• No results found

Early specialisation among Swedish aesthetic performers : exploring motivation and perceptions of parental influence

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "Early specialisation among Swedish aesthetic performers : exploring motivation and perceptions of parental influence"

Copied!
22
0
0

Loading.... (view fulltext now)

Full text

(1)

http://www.diva-portal.org

This is the published version of a paper published in International Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology.

Citation for the original published paper (version of record): Downing, C., Redelius, K., Nordin-Bates, S. (2021)

Early specialisation among Swedish aesthetic performers: exploring motivation and perceptions of parental influence

International Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, : 1-20 https://doi.org/10.1080/1612197X.2021.1940239

Access to the published version may require subscription. N.B. When citing this work, cite the original published paper.

This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons

Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/ by-nc-nd/4.0/), which permits non-commercial re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited, and is not altered, transformed, or built upon in any way.

Permanent link to this version:

(2)

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at

https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=rijs20

International Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology

ISSN: (Print) (Online) Journal homepage: https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/rijs20

Early specialisation among Swedish aesthetic

performers: exploring motivation and perceptions

of parental influence

Charlotte Downing, Karin Redelius & Sanna Nordin-Bates

To cite this article: Charlotte Downing, Karin Redelius & Sanna Nordin-Bates (2021): Early specialisation among Swedish aesthetic performers: exploring motivation and perceptions of parental influence, International Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, DOI: 10.1080/1612197X.2021.1940239

To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/1612197X.2021.1940239

© 2021 The Author(s). Published by Informa UK Limited, trading as Taylor & Francis Group

View supplementary material

Published online: 17 Jun 2021. Submit your article to this journal

Article views: 173 View related articles

(3)

Early specialisation among Swedish aesthetic performers:

exploring motivation and perceptions of parental in

fluence

Charlotte Downing , Karin Redelius and Sanna Nordin-Bates

Swedish School of Sport and Health Sciences, Stockholm, Sweden

ABSTRACT

Early specialisation is largely advised against, partly due to the postulated negative motivational implications. However, early specialisation is commonly considered necessary for high-level performance in aesthetic activities, such as gymnastics and dance. The present study, therefore, explores the relationship between motivation and early specialisation in a sample of Swedish aesthetic performers, from a self-determination theory perspective. The aims of this study were twofold: (1) to identify whether early specialisation is associated with motivation (autonomous motivation, controlled motivation, and dropout intentions) within a sample of aesthetic performers, and (2) to investigate if such relationships are moderated by perceptions of parental influence. Two hundred and ninety high-level aesthetic performers (M = 15.88 years old, SD = 2.34; 83% female) were recruited from Swedish clubs and schools to complete a questionnaire pack. The questionnaire pack included questions concerning demographic information, specialisation history, motivation, dropout intentions, and perceptions of parental influence. The results of our analyses do not support the claims that early specialisation is associated with negative motivational implications. In fact, the results show that those who reported a higher degree of specialisation ≤ 12 years old reported less controlled motivation than those who reported a lesser degree of early specialisation. Additionally, perceptions of parental influence were not found to moderate the relationship between early specialisation and motivation. These results are discussed in relation to the growing critique regarding the conceptualisation and measurement of early specialisation in sport literature.

ARTICLE HISTORY Received 12 June 2020 Accepted 28 April 2021 KEYWORDS

Early specialisation; aesthetic sports; dance; dropout; self-determination theory

The notion of early specialisation

Early specialisation is commonly defined as intense participation in one sport, to the exclusion of others, that is trained and/or competed in for more than 8 months of the year, characteristically referring to pre-pubertal (≤12 years old) children (LaPrade et al.,

2016). The topic of early specialisation garners a great deal of academic and media inter-est. For example, the developmental model of sport participation (DMSP; Côté et al.,

© 2021 The Author(s). Published by Informa UK Limited, trading as Taylor & Francis Group

This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/), which permits non-commercial re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited, and is not altered, transformed, or built upon in any way.

