• No results found

Triadic conflict mediation as socialization into perspective taking in Swedish preschools

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "Triadic conflict mediation as socialization into perspective taking in Swedish preschools"

Copied!
9
0
0

Loading.... (view fulltext now)

Full text

(1)

ContentslistsavailableatScienceDirect

Linguistics

and

Education

j ou rn a l h om ep a g e :w w w . e l s e v i e r . c o m / l o c a t e / l i n g e d

Triadic

conflict

mediation

as

socialization

into

perspective

taking

in

Swedish

preschools

Asta

Cekaite

LinköpingUniversity,58183Linköping,Sweden

a

r

t

i

c

l

e

i

n

f

o

Articlehistory: Received3October2018

Receivedinrevisedform24April2019 Accepted2August2019

Availableonline31October2019 Keywords: Conflictmediation Teacher-studentinteraction Perspectivetaking Languagesocialization Multipartyinteractions

a

b

s

t

r

a

c

t

Thispaperisavideo-ethnographiclanguagesocializationstudythatexaminesthediscursive,linguistic andembodiedfeaturesoftheteachers’andchildren’swaysofconflictmediationandresolution.Thestudy describesthewaysinwhichyoungchildren(threetofive-yearolds)inseveralpreschoolsinSweden arebeingsocializedintotheinteractionalcompetencesandperspectivetakingnecessaryformanaging conflictsituations.Itisshownthatteacher-guidedconflictresolutionwasaccomplishedthroughtriadic interactionsinvolvingtheteacherasamediator.Teachersusedquestionstoencourageanddirectchildren toconveytheirvolition,wants,andwishestotheirpeers,andrequestedtheotherchildrentolisten.Itis arguedthatsuchdiscursivepracticessocializechildrentoperspectivetaking,andthatthissocialization islinkedtowidersocietalnormsofSwedishpreschoolsandSwedishsociety,namelydemocraticvalues ofequality,agency,individualism,andsolidarity.

©2019TheAuthor.PublishedbyElsevierInc.ThisisanopenaccessarticleundertheCCBY-NC-ND license(http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/).

1. Introduction

Earlychildhoodeducationsettingsareorganizedaroundvarious

societallypoignantnormsandvaluesandtheyconstitute

signifi-cantandmultifacetedarenasforchildren’ssocialization.InSweden,

where preschools are attended by 95% of one to five-year-old

children,socializingchildrenintomorallyandemotionally

appro-priate,societallyvaluednormativeconductusuallyisconsidered

tobepartofteachers’institutionalresponsibilities.The

socializ-ingpotentialsofadult-childinteractionsinsituationsthatrevolve

aroundmundanemoralissues–especiallypeerconflicts–provide

afruitfulsiteforuncoveringotherwiserarelyarticulated

norma-tivesocio-culturalassumptionsofhowtoperformactions,express

emotionsandmaintainrelationships.Children’speerconflicts

pro-vide occasions for socialization into interactional competences,

comprising discursive ways of handlingconflict situations and

maintainingsocialharmony.Teachers’discursivepracticesarethus

contextdependent(Ochs,1996).Teacherscanexertovertorcovert

socialcontrol,providingchildrenwithpossibilitiesorlimitingtheir

space for negotiationor resistance. They can alsodemonstrate

attentiontochildren’sperspectives,ordisciplinethem(Ahn,2016;

Cekaite,2013;Demuth,2013).Perspectivetakingcanconstitute

animportantfeatureofconflictresolution,becausetheabilityto

understandothers’perspectivesisoneofthesignificant

charac-E-mailaddress:asta.cekaite@liu.se

teristicsofhumansocialityanddevelopment(Tomasello,2019).

Itisassociatedwiththesocialactor’sorientationtowardsothers;

itmakesitpossibletounderstandandattendtotheother’sneeds

andthereforeconstitutesoneofcornerstonesofmoral

accountabil-ity(Keane,2016).Notably,somediscursivepracticescancomprise

variousnormativedilemmas(e.g.,regarding alignmentof

adult-child, individualvs. collectiveperspectives,Goodwin&Cekaite,

2018).

Byadoptingalanguagesocializationperspectivewhichviews

languageuseas inextricablyrelated tothe indexingof cultural

values(Ochs,1996),thepresentstudyexaminesthelinguistic,

dis-cursiveandembodiedorganizationofteacher-mediatedchildren’s

peerconflictresolutioninpreschoolsinSweden.Itdiscusses

chil-dren’ssocializationbyexaminingwidelyusedteacherstrategies,

herecalled–triadicconflictmediation–thatencourageandguide

childrenbothtoarticulatetheirownandtotakeintoaccounttheir

opponents’perspectives,andhighlightsthesocialandmoralnorms

thatarebeingimplementedbythediscursivepractices.Thestudy

isbasedonavideo-ethnographyfromthreepreschoolsinSweden

(forchildrenfromthreetofive-yearsold)conductedduringaperiod

ofapproximatelyoneyear.Themethodsadoptedcombinea

micro-analyticapproachtoeverydaymultimodalinteractions(Goodwin,

2006)withethnographicfieldworkoflanguagesocialization

prac-tices(DeLeón,2012).

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.linged.2019.100753

(2)

1.1. Theoreticalperspective

Thepresentstudyisinformedbyalanguagesocialization

per-spective(e.g.,Schieffelin&Ochs,1986; seeintroduction tothis

specialissue).Fromthisperspective,socializationisaprocessof

assigningsituationalmeaningstoparticularlinguisticand

embod-iedactsandforms(i.e.ofindexingtemporalandspatialmeanings,

social identities, social acts as well as affective and epistemic

stances,Ochs,1996:410).Socialinteractionisviewedas

partic-ipationwithinsociallysituatedactivitiesandasaccomplishment

withindynamicallyevolvingcontextualconfigurationsthatentail

language,thematerialenvironment,facialexpressionsand

embod-ied action (Goodwin, 2018). In particular, affective and moral

stances reflect and instantiate culturalexpectations, social and

moralvalues.Inordertobeabletoparticipateinvarious

socio-culturalcommunitieseffectively,socialactorsneedtoproduceand

interpretbehaviorinawaythatalignswiththenorms,practices

andvaluesof thecommunity (Ochs,1996:129),and beableto

taketheperspectiveofothers.Accordingly,children’sacquisition

ofsocioculturalcompetence,languageandembodiedrepertoires

andmoralnormsareprocessesthatmutuallyinformoneanother.

Importantly,childrenareviewedasagentsinthesocialization

pro-cesswhoalignwith,resist,orignoreadults’actions.

Moral accountability is inextricably linked to social actors’

orientation to the perspectives and actions of others. Based

oninteractionalandethnomethodologicallyinspiredapproaches,

heremoralityisconceivedasamundane,“inherentandcommon

featureofeverydayencounters,situatedwithinthebasicstructure

ofdiscourse”(Bergmann,1998:283;Keane, 2016).Accordingly,

themoralorderisinvoked,negotiated,andevencontestedwhen

breachesinsocialpracticeoccur(Bergmann,1998;Goffman,1971):

thecategorizationofaneventasproblematicprovidesthegrounds

onwhichtoblameoraccountforsocialactionsthatdepartfrom

thenormativeorder.

1.2. Children’sconflictsineducationalsettingsanddiscursive

socializationstrategies

Interactionalresearchonchildren’speergroupsshowsthat

chil-drencanholdeachotheraccountableforbreachesofsocial and

moral order by, for instance, engaging in sophisticated

discur-sivepractices –she-said-he-said–narrativesthat articulateand

justifyvariousversionsandothers’perspectivesonproblematic

events(Goodwin,1990).Educationalsettingsalsoprovideasocial

spacewherepeergrouprelationsandfriendshipsarenegotiated

andestablished(Bateman,2015;Björk-Willén,2018;Evaldsson&

Melander,2016;HolmKvist,2018).Indivertibly,theyarerecurrent

sitesofpeerconflictsandnegotiationsofsocialorder;moreover,

theyaresiteswherechildren’ssocialrelationsareshapedinthe

peergroup,aswellasbyteachers(Burdelski,2015;Cekaite,2012b;

Danby&Theobald,2012).

