker et. al., 2010b).

Living organisms are sensitive to light. A slight change in light level could, for example, alter the behaviour of an organism. Light induced responses can be found even in one-celled organisms (Gregory, 1978). Artificial light can therefore cause problems for many species.Organisms from all three kingdoms have a circadian clock that is affected by light (Dunlap, 1999). This means that artificial light could modifyan organism’s circadian clock so that theday-night cycle has, for example, longer days and shorter nights. Artificial light can cause predatory daytime feeders to extend their activity which might increase predation pressure on nocturnal prey species. (Hölker et al, 2010b). For example, predation rate on nocturnal planktivorous fish from diurnal piscivorous fish can increase under illumination (Hobson, 1965). Insects are drawn to light which makes them vulnerable to predators such as bats, frogs or toads (Royal Comission, 2009).Illuminated roads can cause disorientation among mammals since the rapid shift in illumination can affect their vision in a way that makes it hard for them to see dark areas (Rich &Longcore, 2006). Also, migratory birds tend to move towards light at night and has shown a reluctance to leave the illuminated area once it has entered it.

Many studies have been conducted on how different invertebrates are affected by artificial light. The most famous example is probably the study of moths and how their attraction to artificial night lights due to their flight-to-light behaviour has negative effects on their survival (Frank, 2006) It can interfere with mating, dispersal, and migration; it can disturb

(4)

3

feeding; it increases predation risk; it can also trap moths in buildings, divert them into vehicular traffic, and kill the moths instantly when they fly into lamp housings. A number of studies have described how artificial light affects terrestrial organisms, such as mammals and insects (Rich and Longcore, 2006). In contrast, the effects of artificial light on freshwater organisms has seldom been studied.

Freshwaters cover only about 0,8% of the total surface of the globe. However, 9.5% (126,000 species) of all species on earth are freshwater organisms (Dudgeon et al., 2006;Balian et al., 2008). This means that of the world’s total biodiversity, much of it lives in freshwater. These organisms could potentially be affected by artificial lighting since many freshwater systems are located in or near cities (Moore et. al., 2006), and hence may be affected by artificial light.A fish species that has been shown to be affected by artificial light is the European minnow (Phoxinusphoxinus). Its activity level is significantly lower at high light intensities than at low light intensities (Harden Jones 1956). Nocturnal fish species have proven to be especially sensitive to light where some species show negative phototaxis at light levels as low as 10-2 lux (Rich &Longcore, 2006).A study on concealment behaviour in juvenile rainbow trout (Oncorhynchusmykiss) showed that the fish did not come out of concealment until the light had dropped below a certain level (Contor& Griffith, 1995).

Artificial night lighting could cause diurnal fish species to feed at night as well. This could have a negative effect on nocturnal fish species since competition for food increases (Rich &Longcore, 2006).The foraging ability is also affected by light. For example, the capture rate for both European perch (Perca fluviatilis) and ruffe (Gymnocephalus cernua) has been shown to increase when light intensity increases (Bergman, 1988).Diel vertical migration of

Daphniais reduced by urban light pollution (Moore et al., 2000), making them more

vulnerable to predatory fish.

Even though many studies have been carried out on invertebrates and artificial night lighting, few have been done on the impact of illumination on macroinvertebrate drift. An experiment

on the nymph of the mayfly Baetisvagans and the

crustaceanGammaruspseudolimnauesshowed that artificial light had a strong negative effect on drift. Night-time drift in both species decreased to near daytime levels at light levels of 1 lux or higher (Holt & Waters, 1967). Knowledge of how macroinvertebrate drift is affected by artificial light is not only important from the invertebrates’ perspective, but also for predatory fish species. A study by Holt & Waters (1967) on invertebrate drift showed that drift abundance should decrease at increasing light levels.Macro invertebrates that tend to drift

(5)

4

during the day might also extend their activity due to the artificial light, creating a mix of daytime and night time drifters. Therefore species richness should be higher in the treatments with light.

The aims of this study are to investigate if artificial light at night affects the abundance and species richness of invertebrate drift. It is hypothesized that (1) the drift abundance will be lower under illumination, and (2) the species richness of drift will decrease under illumination. These hypotheses are tested experimentallyin a natural stream.