CONTACT Charlotte Downing charlotte.downing@gih.se https://doi.org/10.1080/1612197X.2021.1940239

(4)

2007), which outlines several risks of early specialisation, had been cited over 950 times at the time of writing (Google Scholar). Several organisations such as the American Ortho-paedic Society for Sport Medicine (LaPrade et al.,2016) and the International Society of Sport Psychology (Côté et al.,2009) recommend against early specialisation, in part due to the potential motivational risks such as dropout and reduced enjoyment. Many of the numerous reviews conclude that early specialisation is not necessary for high-level success and should therefore be avoided to safeguard against the postulated risks (e.g., Gould,2010; Malina,2010; Wiersma,2000). Other researchers have suggested that early specialisation has been labelled as “bad” irrespective of the inconsistent results within previous literature (Baker et al., 2021; Ford & William, 2017; Kliethermes et al., 2021; Mosher et al.,2020; Waldron et al.,2020). Despite the widespread and often uncritically accepted recommendations against early specialisation, it is evident that such training continues to occur in youth sport internationally.

Early specialisation may be particularly prevalent in aesthetic sports where it has been highlighted as potentially advantageous to meet the demands of current sport structures (e.g., Kliethermes et al., 2021; LaPrade et al., 2016). Empirical evidence exploring the psychological correlates of early specialisation within this population is lacking, however. This provides rationale for the present study, which explores the relationship between early specialisation and motivational factors among a sample of Swedish aes-thetic performers.

A long standing argument in favour of early specialisation is that it can be associated with high-level performance. For instance, one early study reported accumulated training hours to be positively related to attainment in gymnastics (Hume et al., 1993). A study similar in design found that although there were no significant differences in the age at which Olympic and international level gymnasts began training, the Olympic gymnasts started competing at the regional level earlier (Law et al.,2007). Thesefindings have often been interpreted to suggest that aspects of early specialisation may be beneficial for expertise development in aesthetic activities (e.g., Coutinho et al., 2016; Côté et al.,

2007; Jayanthi et al., 2013; LaPrade et al., 2016). However, these studies primarily focused on training volume. They did not capture all of the collective components of early specialisation, such as the onset of year-round training in addition to training volume and limited participation in other sports. The conclusions that may be drawn from studies like these regarding early specialisation are therefore limited.

While potentially increasing attainment, early specialisation is postulated to reduce enjoyment (Côté et al., 2007; Côté & Vierimaa, 2014). This suggestion is grounded in the notion that specialised training for the purpose of skill acquisition, through deliberate practice, is not inherently enjoyable (Ericsson et al.,1993). However, sport research has questioned this assumption, instead showing that athletes often enjoy their deliberate practice (Baker & Young,2014). Moreover, there is a lack of empirical research exploring the relationship between enjoyment, and overall motivation, in relation to early special-isation. The literature that does exist has found varied results. For example, Russell and Symonds (2015) found that, within a sample of athletes from various sports and perform-ance levels, motivation towards participation did not differ between specialised and non-specialised athletes. Other researchers found that gymnasts who reached milestones of specialisation earlier, such as commencing competition, were less likely to enjoy their training experience (Law et al.,2007).

(5)

Previous research has suggested that lack of enjoyment may also be a precursor to dropout (Salguero et al., 2003). Wall and Côté’s (2007) study on a small group of ice hockey players found that dropouts begun more specialised training earlier than continu-ing players. Such findings suggest that early specialisation may have negative impli-cations for optimal motivational engagement. Although widely referenced as evidence against early specialisation, Wall and Côté’s (2007) study reported no differences between continuing and dropout players in terms of time in organised sport, specialised training camps and playful engagement. Instead, just one aspect of specialised training, off-ice training, was related to dropout. A more recent study by Larson and colleagues (2019) reported no significant relationships between early specialisation and dropout in competitive swimmers. This raises questions concerning the strength of the previously reported associations between early specialisation and negative psychological impli-cations, not least because Larson et al.’s study comprised both a stronger research design and a larger sample.