Whenlookingthroughthelensofmundanemorality,thereisa

greatdealofvariabilityindiscursivesocializationpracticesamong

educationalsettingsindifferentcultures.Researchshowsthatthe

ascriptionandnegotiationofmoralagency–suchasintentionality

andaccountabilityforuntowardactions–canbeaccomplishedin

rathercontrastingways.Theteacherscanattendtotheinstitutional

orindividualchild’sperspectives,useprohibitionsorexertcovert

controlinordertoachievechildren’saccountabilityand

compli-ance.Forinstance,Lo(2004,2009)hasshownthatKoreanheritage

languageteachersdeployedpresumptivequestionsinmaking

stu-dentsaccountableandassigningthemculpabilityforwhatadults

viewedassocioculturallyinappropriateconduct.InaThai

class-room,children wereheldaccountablefor actinginsocially and

morallyappropriateways(e.g.,displayingrespect,byspeakingand

actinginanaffectivelylevelledmannertowardsadults,Howard,

2009).InastudyofAmericanmiddle-classpreschools,Ahn(2010)

examinedteachers’discursivestrategiesusedforconflict

resolu-tion.Ahn’sstudyshowsthatpreschoolteachersaimedtoinculcate

in the children so-called practices of ‘emotion metanarrativity’

(2010: 99) and to encourage children to articulate their inner

emotional perspective tothe others. The teachers taught them

to communicate emotionsby using verbalizations suchas ‘say

‘Ifeel sad’ (when you do this to me)’and configure

verbaliza-tionsasdiscursivemovestopreventandmakepeersaccountable

for theiruntoward acts.Thiskindof meta-leveltalk

conceptu-alizedemotionsasatransparentandspontaneousexpressionof

anindividual’sfeelingsandarticulationofheraffective

perspec-tive.However,assuggestedbyAhn,intheyoungchildren’speer

groupsocialinteractions,emotionmetanarrativitywasexploitedas

apowerfuldiscursivetoolinordertoadvancetheirsocialpositions,

toincludeorexcludemembersofthepeergroup.

Inastudyofa preschool,Burdelski(2013,this specialissue)

showsthatadultsmediatedinchildren’sconflictsandother

inter-actionsbyusinga‘triadicgloss’(e.g.,reportingonechild’sactions

orfeelingstoanotherandusingformulaicexpressionsinorderto

teachchildrenwhattosay).However,inanotherstudy,Japanese

preschoolteacherstookalessinterventionistapproachby

encour-agingchildrentoworkouttheconflictsontheirown(unlessthey

escalatedtoomuch)(Tobin,Wu,&Davidson,1991).Yetanother

wayofaddressingchildren’sdistressinconflictsisdocumentedin

Moore’s(2013)studyofaRussianpreschool.Here,adultcaregivers’

admonishmentsdemanded children to constraintheirnegative

emotions:teacherssocializedchildrentoacquireawarenessthat

theirnegativeemotionsarepubliclymonitoredanddisapproved

oftheirpeers(seealsoAhn,2016onKoreanteacher’sprompting

childrentomodifytheirconductbyshowingconcerntoteacher’s

negativefeelings).1 Itisnotonlyteachers,butalsocaregiversin

informalsettings,whosocializechildren’sawarenessofthe

oth-ers’perspectives.Clancy’s(1999)researchwithJapanesechildren

showsthatcaregiversformulateand‘quote’thefeelingsandneeds

ofotherstoencouragechildrentotaketheirperspective.Multiparty

familydiscourseisshowntoprovideaneedforperspective-taking

andunderstandingdifferentsocialactorswithvaryingauthority,

andknowledge(Blum-Kulka&Snow,2002).

Researchalsodemonstratesthattheadults’positioningasan

institutionalauthoritywithresponsibilitiestocareforandtoteach

children is inextricably related totheir responsibilities to

han-dle morally intricate and ambiguous situations. Adults can be

calleduptorespondto,ortomediatein children’sconflicts on

thebasisofpeergroupmembers’reports,accusations,ormorally

poignant negative affectivestances, such as crying.As

demon-strated by studiesonadults’ verbalresponses toreports about

children’snormativetransgressionsininformalfamily(Sterponi,

2014) and formal (Cekaite, 2012a, 2013) educational settings,

adults’responsesdiscursivelyaccomplishedamoralsenseofthe

events.Variouswaysofformulatingquestions(‘whathappened?’,

‘howcome?’vs.‘whydidyoudothat?’)aboutthechild’salleged

normative transgressionsascribed children various moral

posi-tionsorotherwiseevaluatedtheirconductvis-à-viseachother.The

adultspositionedchildren asculpableorgave them‘thebenefit

ofdoubt’;andprovidedorconstrainedtheinteractional

possibil-itiesforarticulatingtheirmoralpositionandperspective.Moral

order was instantiated through accounting practices employed

tonegotiate responsibility and(re-) categorize theproblematic

event(e.g.,Sterponi,2003).Asdemonstratedinastudyon

chil-dren’speerconflictsinaSwedishkindergartenclassroom(Cekaite,

1Analternativeapproachtochildren’sresistantactionsduringconflicts

con-ceptualizeschildren’sresistanceaswaysofdemocracylearninginpreschool,see

(3)

2012a),thereportedpastactionswerenot‘objectively’or

‘substan-tially’wrong,andtheirmoralvalenceascomplaints,accusations

andcounter-accusationswasascribedwithintheteacher-initiated

triadic framework for children’s perspectival tellings (see also

Burdelski,thisspecialissue;Moore,thisspecialissue).Inall,

pre-viousstudiesshowthatteachers’conflictmediationcansocialize

variousvaluesandnorms,andprovidechildrenwithsignificantly

differentparticipatoryrightsandlearningopportunities.

2. Method

2.1. Data,settingandparticipants

Thedatainvolveamulti-sitedvideo-ethnographyconductedin

threeregularSwedishpreschools forthreetofive-year-old

chil-drenduringaone-yearperiod.2InSweden,publicpreschoolsare

attendedbyapproximately95%ofchildren,andconstitutethemain

earlychildhoodsettingsfor“educare”:theyprovidechildcareand

educatechildren.Approximately15childrenandthreeeducators

fromeachpreschool(45childrenand10educatorsintotal)

partic-ipatedinthestudy.Twopreschoolswerelocatedinamiddleclass,

andoneinalow-socioeconomicarea.3

Video-recorded data (approximately 30hours in each

preschool)waslogged,andrecurrentinteractionaleventsnoted.

Peerconflictsin therecordingsfromthethree preschoolswere

identified(54intotal).Conflictsusuallyoccurredduringchildren’s

freeplay,whichconstituteda significantpartofdailyactivities.

The peer conflicts involving teacher-mediation were viewed

repeatedly,interactionallyanalyzedandtheircommondiscursive

featuresacrossthesitesdiscerned.Situationswheretheteachers

tookaprominentroleinconflictresolutionwereselectedfor

fur-theranalysis.Thecategoriesevolvedinductivelyandtheanalyses

werediscussedatlocalandinternationalseminars.Repeateddata

sessionswithinaresearchgroupcontributedtoidentificationof

recurrentsocioculturalfeaturesoftheencounters.

Theanalyticalmethodsadoptedcombineethnographic

field-work with a microanalytic approach to everyday interactions

(Goodwin,1990;Goodwin&Cekaite,2018),whichmeansthattalk

is studiedassocial action, produced bytheparticipantswithin

embodied participation frameworks (Goodwin, 2018; Goodwin

&Goodwin, 2004).Such an approach entailsdetailed attention

tointeractionalwork,exploredthroughtheparticipants’

sense-makingorientations,whicharedisplayedonaturn-by-turnbasis

inverbalandembodiedactions,embeddedwithinandindexicalof

socioculturalnormsandvalues.