Material and methods

The experiments were conducted on the Llanmaes Brook at Llanmaes, Vale of GlamorganWales, UK(29°80’31”E, 16°94’44”N). The study site is located in a lowland rural area with predominantly arable and pastoral farmland. The area directly surrounding the stream is the small village of Llanmaes which has very little infrastructure and no street lighting influencing the stream. Directly adjacent to the stream is on one side a horse paddock and on the other side (fenced off) is a small children’s play park.A high-pressure sodium streetlight was placed on a steel tripod 1 m above the streambed in the river. To reduce the intensity of the light to realistic street light levels, neutral density filters were attached to the lamp. Three treatments were used in this experiment; 1) the light was off all night, acting as a control 2) the light was on from 20.30 GMT to 2.30 GMT (called full night illumination), corresponding with how the street lights are used in the area, and finally 3) the light was on from 20.30 GMT until (called partial night illumination) midnight. The three treatments were replicated three times and the order of presentation of the treatments was random.

Three experimental drift nets, referred to as nets B, C, and D,(500µm mesh size, EFE and GB Nets, Cornwall) were placed at increasing distances from the street light (see Figure 1) and one net, Net A, was placed 1 m upstream of the street light as a control net. Net B was placed 1 m downstream from the streetlight. Net C was placed 0.25 m downstream from Net B, and Net D was placed 0.25 m downstream from Net C.

(6)

5

Figure 1 – Experimental set up (not to scale). The control net was placed upstream from the light source. Nets B-D were placed downstream from the light source.

Since shifts in the timing of drifting behaviour can occur, the nets were emptied every two hours to make it possible to identify these shifts. This makes up a total of 45 different samples (5 samples per day for 9 days). Staggering the nets across the stream made it possible to empty one net without blocking the light for the other nets, and without disturbing the stream bed in front of the net. The light intensity was measured at the net entrance and 1 m upstream every time the nets were emptied (Table 1). This was done with a lux light meter (HI 97500, accuracy ±6%, ±2 digits, HANNA Instruments, USA).

Table 1 – Average light intensities across all nets, both immediately in front of the drift nets and 1 m upstream of the drift nets. The control values are the light intensities without artificial illumination.

Control (lux) Experimental (lux)

Net 1 m upstream In front 1 m upstream In front

A <1 <1 <1 <1

B <1 <1 170 13.5

C <1 <1 76.5 7

D <1 <1 54 2.5

The sampledmacroinvertebrates were preserved in 70% ethanol on site. They were later sorted into species groups using a dissecting microscope at 6x magnification. The drift samples from each night were quantified so that the total number of drifting individuals and the taxonomic composition could be compared.

The three experimental nets B, C, and D were compared separately to the control net A. The difference in drift abundance and species richness between each experimental net and the control net A was tested. Since the collected data were not of equal variance, the

(7)

non-6

parametric Kruskal-Wallis test was used for statistical analysis. Microsoft Excel 2013 was used for this.

Results

The total number of invertebrates collected in this study was 6677. The two most common invertebratesfound across all samples were Baetidae (36%) and Gammarus (25%)(Figure 2). Other invertebrate groups comprising over 5% of the total number of invertebrates were Helophoridae (11%), Chironomidae larva (8%), and Simuliidae (6%). The total number of species collected was 51.

Figure 2 – Percentage in terms of numbers of the most common invertebrates found in the current study (pooled for all drift samples)

The Kruskal-Wallis test showed that there wasno treatment effect for the difference between net A and net B, nor was there a difference for the difference between net A and C (Figure 3). For the difference between net A and net D, there was a treatment difference (Figure 3). The drift was much lower when the light was on, either through the entire night or for the first part of the night than when the lights were off for the entire night.

0 5 10 15 20 25 30 35 40

Baetidae Gammarus Helophoridae Chironomidae larva Simuliidae P erce n tage Invertebrate type

(8)

7

Figure 3 – The average difference in drift abundance between netA and net B (net net B), between net A and net C (net A-net C), and between A-net A and A-net D (A-net A-A-net D)for the three treatments. On = the light was on the entire night, Off = the light was off the entire night, Part = the light was on during the first part of the night. Only the difference between net A and net D was significant.

When it comes to species richness there was no treatment effect for the difference between net A and net C, nor was there a difference for the difference between net A and net D (Figure 4). There was, however, a treatment effect for the difference between net A and net B. Species richness was much lower in net B when the lights were on for the first part of the night compared to when the lights were off or on the entire night (Figure 4).