Motivational aspects of early specialisation

There is a large body of research suggesting that motivation is one of many important factors in athlete development and success (e.g., Adie et al.,2008; Moesch et al.,2013). Motivation drives talent development in sport and ultimately impacts athletic perform-ance and wellbeing (e.g., Elbe & Wikman,2017; Martindale et al.,2005). Therefore, early specialisation has been suggested to be detrimental for long-term athlete development if it inhibits the development of intrinsic motivation (Côté et al.,2007).

From a self-determination theory perspective (SDT; Deci & Ryan,1985; 2000), motiv-ation is conceptualised to comprise six motivmotiv-ational regulmotiv-ations which correspond to being either controlled (external and introjected), autonomous (identified, integrated and intrinsic), or absent (amotivation). Controlled motivational regulations reflect beha-viours associated with external pressures from rewards or punishments, or internal press-ures from guilt, whereas autonomous motivation regulations reflect volition, meaningful engagement and enjoyment (Deci & Ryan, 2000). Early specialisation may foster con-trolled motivation if the training environment is perceived as pressurising, or the athletes do not perceive choice and autonomy in their continued training.

Autonomous motivation has long been considered beneficial to athlete development (Robinson & Carron,1982; Vallerand & Bissonnette,1992; Vink et al.,2015). The association between autonomous motivation and achievement may exist because people exert more effort into, and are more engaged in, an activity that is enjoyable (Ryan & Deci,2000). Research involving high-level adolescent dancers identified that intrinsic motivation strengthens enjoyment, which in turn drives commitment (Aujla et al.,2014). We there-fore explore motivational outcomes within aesthetic activities, where early specialisation may be particularly prevalent.

In addition to autonomous and controlled motivation, the present study also explores performers’ dropout intentions as an additional dimension of motivation. Dropout is often considered a potential behavioural outcome of sustained controlled motivation and/or amotivation (Vallerand & Bissonnette, 1992). For example, one study of Australian youth gymnasts reported that continuing gymnasts had greater intrinsic motives, and lesser extrinsic motives, in comparison to gymnasts who eventually dropped out (Ryska

(6)

et al.,2002). Importantly, previous researchers have discussed the multifaceted nature of dropout from physical activities. As such, intention to drop out is added as a supplemen-tary variable to account for aspects of dropout that may not be related to motivation, such as conflicting demands (Aujla et al.,2014). Several studies have explored the relationship between early specialisation and dropout, with mixed results (e.g., Larson et al.,2019; Wall & Côté,2007). It is important, therefore, to continue to develop research in this area, par-ticularly in new populations.

Despite the prevalence of articles discussing the potential motivational implications of early specialisation, DiSanti and Erickson (2019) suggested that much of the research in this area lacks a theory-driven approach. In the present study, therefore, we adopted a theoretical approach to motivation. By viewing the potential associations between early specialisation and motivation through the lens of SDT, we hope to build upon pre-vious research. Using SDT also allows a theoretically integrated way to enhance knowl-edge, by examining the potential role of parental influence in the early specialisation – motivation relationship.

Parental influence in the context of early specialisation

The role of parents in athletic development has been explored from many perspec-tives. Parents are often initiators for their child’s involvement in sport and are typically responsible for providing logistical and financial support (e.g., Gould et al., 2008). In addition to initiating sport participation, parental influence remains a unique and important factor throughout all stages of athletic development (Coakley, 2006; Knight, 2017). As such, parents potentially have a significant impact, both positive and negative, on the athletic development of their children. Within the boundaries of this study, we consider how parents may impact performer motivation. A previous study of female gymnasts highlighted that autonomy support provided by parents and coaches was associated with higher autonomous motivation for training (Gagné,

2003). However, due to the well-known and perhaps overstated association between training volume and achievement (Ericsson et al., 1993), parents may encourage, or even pressurise, their children to specialise at a young age (Patel & Jayanthi, 2018). The role of parents is, therefore, important to explore in relation to the postulated out-comes of early specialisation.