Thepresentconflictmediationanalysisisbasedontheentire

collectionofconflictmediationepisodes(54cases,representing

three preschools). It revealedsimilar discursivepatterns that a

numberofteachersacrossthepreschoolsusedwhenresponding

tochildren’sconflicts.Themaincharacteristicsinvolve

socializa-tionstrategiesbasedonteachers’:i)invitingbothsides’tellingof

theevents;ii)usingvariousquestionformatstoguidethe

chil-dren’stellings,andiii)arrangingchildrenintotriadic“participation

frameworks”(Goodwin&Goodwin,2004).Theresultsreportedin

thepresentstudyexemplifythecorefeaturesofthefindings.In

2TheRegionalEthicalBoardhasapprovedtheproject.Writtenandoral

infor-mationwasprovidedtothestaffandparents,andthosewhowishedtoparticipate signedaconsentform.Participationwasvoluntaryandthestaffandparentswere informedoftheirrighttowithdrawfromthestudyatanytime.Whilevideo record-ing,theresearcherwassensitivetothereactionsofthechildrenandthestaff, andstoppedwhenthereweresignsofdiscomfort.Thenamesandimagesofthe participantsareanonymized.

3Thepresentstudyispartofaproject‘Communicatingemotions,embodying

morality’(PIA.Cekaite).FinancialsupportfromSwedishResearchCouncilis grate-fullyacknowledged.ThanksChildUnitparticipantsfortheircomments.

ordertobeabletodemonstratethetemporalcomplexityofconflict

resolutionpractices,anextendedtrajectoryofconflictandconflict

resolutionwasselectedforthepresentationinthisarticle.The

par-ticularcaseofconflictmediationoccurredinapreschoollocatedin

amultiethnicpreschoollocatedinalowsocioeconomicarea.

2.2. Children’sparticipatoryrightsinsocietyandeducational

institutionsinSweden

AstrongemphasisinSwedishsocietyatlarge,andintheearly

childhoodeducation,isplacedontheactiveparticipatoryrightsof

children.AnimportantgoaloftheSwedisheducationalsystemis

tofosteregalitarianvaluesandequalityinschoolsandsociety.For

instance,theSwedishNationalCurriculumforpreschool(policy

thatismandatoryforallpreschoolsinSweden)detailseducators’

responsibilitytofosterchildren’srespectforothersandtheir

per-spectives,assistthemindevelopingtheirabilitytounderstandtheir

rightsandobligationsandtakeresponsibilityfortheiractions.This

alsoincludesassistingchildrenindevelopingtheirabilitiesto

lis-ten,reflectonandunderstandtheother’sperspectivesaswellas

abilitiestoformpersonalstandpointsandexpressthemtoothers

(CurriculumforthePreschool,2018).Embeddedwithintheimplicit

andwidelyacceptednotionsofthequalitiesofgoodcitizenship,

thesetasksechothewider societaldemocraticideologyof

chil-dren’sparticipatoryrights (seealsoAronsson,2012,Goodwin&

Cekaite,2013onparent-childrelations)and“egalitarian

individ-ualism”in Sweden(Cekaite,2012b), accordingtowhich people

shouldbetreatedasequals,infundamentalworthorsocial

sta-tus.Simultaneously,thereisastrongemphasisontheindividuals’

possibilitiesforself-realization,andinfallibilityofone’swantsand

wishes.

3. Findings

In video-ethnographic data collected in three different

preschools in Sweden, a characteristicand prevalent discursive

organizationofteachers’andchildren’sdealingwithchildren’speer

groupconflictswasdiscerned.Thisdiscursiveorganizationishere

calledtriadicteachermediation,whereteacher/mediatorandtwo

or morechildren participated.It involves phaseswhere:i) one

childinformsabouttheviolation;ii)theteacherinitiatesthetwo

children’stellingsabouttheproblematicevent;iii)theteacher

sug-gestsaresolution,requestingthetwochildren’salignmentwith

andconfirmationoftheproposedsolution.Inthefollowing,this

triadicmediationanditssocializationpotentialsintoperspective

takingwillbedescribed.Thestudypaysparticularattentiontothe

embodiedfeaturesandspatialorganizationofthismediation(i.e.,

whathereiscalledthetriadic/multipartyparticipationframeworkof

moralaccountability).

3.1. Reportinganormativetransgressionandachievinga

preliminaryproblemdefinition

The child’s initialinforming about a problematic event was

accomplishedasareportoftheinfractiontotheteacher,an

insti-tutionalauthoritywhocansetthingsright.Thereportarticulated

thechild’sperspective;itwasdesignedinvariousways,e.g.asa

verbaltellingabouttheuntowardact,apublicdisplayofanegative

affectivestance(i.e.,crying),causedbythenormativeinfraction.In

Extract1a-b,theconflictarisesduringchildren’sfreeplayandis

initiallyindicatedbyachild’sloudcrying.Thechildrenareplaying

anddancinginthe‘pillowroom’withoutteacherspresent.When

JohnieclimbsontothewindowsillandMiranpullshimbacksohe

fallsdownontothesofa,Johnieimmediatelystartscrying.

Exerpt1aParticipants:teacher,boysJohnie(3.5y.),Babir(4y.),

(4)

01. Johnie: U:HU:HUHUHU((crying)) 02. Babir: ‘u:hu:huhuhu’((mockcrying)) 03. Johnie: U:HU:HU

04. Teacher: ((enterstheroom)) 05. Johnie: ((approachesteacher))

06. Teacher: ((stopsmusic,sitsclosetoJohnie))

07. Johnie: MIRANPUTTARSÅMEDMINTRÖJA:((Fig.1)) MIRANPULLSMYSWEATSH:IRT

LIKETHIS((sobbing)) 08. Teacher: Jaha:komdå!Kom!

Ohye:scomehere!Come!((touchesJohnie’s shoulder,bringshimcloser,hugshim, putshimonherlap))

09. Johnie: UHUHU

10. Teacher: Jagskatröstadiglite. Iwillcomfortyoualittle.

11. Johnie: HANDROGMINTRÖJASÅDÄ:R!((Fig.2)) HEPULLEDMYSWEATSHIRTLIKE THI:S!((demonstratespullingtheT-shirt)) 12. Teacher: [Jaha:

Aha:

13. [((Miran,Felis,Babir,Milasitquietly)) 14. Johnie: uhuhhu

15. Teacher: Gillarintedudet? Don’tyoulikethat? 16. Johnie: ((shakeshishead‘no’)) 17. Teacher: Nä,iblandkanjagocksåtyckaatt 18. jagintevillattnåndrarilinnensåhär.

No,sometimesIalsothinkthatIdon’t wantanybodypullingmyshirtlikethis. ((lookingatBabir))

19. Johnie: uhhuhhu

20. Teacher: Så.Dåkanmansäga’nejdetgörjaginte’. OK.Thenyoucansay’noIdon’t(wantto)’. 21. Johnie: Detgörjaginte:.

Ido:n’t(wantto).((whiningvoice)) 22. Johnie: Jabra::.

Yesgoo::d.

Johnie’sloudcryingcallstheteacher’sattentionandinvokes

herinstitutional responsibilitiestoattendtothepotential

prob-lemandsheenterstheplayroom(lines1-4).WhenJohniereports

anuntowardeventwithanaccusatorystatement‘Miranpullsmy

sweatshirtlikethis’,theteacheraffirmsJohnie’stelling‘Ohyes’and

physicallycomfortshim(lines7-8).Sheputshimintoherlapand

embraceshim(e.g.,onaffectionatetouchinpreschoolsseeCekaite

&Bergnéhr,2018)arrangingarelationallysignificantbodily

for-mation.BygesturingtoJohnietocomeandthenscoopinghimup

intoherlap,theteacherpositionsthetwochildren(assumedto

beinvolvedintheproblematicevent)tositclosetoeachother.In

thisway,theteacherestablishesanembodiedmultiparty

frame-workofparticipationinaremedialinterchange(Goffman,1971).