(9)

8

Figure 4 - The average difference in species richness between net A and net B (net net B), between net A and net C (net A-net C), and between A-net A and A-net D (A-net A-A-net D) for the three treatments. On = the light was on the entire night, Off = the light was off the entire night, Part = the light was on during the first part of the night. Only the difference between net A and net B was significant.

Discussion

According to the results, the species richness was lower in net B compared to net A when the light was on for the first part of the night. Net B was closest to the street light compared to net C and D, which means that the light level was highest by net B. The light level in front of net B was 13.5 lux whereas the light level was 7 lux in front of net C, and 2.5 lux in front of net D. This indicates there may be some invertebrate species present that are sensitive to light levels higher than 7 lux.

The results also showed that the drift in Net D was lower than the drift in the control net in the treatments with illumination for the whole night or part of the night. The fact that there was no difference between the control net A and nets B and C is difficult to explain since net D was placed furthest away from the street light. The light levels were therefore lower at net D than at nets B and C. Logically, the effect should have been smallest at net D. One possible explanation that is not very satisfying is that it is a chance effect, i.e. there is no real treatment effect, the statistical significance is spurious.

Assuming the difference is not just a coincidence, another explanation for the difference in drift in net D might be that there were local differences in invertebrate species composition in

(10)

9

that specific area. The invertebrates inhabiting that area of around net D might be more light-sensitive than the invertebrates inhabiting the areas around the other nets. However, considering that there was no difference in species richness in net D compared to net A, this seems unlikely.

Studies have shown that predatory fish have an increased predation intensity when larger sizes of prey are present. Predators such as trout use visual means to locate and catch their prey. Larger prey species are easier to see from a longer distance, and these are therefore favoured (Ware, 1972). When feeding on Baetis, brook trout prefer large prey over small prey when feeding during the day (Allan, 1978). This means large-sizeBaetisprobably at greater risk when drifting during the day compared to smaller Baetis. In his study, Allan found that invertebrates from the larger size classes drift more during the night compared to the smaller invertebrates. The strength of nocturnal drift behavior seems to be related to how vulnerable prey are to fish predation. This might explain why the drift was lower when the area around net D was illuminated. Perhaps larger invertebrates, such as Baetis, inhabitet the area around net D.

A correlation has been shown between diel changes in the abundance of drifting invertebrates and diel changes in the amount of food in the stomachs of rainbow trout (Salmo gairdneri) and brown trout (Salmo trutta;Elliot, 1973). The feeding occurred mostly in the early hours of the night. For future studies it might be beneficial to estimate the abundance of predatory fish species during the night in addition to measuring invertebrate drift. A study on drift behaviour in mayflies (Baetisvagans) showed that the presence of a predatory fish species changed the drift patterns of the mayfly (Flecker, 1992). In streams without a predator mayfly drift occurred both during the day and during the night. In streams where a predatory fish species was present, drift occurred almost exclusively at night. Considering the strong relationship between the presence of fish and invertebrate drift behaviour, it might be good in future studies to include estimates of fish abundance.

(11)

References

Allan, J.D. 1978. Trout Predation and the Size Composition of Stream Drift. LimnolOceanogr 23:1231±1237.

Balian, E.V., H. Segers, C. Lévèque, K. Martens. 2008. The Freshwater Animal Diversity Assessment: An Overview of the Results. Hydrobiologia595:627-637.

Bergman, E. 1988. Foraging Abilities and Niche Breadth of Two Percids: Percafluviatilis and Gymnocephaluscernua, under Different Environmental Conditions. Journal of Animal

Ecology 57: 443-453.

Contor, Graig R. & Jason S. Griffith. 1995. Nocturnal Emergence of Juvenile Rainbow Trout from Winter Concealment Relative to Light Intensity. Hydrobiologia299:179-183.

Dudgeon David, Angela H. Arthington, Mark O. Gessner, Zen-Ichiro Kawabata, Duncan J Knowler, Christian Lévèque… Caroline A. Sullivan. 2006. Freshwater Biodiversity: Importance, Threats, Status and Conservation Challenges. Biological Reviews 81:163-182. Dunlap, Jay C. 1999. Molecular Bases for Circadian Clocks. Cell 96: 271-290.