Gould (2010) commented that parents can become overinvolved in youth sport because their self-competence as a parent is contingent on their child’s success. Coakley’s (2006) study also highlighted that parents are attributed success as a result of their child’s excellence in sport. Particularly in aesthetic sports, where young peak performance ages perhaps encourage early specialisation, young athletes and dancers may not be able to make informed decisions about their training. It is therefore possible that parents, and not the children themselves, are making the decision to specialise. The lack of autonomy derived from parents making decisions on behalf of their child can potentially be detri-mental to the development of autonomous motivation (Deci & Ryan,2000). As a result, those with parents who offer less autonomy support may be at greater risk of negative outcomes following early specialisation. Therefore, in the present study, we explored per-ceptions of parental influence as a potential moderator of the proposed relationship between early specialisation and motivation.

(7)

To our knowledge, potential moderators have not been explored within the early specialisation literature. Instead, many of the studies exploring early specialisation do so withfirst generation research questions; investigating for example, if x is related to y (Zanna & Fazio,1982). The present study is, therefore, novel in introducing perceptions of parental influence as a potential moderator between early specialisation and motiva-tional implications (i.e., a second-generation research question).

Early specialisation in the Swedish context

As social-cultural factors may influence the process of early specialisation, it is important to consider the Swedish context of this research. The Swedish sports confederation (Riksidrottsförbundet,2019) supports and advocates the notion of“sport for all”, with a general mission to have as many people involved in sport for as long as possible. As such, the Swedish perspective in general opposes early specialisation in favour of promot-ing life-long engagement in sport for all, via playful and diversified early sport experiences (Riksidrottsförbundet, 2019). Despite this “sport for all” mission, Sjöblom and Fahlén (2010) suggested that sport in Sweden has shifted towards a more competitive and inten-sified culture where greater resources are available to sport clubs who have higher com-petitive success. Favouring comcom-petitive results, as well as increased commercialisation of youth sport, may drive early specialisation despite the strongly portrayed message against single sport specialisation before age 13 from the national sports confederation (Riksidrottsförbundet,2019).

Research in Sweden indicates that aesthetic athletes on national teams are younger on average than other national team athletes, with over 85% of aesthetic athletes not having participated in any other organised sport besides their main one (Fahlström et al.,2015). As such, early specialisation is potentially more commonplace in aesthetic activities, indi-cated by the possibility for gymnasts to begin training from around age three, and dancers being able to audition for full-time training at age nine. When warnings against early specialisation in the majority of sports are both prevalent and strongly con-veyed, it is worrisome that there is so little research on aesthetic populations.

Similar to the organisation of youth sport, the role of parents is also impacted by cul-tural factors. Eliasson (2015) suggested that Swedish parents involve their children in sports for reasons such as socialisation andfitness, and have little expectation of competi-tive results. As such, parents in Sweden appear to prioritise overall child development and the nurturing of positive behaviours such as teamwork, rather than specific skills develop-ment. Parents prioritising wider developmental aspects of sport involvement, and gener-ally focusing on the best interest of their children, perhaps reflects autonomy supportive parenting behaviours within the context of Swedish youth sport.

Aims

Responding to calls in previous research for further empirical evidence regarding early specialisation (e.g., Ford & William,2017; Kliethermes et al.,2021), and drawing on the somewhat contradictory research and the theoretical foundation of SDT, the aims of this study were twofold: (1) to identify whether early specialisation is associated with motivation (autonomous motivation, controlled motivation, and dropout intentions)

(8)

within a sample of aesthetic performers, and (2) to investigate if such relationships are moderated by perceptions of parental influence.

Methods

This study comprised a cross-sectional questionnaire-based design. Aesthetic performers in Sweden were recruited, via purposeful sampling, to complete questionnaires concern-ing demographic information, specialisation history, motivation, dropout intentions, and perceptions of parental influence. The questionnaire pack includes both validated and study-specific measures to enable exploration of the study aims.