Johnieembellisheshisaccusationbyshowingwhathashappened:

hedemonstrativelypullshist-shirtandoverlayshiscomplaintwith

anaffectivestance,positioninghimselfasavictimofMiran’s

unto-wardconduct(line11).

Theteacher’scomfortingactionsarepublic–visibleandaudible

–fortheentirechildren’sgroup(lines8,10;15).Johnie’saffectively

chargedaccusationsaresuccessfulinmediatinghisindividual

neg-ativeexperience: theteachernot onlycomforts Johniewithan

embracebutalsousesaquestion‘Don’tyoulikethat?’to

expli-cateandmakepublichisnegativestancetowardsMiran’sphysical

actionsofpulling(line15).

Johnieconfirmstheteacher’sinterpretationbyshakinghishead

(signalingthathedoesnotlikethat,line16)andtheteachermakes

publichisnegativeexperience,linkingitcausallytothepeer’s

phys-icalaction.HergazeatBabir(whomsheostensiblyassumestobe

theculpablechild,despiteJohn’s accusationof Miran)indicates

thattheaffirmationofJohnie’snegativefeelingsandher

comfort-ingisnotonlydirectedatthecryingchild,butalsoatthealleged

perpetrator(lines17-18).Oneofthewaysthatthechildrencan

gainknowledgeabouthowtointerpretandnarrativelyconstruct

theirmoralaccountabilityandemotionsisthroughhearingadult

andotherpeers’narratives(Bruner,1990;Cekaite,2013).Here,the

teacher’spersonalexample(Griswold,2010)‘nosometimesIalso

thinkthatIdon’twantanybodypullingmyshirtlikethis’

verbal-izesanaligningsensorialandaffectiveexperience,anindividual

evaluativeperspective,andsupportsthecryingchild’semotional

stance.Theteacher’ssuggestionandprompttoJohnietotellMiran

thathedoesnotwanthimtopullhisshirt(‘thenyoucansay‘noI

don’t(wantto)’,line20)instructsandmodelsverbalwaysof

pre-emptingtheconflictualsituationandnegativeemotionsthatneed

training(theteacherusesanauxiliaryverb‘gör’‘do’toretroactively

refertothevolitionverb‘vill’inherpreviousutterance,line15-18).

Theteacherpromptsthechild(usingelicitedimitation)toconvey

hisexperience(negativewantsandpreferences)(lines20-22).

Theteacher’ssolicitationofthecryingchild’stellingisa

sig-nificantandrecurrentsocializingstrategyintheSwedishcultural

context (see also Cekaite, 2013on similar accountability

prac-tices in a kindergartenclassroom) thatprovidesthe child with

opportunitiestoarticulatehis/herownperspective.Importantly,

theratificationofthecryingchild’snegativeexperiencedoesnot

resultintheteacher’soutrightdiscipliningoftheimplicatedchild

orageneralstatementofrules.Rather,theteacherusesdiscursive

strategiesthatsolicittheotherparticipant’stellingand

perspec-tive,aswillbedemonstratedinEx.1b.Suchstrategycanbeseenas

aneverydaydiscursiveimplementationofademocraticand

egal-itariansociety,where children areencouragedand expectedto

verbalizetheirviews.

3.2. Solicitingandlisteningtochildren’salternativeversionsof

events:enactingandsocializingtherighttospeakandbeheard

For theSwedishpreschool teachers,gettingonly onechild’s

(the‘victim’s’)perspectiveontheuntowardeventisnotenough

to achieve a solution and to finish a remedial interchange

(Goffman,1971).Theteachers’conflictresolutioninvolved

medi-atingbetweenthechildrenbyassigningequalspeakershiprights

both tothe‘victim’ and thealleged perpetrator intelling their

versionoftheconflictualevent.Usually,theteachersposed

open-endedquestionsaboutwhathadhappenedandinvitedatelling

(5)

(Goodwin&Goodwin,2004).Thisdiscursivepracticewas

multi-dimensional:Theteachersengagedthechildrenintheimmediate

conflictresolutiononthebasisofthealternativeversionsofthe

problematic event. By soliciting and scaffolding children’s

per-spective taking, teachers socialized them into societally valued

democraticdiscourseandprovidedchildrenwithopportunitiesfor

egalitarianparticipation.

InEx.1b(animmediatecontinuationof1a),theteacher

contin-uestheconflictmediationbyaskingtheallegedperpetratorBabir

‘whathappened?’;however,sheself-correctsandinvitesan

open-endedtellingwithaquestion‘doyoualsowanttotell’(lines23-24).

Theteacher’schoiceoftheverb‘berätta’‘tell’,volitionmodal‘vill’

‘want’andtheinterrogativesyntacticformatindexthatheisinvited

totellhisversionandthatheisofferedanextendedinteractional

floor.With a conjunction ‘också’ ‘too’theteacher makesa link

betweenthissolicitationofastoryandtheprevious(thealleged

victim’s)one.

Excerpt1b

23. Teacher: Vadhände-villduocksåberättaBabirdå? 24. Villduocksåberätta?

Whathappe-doyoualsowanttotell,Babir? Doyoualsowanttotell?

25. Babir: ()kandet-först(.)förstgjordejagmedMiran. ()canit-first(.)firstIdidwithMiran. 26. ((MiranrepeatedlytriestoengageBabirinplay.

TeachertriestogetMiran’sattention)) 27. Teacher: NejmenMirannunuavbrötduBabir 28. närhanskulleberätta.

29. Nufårdulyssnaistället.

NobutMirannowyouinterruptedBabir whenhewasgoingtotell.

Nowyouhavetolisteninstead. 30. Babir: FörstförstgårvidärtillSteve.

Atfirst,atfirstwegotoSteve. ((continuesstoryaboutboys’dance))

31. Teacher: Ochsenkomniinhär.((Fig.3)) 32. OchsentogduiJohnieströja.

Andthenyoucamehere.

ThenyougrabbedJohnie’ssweatshirt. 33. Babir: Nejinteja:g.Miran.

Nonotme:.Miran.

34. Teacher: VardetMiransomdrogitröjan? WasitMiranwhopulledtheshirt? ((toJohnie))

35. Johnie: ((nods))

36. Teacher: Hmhm.((lookingatMiran)) 37. Teacher: Detkan-detkanjuvarasåattmaninte 38. Tyckeromattnåndrariklädernaattdet 39. Kanjagkännanåndrariminakläderna.

40. Miran?FörstårduvadJohniemenade?

41. Hanvillinteattduskadraitröjan. Itcan-itcanbelikethisthatonedoesnot likesomeonepullingone’sclothesand Ifeelthatwhensomeonepullsmyclothes.

Miran?DoyouunderstandwhatJohnie meant?Hedoesnotwantyoutopullhisshirt. ((demonstratespulling))

42. Miran: ((looksdown)) 43. Teacher: Tänkerdupådetdå?

So,areyouthinkingaboutthis? 44. Miran: ((nodsslightly))

WhenBabirtellsabouttheboys’dancingbutisinterrupted

sev-eraltimesbyMiranwhotriestodiverthimintoplay,theteacher

reprimandshimbytellinghimthatheshouldlistentoBabirinstead

(lines27-29).Theteacher’sdisciplininghereshowsthatindividuals

notonlyhavetherighttospeakbuttheyalsohavetolistentothe

otherpartiesandengageinmutualperspectivetaking.Babir

pro-ceedswithhistelling,buthisstorydoesnotmentionthealleged

untowardactofhimpullingJohnie.Inresponse,theteacheraddsa

temporalnarrativelinktotheboy’suntowardact‘Thenyougrabbed

Johnie’ssweatshirt’(lines31-32).