Elliot, J. Malcolm. 1973. The Food of Brown and Rainbow Trout (Salmotrutta and

andS.gairdneri) in Relation to the Abundance of Drifting Invertebrates in a Mountain Stream.

Oecologia 12(4): 329-347.

Flecker, Alexander S. 1992. Fish Predation and the Evolution of Invertebrate Drift Periodicity: Evidence from Neotropical Streams. Ecology73: 438-448.

Frank, Kenneth D. 2006. Effects of Artificial Night Light on Moths. Ecological

Consequences of Artificial Night Lighting (eds C. Rich & T. Longcore): 305-344. Washington

D.C., Island Press.

Gregory, Richard Langton. 1978. Eye and Brain:The Psychology of Seeing. Retrieved fromp. 2013-11-25.

Harden Jones, F.R. 1956. The Behaviour of Minnow in Relation to Light Intensity. Journal of

Experimental Biology 33: 271-281.

Hobson, Edmund S. 1965. Diurnal-Nocturnal Activity of Some Inshore Fishes in the Gulf of California. Copeia 1965: 291-302.

Holden, Alfred. 1992. Lighting the night: Technology, Urban Life and the Evolution of Street Lighting. Places 8(2): 56-59

Hölker, Franz, Timothy Moss, Barbara Griefahn, Werner Kloas, Christian C. Voight, Dietrich Henckel… KlementTockner. 2010a. The Dark Side of Light - a Transdisciplinary Research Agenda for Light Pollution Policy. Ecology and Society 15(4).

Hölker, Franz, Christian Wolter, Elizabeth K. Perkin &KlementTockner. 2010b. Light Pollution as a Biodiversity Threat. Trends in Ecology and Evolution 25: 681- 682

Holt, Charles S. & Thomas F. Waters. 1967. Effects of Light Intensity on the Drift of Stream Invertebrates. Ecology 48(2): 225-234.

Moore Marianne V., Stephanie M. Pierce, Hannah M. Walsh, Siri K. Kvalvik& Julie D Lim. 2000. Urban Light Pollution Alters the Diel Vertical Migration of Daphnia. Verh Internat

(12)

Moore, Marianne V., Susan J. Kohler &Melani S. Cheers. 2006. Artificial Light at Night in Freshwater Habitats and Its Potential Ecological Effects. Ecological Consequences of

Artificial Night Lighting (eds C. Rich & T. Longcore): 345–364. Washington D.C., Island

Press.

Rich, Catherine & Travis Longcore. 2006. Ecological Consequences of Artificial Night

Lighting. Island Press, Washington, D.C.

Royal Commission on Environmental Pollution. 2009. Artificial Light in the Environment. Royal Commission on Environmental Pollution. U.K, The Stationary Office Limited. Vitousek, Peter M., Harold A. Mooney, Jane Lubchenco, Jerry M. Melillo. 1997. Human Domination of Earth’s Ecosystems. Science 41:140-147.

Ware, D.M. 1972. Predation by Rainbow Trout (Salmogairdneri): The Influence of Hunger, Prey, Density and prey size. J. Fish. Res. Board Can., 29, 1193-201.

References

Related documents

The reefs also provided a new “clean slate” substrate for the settling of sessile organisms such as Urticina felina or partially sessile organisms (i. during a certain stage of

According to the central limit theorem we should expect a normal distribution with a small standard deviation when many segment pressures are summed up if ice crushing on

This result becomes even clearer in the post-treatment period, where we observe that the presence of both universities and research institutes was associated with sales growth

Däremot är denna studie endast begränsat till direkta effekter av reformen, det vill säga vi tittar exempelvis inte närmare på andra indirekta effekter för de individer som

The literature suggests that immigrants boost Sweden’s performance in international trade but that Sweden may lose out on some of the positive effects of immigration on

The MMC is operated with dq frame ac-side current control and circulating current control, which exhibits an accentuated phase rotation around fundamental frequency, and the

Scholars express a need to further understand how social mobility, perceived or de facto, affects political outcomes (Daenekindt, van der Waal &amp; de Koster 2017, Day &amp;

Since 1991 the reconstruction of Down town have been driven by Solidere a private construc- tion company with the ambitssion to rebuild and to bring back the life of the