The teacher’s solicitation of the children’s tellings is clearly

directedatthechildrenaskeyparticipantsintheconflict–the

‘victim’andthealleged‘perpetrator’–whomtheteacherinvites

toverballyarticulatetheirperspectivesontheevent.However,in

thiscase,theteachererroneouslyassumesBabir’sculpabilityand

histellingdoesnotproduceanalternativeperspective.Uponthe

clarificationofmisunderstanding(lines34-35),theteacherengages

Miran in a conflictresolution. Sheunpacks and articulates the

victim’sperspectiveandensuresthe‘perpetrator’s’alignmentby

solicitinghisactivedisplayofunderstandingtheotherchild’s

per-spective’Miran?DoyouunderstandwhatJohniemeant?Hedoes

notwantyoutopullhisshirt’(lines37-41).Thesolutiondealswith

theindividualchild’spreferences,andtheteacherusesadiscursive

“activitycontract”(“spokenagreementsaboutfuturecompliance

that makechildren morally accountablefortheirfuture actions

(andforfailedaction)”,documentedinparent-childinteractions

in Sweden,seeAronsson&Cekaite,2011:139). Byformulating

anactivitycontract,theteacherscaffoldstheperpetrator’s

under-standingofthenormativeappropriatenessofone’sactionsfrom

theindividualperspective–likesanddislikes–oftheotherchild

andestablishesthechild’spromiseofmoralaccountability.

Thelackofthetransgressor’sknowledgeaboutthepeer’s

indi-vidual preferences is considered by the teacherto be causally

relatedtothechild’smisconduct.Conflictresolutionistemporally

bi-directional:itdealswithretrospectivenormativeevaluationof

thepasteventsandinvolvesprospectiveorientationtothe

distri-butionofknowledgebetweenthepeergroupmembers.Notonlyis

MiranrepeatedlyinformedaboutJohnie’sindividualperspective,

buthealsoneedstopubliclydisplayandconfirmhiscurrent

under-standingandhisabilitytoactaccordinglyinthefutureencounters

(lines43-44).Inrelationtolanguagesocializationintoperspective

taking,thediscursivestrategiesusedbytheteacherdemonstrate

thatchildren’smoralaccountabilityisinvokednotthrough

child-directedreproachesanddiscipliningbutbyimplicitmeans;thatis,

byseekingthechild’salignmentandindividualunderstandingof

whatconstitutesproblematicconduct.

3.3. Solicitingthechildren’stellingofanindividualperspective

withinatriadicparticipationframeworkofmoralaccountability

Asdemonstrated,theteachersolicitedmultipleperspectiveson

theproblematicevent.Thisdiscursiveactivitywascharacterized

byatriadicparticipationframeworkandconsecutiveallocationof

speakershiprights.Inanembodiedway,theteacherenactedthe

roleofamediatorbypositioningtheinvolvedchildren withina

triadicparticipationframeworkintheproximityofeachotherina

face-to-faceformation(Kendon,1990).Thisbody-spatial

arrange-mentwasaninextricablepartofteacherconflictmediationandcan

beseenasanembodiedframeworkofmoralaccountability.

Animportantpartofsocializingthechildrenintoperspective

takinginvolved modelingthechildren’sinteractionaland social

(6)

todiscernandverballyexpresstheirvolition–whattheydoanddo

notwant.Theteacherrepeatedlymodelledthechildren’ssocialacts

forexpressing,assertingandlisteningtoeachother’sperspectives.

These perspectivesrepresented individual likes, dislikes, wants

anddesires.Theteacher’srepeateddeploymentofthemodal‘vill’

(denotingvolition)andotherlinguisticresourcesservedas

socializ-ingdiscursivetools,indexicalofthesocietalviewsthatforeground

andvalueindividual’sintegrity.

InEx.2a,thechildren’s(JohnieandMiran’s) conflictevolves

againduringtheircontinuationof play-running-dancing.Johnie

gets pinched byMiran and he goesto anotherroom toreport

Miran’suntowardacttotheteacher.Theteacherstopswhatshe

isdoingandaccompanieshimtotheplayroom,wheresheinitially

reproachesMiranbyimplicitlyinvokingtheactivitycontract.She

referstotheirprioragreement‘whatdidwesay?’.Grammatical

andprosodicfeatures(pasttenseandemphasisontheverb‘say’)

marksherquestionasareproach.

Exerpt2a

01. Teacher: Nufårvistängaavdet.

Wehavetoturnitoff(cdplayer)now. 02. Teacher: Miran.Vadsavi?

Miran.Whatdidwesay?

03. (1.0)

04. Teacher: Detärnåntingsomduintevill.((Fig.4)) Thereissomethingthatyoudon’twant.

((toJohnie))

05. Johnie: ((affirmativelyshakeshishead)) 06. Teacher: DåfårdusägatillMiran.

ThenyouhavetotellMiran. 07. Johnie: Jagsa’SLUTA’

Isaid’STOP’((enactsgesture)) 08. Teacher: Hurvillduattdetskavanärdudansar?

Howdoyouwantittobewhenyoudance? 09. Johnie: ((startsjumping-runningaround,

showinghowhedances)) 10. Teacher: Ne-Johnie.

No-Johnie.

11. Teacher: Berättahurdetskavaranärduskadansa? Tellushowitshouldbewhenyou aredancing?

12. Johnie: Sådä:rvilljagdansa.((Fig.5))

Iwanttodancelikethi:s.((runsinacircle)) 13. Teacher: Ok.Ochse:n.

OK.Andthe:n.

14. ((MiranandStevestarttoteasinglylaugh atJohnie’sdance.Teacheradmonishes themmildly))

Theteachersitsinbetweenthetwoboysattheireyeleveland

isafocusoftheirattention.Theteacher’sbodyposture,together

withhergestureandfacialexpression,indicates‘active’and

‘seri-ous’listening,takinginthechild’sperspective.Thisspatial-bodily

positioningnotonlyconfiguresacommonfocusofattentionfor

thechildren but also positions theminto thespatial and aural

proximityofeachother(seealsosimilarparticipationframeworks

arrangedbytheteacherinEx.1a-b).Theteacher,throughherbodily

posture,displaysattentivelisteningofJohnie’stalk,andher

pre-parednesstoattendtoMiran,theallegedlyguiltychild’sviews.

Theembodiedtriadicparticipationframeworkprovidesconditions

for a multipartyexplication ofvarious perspectives in thatthe

teacherpositionsthetwochildrenasspeakersandlisteners.Inthis

embodiedframeworkofmoralaccountability,theteacherguides

thechildren’stalkasarticulationoftheperspectiveofonechildto

theother(Ex.2b).Notableistheteacher’suseofvariouslinguistic

resourceswithinthesemanticfieldofvolitionofindividualchildren

whoaresolicitedtopresentandtolistentoeachother,ratherthan

totheteacher’sinstitutionalauthority.Theteacheruseslinguistic

resourcesthatindexandemphasizetheindividualchild’srightto

self-determination,andhonorsindividualpreferencesandchoices

(‘howdoyouwantittobewhenyouaredancing?’;‘thereis

some-thingthatyoudonotwant’,seealsoEx.1a.,‘hedoesnotwantyou

topullhisshirt’).Socializationintoindividualperspectivesthereby

constitutesapartoftheteacher’sdiscursivepracticesastheadult

accentuateschildren’sagency,theirrighttomakechoicesabout

whattheywant,ordonotwant,andinvokesthepeers’

responsi-bilitiestorespecteachother’schoicesandperspectives.

3.4. Scaffoldingchildren’stellingandparticipatoryrights

ThepresentdatacorpusfromthreeSwedishpreschoolsshows

thattheteacherscreatedanextendedinteractionalspaceforthe

children’s articulation of their story and allocated the children

speakershiprightstomakepublictheirperspectives.However,it

isalsoapparentthatthechildren’stellingsinconflictmediation

werenotentirelyfreebutwerediscursivelyguidedbytheadults.

Teacherscaffoldingandguidingofthechildren’sparticipationto

correspondwiththeinstitutionallyadvocatedmoralperspective

ofwhatisrightandwrongbecameespeciallyvisiblewhen

chil-dren,forinstance,haddisplayedalackofinterest,understanding,

orreluctance,orhadproducedonlyminimalortopicallyirrelevant

verbalcontributions.

AsdemonstratedinEx.2b,oneofthechildreninvolvedinthe

conflictispositionedasthe‘principal’(theonewhoseviewsare

rep-resented)andtheotherchildasthe‘recipient’(theonetowhoman

utteranceisaddressed)(Goffman,1981).Innumerouscaseswhen

thechildrendidnotexpandontheteacher’squestions,theteacher

activelyguidedthecontentoftheirresponsesandcollaboratively

constructedarepresentationoftheindividual’sperspective.Here,

theteacherdirectsseveralquestionstoJohnieabouthis

experi-ences,butwhenshedoesnotreceiveappropriateresponsesshe

startsguidinghistalk.

Exerpt2b

15. Teacher: KännerduattMiranjagardigdå

16. ellerhur?

YoufeelthatMiranischasingyou, don’tyou?((toJohnie))

(7)

17. Johnie: (Fig.6)((dances,triestostandonhishead)) 18. Teacher: Johnie.(0.6)Johnie!

19. Johnie: Ja.

Yes.((slowlygetsupfromthefloor)) 20. Teacher: VillduinteattMiranskajagadigellerhur?

Youdon’twantMirantochaseyou, doyou?((looksatMiran)) 21. Johnie: Nej.

No.

22. Teacher: Hurdå?Hurdåmenardu? How?Howdoyoumean? 23. Teacher: SpringerduefterJohniedå?

AreyouchasingJohnie?((toMiran)) 24. Miran: Nej!

No!

25. Teacher: Nej?Hurmenardudå?

No?Howdoyoumean?((toJohnie)) 26. Miran: Nej.

No.

27. Teacher: Nej.Jagförstårintehurdumenarriktigt. No.Idon’tactuallyunderstandwhat youmean.((toJohnie))

28. Johnie: ((looksatteacherbutdoesnotrespond)) 29. Teacher: Vipratadeförrutattduinteska 30. jagaJohnie.

Wetalkedbefore,thatyoushouldnot

chaseJohnie.((toMiran)) 31. Miran: Va?

What?

32. Teacher: Mensågjordedudetändå. Butthenyoudiditanyway. 33. Johnie: ((nodsdemonstratively)) 34. Teacher: Mh.VetduhurJohniemenardå? 35. VetduhurJohniemenar?

Mh.DoyouknowwhatJohniemeans then?DoyouknowwhatJohniemeans?

((pointsasMiranwhohasdisengaged fromconversation))

36. Miran: Nej. No.

38. Teacher: Dåfårduberättalite.

Thenyouneedtotellhimalittle. ((toJohnie))

39. Johnie: ((talksaboutMiran’sSpiderman costume,runsarounddancing)) 40. Teacher: A.Mendåtänkerdulitegrann.

Yeah.Butyouhavetothink(aboutthis) abit.((touchesMiran))

41. Johnie: Annadetärdanstilldig.

Anna(teacher)thisismydanceforyou. ((runsaround))

TheteacherguidesJohnie’stalkbyaskinghimquestionsand

directinghisresponsestoMiran.SherepeatedlyusesYes/No

ques-tions,formulatingaversionofhisexperiencesandstancestowards

Miran’sactions:‘YoufeelthatMiranischasingyou,don’tyou?’,’You

don’twantMiran tochase you,do you?’ (lines15-16,20).The

teacherrepresentsJohnie’sperspective toboth participants:by

publiclyandcollaborativelyformulatingJohnie’sperspective,she

constructsMiran’schasingactasproblematicandcausallylinksit

toJohnie’snegativeexperience.Thistriadictellingdoesnotinvolve

directquotesandtheteacherenactingthechild’stalk(cf.triadic

glossing,Burdelski,2015).Rather,itscharacteristicformat

com-prisescollaborativelyconstructedutterancesthat representand

confirmthechildren’sperspectivesandseekthechild’s(Johnie’s)

alignment with the teacher’s formulation (lines 15, 20-22, 27,

34-35).Theteacher’sdiscursiveguidancegivesJohniethe

oppor-tunity tocorrect the teacher’sinterpretation of hisperspective

(hiswishes)incasetheteacher’sglossdoesnotreflecthisviews:

Johnieaffirmstheteacher’stalk,agreeingthatchasingisnot

some-thinghewantsorlikesandtheteacher’sversionofthedispute

iscollaborativelyestablishedasa‘correct’versionoftheevents.

However,whentheteacheraddressesMiranwithaquestion‘Are

youchasingJohnie?’(line23),herepeatedlyrefusestoagreewith

this morally poignant formulation and conflicting versions are

producedbyJohnnieandMiranaboutthechasing.Asconflict

pre-emptingmoves,theteacherrepeatedlysolicitstheallegedlyguilty

child’s(Miran’s)perspectiveandworkstoelicithispublicdisplay

ofhisunderstanding,aimingtoestablishmoralintersubjectivity

betweenthepeers(e.g.,addressingMiran‘doyouknowwhatJohnie

meansthen?’,lines35).Theteacher’sconflictresolutionis

associ-atedwithinculcatingknowledgeabouttheindividualperspectives

withaninquiry,ratherthanareproachordiscipliningofthe

cul-pablechild(lines34-35;38).

Inall,Ex.2a-bshowhowtheteacherrepeatedlytriestoengage

thechildrenintosituatedperspective-takingthroughtheuseof

verbalizations,whichisatthecoreofconflictprevention(e.g.,‘do

youknowwhat Johniemeans?’).Thesediscursivestrategiesare

highly focused onanindividual’s preferences. They alsoreflect

normativeexpectationsandassumptionsthatunderstandingthe

other’sperspective–understandingalterity(Bakhtin,1981)–is

importantforthedevelopmentofthechildasamemberofSwedish

society. Discursively, the teacher’s solicitation of the involved

children’stalk(including therepeatedarticulation oftheir

per-spectives, Ex. 1a-b) not only creates conditions for egalitarian

participation, but alsoconstitutes an interactional template for

youngchildren’sdevelopmentofinteractionalcompetences.Here,

the children receive guidanceon how toproduce verbally and

morallyconvincingtellingofuntowardevents.Perspectivetakingis

thusnotmerelyacognitivefeature,characterizingthechild’s

devel-opment,butis trainedand exercisedindiscursivepractices (cf.

multipartyparent-childinteractions, Blum-Kulka&Snow,2002;

DeLeón,2012).

In addition, theteacher scaffolds thechildren’s verbalskills

forpreventingnegativeemotionsandphysicalnormative

trans-gressions.Teacherconflictmediationconstitutesaninstitutionally

guidedbutdialogicallyconstructedgroundformoraland

interac-tionalsocialization,whichthechildrennotonlyalignedwithbut

alsore-interpretedandresisted.

4. Concludingdiscussion

The present study examined the discursive, linguistic and

embodiedfeaturesoftheteachers’andchildren’swaysof

organiz-ingandparticipatinginconflictmediationandresolution.Indoing

so,itdescribedthewaysinwhichchildreninpreschoolsinSweden

arebeingsocializedintotheinteractionalcompetencesnecessary

formanagingconflictsituations.Thestudydocumentedhowpeer

conflictresolutionwasaccomplishedasteachertriadicmediation,

locatedwithinembodiedmultipartyframeworksof

accountabil-ity,howchildrenweresocializedintoperspectivetaking,andhow

moralnormswereinvoked,inculcatedorresisted(seealsoCekaite,

2012a;Burdelski,thisspecialissue;Moore,thisspecialissue).The

studyrevealedtheambiguityandcomplexityofconflictresolution

thattheteachersconfiguredasegalitariandiscourse,butthatthey

werenotalwaysabletoachievebecauseofthelackofthechildren’s

activeandnormativelyappropriateparticipation.

4.1. Triadicconflictmediation

Thecharacteristicfeatureofconflictresolutiondocumentedin

(8)

2018,seealsoresearchonkindergartenandprimaryclassrooms,

Cekaite,2012b,2013)wasassociatedwiththeestablishmentof

atriadicparticipationframework withinwhichtheparticipants

togetherexplicatedmoralandemotionalmeaningsand

perspec-tives.Theembodiedmultipartyconstellationofparticipantsserved

asinteractionalinterpretiveframeworksinthejointinvestigation

andevaluation,andmoralsanctioningandsocializationofthe

chil-dren’sfuture actions and social relations.Strongemphasis was

placedonallparties’righttobeheardaswellasontheimportance

oflisteningtoothersandtryingtounderstandtheirperspective.

Telling one’s own and listening to the opponent’s versions

ofeventsasforms ofperspective takingwasguidedand

struc-turedthroughtheteachers’epistemicquestionsthatinvokedmoral

accountabilities of the parties (cf., Sterponi, 2014). Through a

rangeofdiscursiveconstructions,morallyacceptableconductand

responsibilitieswereprimarilylinkedtothechild’s–individual

actor’s–preferencesratherthantoadultauthorityandcollective

values.Suchconflictresolutionpracticesthatencouragechildren

tofocusonothers’perspectivescanberelatedtosocietalvalues

(includingthetaskssettotheteachersbytheSwedishNational

Curriculum)tofosterandsocializechildrenintotheunderstanding

ofindividualfreedomandintegrityandtheequalvalueofall

peo-ple.Theyoungchildren(threetofiveyearsold)weresocializedto

pre-emptconflictbytakingintoaccounttheindividual’spersonal

choices,volition,andemotionalstates.Theunderlyingnormative

expectations,indexing the wider cultural normsof democracy,

justiceandequality,children’sparticipatoryrights,individualism,

andgroupsolidaritywereinstantiatedthroughtheteacher’sand

thechildren’s discursivestrategies. The teacher’s triadic

medi-ation within an embodied framework of mutual accountability

canbeseen as“theculturalstructuringof everydaylife” (Ochs,

1996),involvingunderstandingone’srightsandobligations,

tak-ingresponsibilityforone’sactions,listening,reflecting,expressing

one’sownopinionsandunderstandingother’sopinions.This

child-centered approach is in line with the broader social values in

Swedishsocietymoregenerally,andinSwedishschools in

par-ticular.

4.2. Elicitationofperspectivetakingwithinanembodied

frameworkofmoralaccountability

Teachers’ solicitation of children’s alternative versions of

theproblematic eventswithin triadicparticipation frameworks

openedupaninteractionalspacetoengageintheprocessof

social-izingthestudentsinhowtomanageconflictverbally(e.g.,Ahn,

2010).Thechildrenweresocializedtodeployverbaland

embod-iedresources,todiscerntheirindividualvolition,wants,wishes

andpreferences (e.g.,whataction theylikeor not)andto

ver-ballyarticulatetheirperspectivestotheirpeers.However,inmany

casesyoungchildren’sverbalparticipationwasminimal.For

var-iousreasons(lackofinterestorunderstanding,reluctance),they

repeatedlydigressedfromparticipatinginwaysrequestedbythe

teacher.Notably,theteachers’interactionalworktoestablishand

sustainanegalitariandiscursiveorganizationofconflictresolution

demonstrates their orientation to justice, rights, and

responsi-bilities.Discursivestrategiesemployedallowedeachindividual’s

positiontobearticulatedtowardstheteachers,andotherpersons

involvedintheconflict.Theteachersaimedtosecureajustaccount,

attimesscaffoldingandguidingchildren’sperspectivetaking.In

suchways,triadicteachermediationconstitutedaninteractional

traininggroundforthechildren’suseofdiscursivestrategiesfor

handlingfutureconflictsituationsandpre-emptingconflicts.This

kindofmediationinstantiatedthenotionofmorality that

con-veyedstrongorientationtowardsindividual’srightstoarticulate

one’sperspectiveandtobeheard,implementingsocietal

egalitar-ianindividualism,whereindividual’sperspectivesareputinthe

foreground,simultaneouslyashumans’equalvalueisstated.

4.3. Dilemmasassociatedwithtriadicconflictmediation

Importantly,someoftheimplicationsofthestudyarerelated

tothedilemmasembeddedwithinthediscursiveimplementation

of theegalitarian and individualistic societalviews. The

teach-ers’solicitationoftellingsfromboththe’victim’and’guiltychild’

wasindexicalindexicalofwidersocietalviewsonchildren’srights

anddemocracy,andtheconceptualizationofchildrenofvarious

agesascompetentandreceptivetorationalargumentation(e.g.,

Aronsson & Cekaite, 2011; Goodwin & Cekaite, 2018 on

simi-lardiscursiveorganizationinSwedishparent-childinteractions).

Suchargumentationandmoralreasoningwerenotneutral.Rather,

thesediscursivepracticeswereimplicitlysteeredbytheteachers

towardsaparticularmoralview.Theteachersdidnotarticulate

orpresentthisviewexplicitlyastheirownorinstitutional

nor-mativestance.At times,suchtriadic teachermediationand its

implicit moralinstructionswereunsuccessful inthat a

particu-lar,institutionallydesirabledirectionofmoralinstructionwasnot

easilyaccomplishedinsocialinteractionwiththeyoungchildren

whodidnotengageinthetellingsinexpectedways.Theadultsin

suchcasesassistedandsteeredthechildrenintheproductionof

theinstitutionallyrelevantnormativediscourse.Paradoxically,by

usingopen-ended,polarandtag-questions,theystrictlyscaffolded

thechildrenintotakingontheinstitutionalnormsofconduct.At

thesametime,theydownplayedthearticulationof,fortheschool

orsociety,acommonsetofrules,normsandexpectations.

Bymodelingthechildren’sdeploymentofthelinguisticand

dis-cursivefeaturesthatforegroundedtheindividualchild’swishes,

ratherthangeneralmoralnorms,institutional expectationsand

values,thepreschoolteachersconveyedpositivestancestoward

children’sindividualrightsinrelationtotheirpeersandindexed

theviewof social relationshipsas beingaboutindividual

pref-erences.Duringconflictresolutionpractices,theteacherscanbe

seentoworktofosterdemocraticvalues,perspectivetakingand

equality,aswellaschildren’sparticipatoryrights,foregrounded

bywidersociety.Simultaneouslyteachers’primaryorientationto

individual’sprivateemotionalstatessocializedindividual-directed

empathy,rather than commonalityand sharedmoral

responsi-bility.Such practicesmakeitpossibletodiscursivelyinstantiate

children’s rights toparticipation and agency, atthe same time

astheymayfosterindividualismanddownplaythesharedmoral

order,commonalityandsharednessofmoralvalues.

5. Transcriptionconventions

: :prolongedsyllable AMP :relativelyhighamplitude (()) :furthercommentsofthetranscriber ? :denotesrisingterminalintonation . :indicatesfallingterminalintonation

bro :soundsmarkedbyemphaticstressareunderlined kommer :indicatestalkinSwedish

(.) :micropause

(0.5) :pauselengthinseconds come :translationtoEnglish

[ :indicatesoverlapintalkornonverbalacts

ConflictsofInterest

Theauthorsdeclarethattheyhavenoknowncompeting

finan-cialinterestsorpersonalrelationshipsthatcouldhaveappearedto

(9)

References

Ahn,J.(2010).‘I’mnotscaredofanything:Emotionassocialpowerinchildren’s worlds.Childhood,17,94–112.

Ahn,J.(2016).“Don’tCry,You’reNotaBaby!”:Emotion,roleandhierarchyinKorean languagesocialisationpractice.Children&Society,30,12–24.

Aronsson,K.(2012).Dailypracticesandthetimepoliticsoffamilylife.InM. Hede-gaard,K.Aronsson,C.Hojholt,&O.Ulvik(Eds.),Children,childhoodandeveryday life(pp.75–90).Charlotte,NC:InformationAgePublishing.

Aronsson,K.,&Cekaite,A.(2011).Activitycontractsanddirectivesineveryday familypolitics.DiscourseinSociety,22(2),1–18.

Bakhtin,M.(1981).Thedialogicimagination.Austin:UniversityofTexas.

Bateman,A.(2015).Conversationanalysisandearlychildhoodeducation.The co-productionofknowledgeandrelationships.Surrey:AshgatePublishing.

Bergmann,J.(1998).Introduction:Moralityindiscourse.ResearchonLanguageand SocialInteraction,31,279–294.

Björk-Willén,P.(2018).Learningtoapologize:Moralsocializationasaninteractional practiceinpreschool.ResearchonChildrenandSocialInteraction,2,177–194.

Bruner,J.(1990).Actsofmeaning.Cambridge,MA:HarwardUniversityPress.

Burdelski,M.(2013).“I’msorry,flower”Socializingapology,relationships,and empathyinJapan.PragmaticsandSociety,4(1),54–81.

Burdelski,M.(2015).Reportedspeechasculturalglossanddirective:Socializing normsofspeakingandactinginJapanesecaregiver-childtriadicinteraction. Text&Talk,35(5),575–595.

Blum-Kulka,S.,&Snow,C.(Eds.).(2002).Talkingtoadults:Thecontributionof multi-partydiscoursetolanguageacquisition.Mahwah,NJ:LawrenceErlbaum.

Cekaite,A.(2012a).Affectivestancesinteacher-novicestudentinteractions: Lan-guage,embodiment,andwillingnesstolearn.LanguageinSociety,41,641–670.

Cekaite,A.(2012b).Tattlinganddisputeresolution.InS.Danby,&M.Theobald(Eds.), Disputesineverydaylife:Socialandmoralordersofchildrenandyoungpeople(pp. 165–193).NewYork:Emerald.

Cekaite,A.(2013).Socializingemotionallyandmorallyappropriatepeergroup con-ductthroughclassroomdiscourse.LinguisticsandEducation,24,511–522.

Cekaite,A.,&Bergnéhr,D.(2018).Affectionatetouchandcare:Embodiedintimacy, compassionandcontrolinearlychildhoodeducationinSweden.EuropeanEarly ChildhoodEducationJournal,26(6),940–955.

Clancy,P.(1999).ThesocializationofaffectinJapanesemother-childconversation. JournalofPragmatics,31,1397–1421.

Danby,S.,&Theobald,M.(2012).Disputesineverydaylife:Socialandmoralordersof childrenandyoungpeople.NewYork:Emerald.

DeLeón,L.(2012).Languagesocializationandmultipartyparticipationframeworks. InA.Duranti,E.Ochs,&B.Schieffelin(Eds.),Thehandbookoflanguage socializa-tion(pp.81–111).Malden,MA:Wiley-Blackwell.

Demuth,C.(2013).Socializinginfantstowardaculturalunderstandingofexpressing negativeaffect:ABakhtinianinformeddiscursivepsychologyapproach.Mind, CultureandActivity,20,39–61.

Evaldsson,A.-C.,&Melander,H.(2016).Managingdisruptivestudentconduct: Neg-ativeemotionsandaccountabilityinreproach-responsesequences.Linguistics andEducation,37,73–86.

Goffman,E.(1971).Relationsinpublic.NewYork:BasicBooksInc.

Goffman,E.(1981).Formsoftalk.UniversityofPensylvaniaPress.

Goodwin,C.(2018).Co-operativeaction.Cambridge:CambridgeUniversityPress.

Goodwin,C.,&Goodwin,M.H.(2004).Participation.Handbookoflinguistic anthro-pology.WileyBlackwell.

Goodwin,M.H.(1990).He-said-she-said.IndianaUniversityPress.

Goodwin,M.H.,&Cekaite,A.(2013).Calibrationindirective/responsesequencesin familyinteractions.J.Pragmat.,46,122–138.

Goodwin,M.,&Cekaite,A.(2018).Embodiedfamilychoreography:Practicesofcontrol, careandmundanecreativity.Routledge.

Goodwin,M.(2006).Thehiddenlifeofgirls.Malden:BlackwellPublishing.

Griswold,O.(2010).TheEnglishyouneedtoknow.Thelanguageideologyina citizenshipclassroom.LinguisticsandEducation,22,406–418.

Johansson,E.,&Emilsson,A.(2016).Conflictsandresistance:Potentialsfor democ-racylearninginpreschool.InternationalJournalofEarlyYearsEducation,24, 19–35.

HolmKvist,M.(2018).Children’scryinginplayconflicts:Alocusformoraland emotionsocialisation.ResearchonChildrenandSocialInteraction,2,153–176.

Howard,K.(2009).“WhenmeetingKhunteacher,eachtimeweshouldpayrespect”. StandardizingrespectinaNorthernThaiclassroom.LanguageandEducation,20, 254–272.

Kendon,A.(1990).Spatialorganizationinsocialencounters:TheF-formation sys-tem.InA.Kendon(Ed.),Conductingsocialinteraction(pp.209–238).Cambridge: CambridgeUniversityPress.

Keane,W.(2016).Ethicallife:Itsnaturalandsocialhistories.Princeton:Princeton UniversityPress.

Lo,A.(2004).EvidentialityandmoralityinaKoreanheritagelanguageschool. Prag-matics,2–3,235–256.

Lo,A.(2009).LessonsaboutrespectandaffectinaKoreanAmericanheritage lan-guageschool.LinguisticsandEducation,20,217–234.

Moore,E.(2013).“Childrenarealllookingatyou”Childsocialization,directive tra-jectoriesandaffectivestancesinaRussianpreschool.PragmaticsandSociety, 4(3),317–344.

Ochs,E.(1996).Linguisticresourcesforsocializinghumanity.InJ.Gumperz,&S. Levinson(Eds.),Rethinkinglinguisticrelativity(pp.407–437).NewYork: Cam-bridgeUniversityPress.

Schieffelin,B.,&Ochs,E.(1986).Languagesocializationacrosscultures.Cambridge: CambridgeUniversityPress.

Sterponi,L.(2014).Caughtred-handed:HowItalianparentsengagechildrenin moraldiscourseandaction.InC.Waynryb,&H.E.Recchia(Eds.),Talkingabout rightandwrong:Parent-childconversationsascontextformoraldevelopment. Cambridge:CambridgeUniversityPress.

Curriculumforthepreschool,Lpfö18.Stockholm:Skolverket,2018.

Tobin,J.,Wu,D.,&Davidson,D.(1991).Preschoolinthreecultures:Japan,Chinaand theUnitedStates.Yale:YaleUniversityPress.

Tomasello,M.(2019).Becominghuman:Atheoryofhumanontogeny.Cambridge,MA: HarvardUniversityPress.

References

Related documents

Furthermore, and for the purpose of this literature review, the reader is reminded that parental engagement is defined ‘as motivated parental attitudes and behaviours intended

The preschool teachers’ interpretation of what mathematics for young children is and how it can be communicated to them affects the rules of a didactic contract and the

46 Konkreta exempel skulle kunna vara främjandeinsatser för affärsänglar/affärsängelnätverk, skapa arenor där aktörer från utbuds- och efterfrågesidan kan mötas eller

The increasing availability of data and attention to services has increased the understanding of the contribution of services to innovation and productivity in

Generella styrmedel kan ha varit mindre verksamma än man har trott De generella styrmedlen, till skillnad från de specifika styrmedlen, har kommit att användas i större

I dag uppgår denna del av befolkningen till knappt 4 200 personer och år 2030 beräknas det finnas drygt 4 800 personer i Gällivare kommun som är 65 år eller äldre i

Espon har ännu inte studerat vad olika städers och regioners tillgång till och användning av internet verkligen betyder för deras utveckling, och för utvecklingen i andra delar av

The present study is a qualitative empirical research that aims to explore the beliefs of preschool educators (principals, teachers and childminders) on early multilingualism