Participants

Four recruitment criteria were employed for this study: that participants should (1) con-sider their aesthetic activity to be their“main” activity, (2) train three or more times per week, (3) be engaged in regular competitions (sport) or in full-time training (dance), and (4) be aged 12–20 years. Two hundred and ninety aesthetic performers aged 12– 20 years (M = 15.88 years old, SD = 2.34; 83% female) from schools, clubs and teams within Sweden were surveyed. The sample was comprised of 115 gymnasts (25 artistic; 22 rhythmic; 51 team; 17 trampoline), 71 dancers (including a wide spread of dance styles, such as ballet, jazz and modern), 69figure skaters, 27 divers and 8 synchronised swimmers. The performers trained an average of 15.00 h per week (SD = 7.74) and over 70% were training at national or international level (sport) or were in vocational or pro-fessional training (dance).

Measures

Early specialisation

An index approach capturing degrees of early specialisation was used (Downing et al.,

2020). The index approach aims to capture each of the individual components of early specialisation (single sport, intensity, year-round, and ≤12 years old), in line with LaPrade and colleagues’ (2016) definition. As such, participants responded to questions in relation to their training volume (e.g., When you were 6 years old or younger, how many times per week did you train?), year-round engagement (e.g., between 7 and 9 years old, did you train during term time only/term time plus some holidays/term time plus most holidays/term time plus all holidays?), the age at which they reached particular specialisation milestones (e.g., How old were you when you started your main activity?), and whether or not they sampled other physical activities (e.g., What other physical activi-ties have you participated in? Include start and stop ages).

Scores for each specialisation marker are weighted and added together to produce an index ranging from 0 (low/no early specialisation) to 42 (high degree of early specialis-ation). Workshops led by two of the authors were conducted with seven professionals from both applied and academic contexts to theorise what can be considered low, mod-erate, and high for each early specialisation marker. Drawing upon the knowledge and experience of our workshop attendees, we conceptualised that a low score would be

(9)

obtained by those who reached key milestones of early specialisation later, sampled other sports or activities, and generally trained less. A moderate degree of early specialisation would apply to those who reached several key milestones of early specialisation prior to age 13, perhaps sampled other sports/activities, and generally trained a moderate amount. A high degree of early specialisation would apply to those who reached most key milestones of early specialisation at a young age (≤6 years), never sampled other sports/activities or became single sport participants very early, and generally trained a lot from a young age.

Motivation

For motivation, the Behavioural Regulation in Sport Questionnaire (BRSQ; Lonsdale et al.,

2008) was used. This questionnaire is widely used in sport psychology and was validated in Swedish by Stenling and colleagues (2018). The question stem“I participate in dance/ sport… ” was used with 24 answers conforming to the six motivational regulations. For example, “because it is fun” is a response corresponding to intrinsic motivation, and “because I would feel guilty if I quit” signifies introjected motivation. Participants responded to the items using a 7-point Likert scale from 1 (not true at all) to 7 (very true). The BRSQ has shown good levels of internal consistency, test-retest reliability and factorial validity in previous research (Lonsdale et al.,2008; Stenling et al.,2018). In line with previous research (e.g., Fenton et al.,2016), the subscales were combined to form two composite variables for autonomous (intrinsic, integrated and identified), and con-trolled (introjected, external) motivation.

Dropout intentions

For dropout intentions, a study-specific questionnaire was created as no standardised measures exist. The questionnaire is comprised of three items, which were adapted from previous studies by Le Bars et al. (2009) and Guillet et al. (2006); “is it likely that you will drop out of your main activity within the next three years?”, “will you continue to participate in your main activity at a high level?” and “have you previously considered dropping out of your main activity?” Each item was rated on a 7-point Likert scale with the following anchors: 1 (not at all), 4 (maybe), and 7 (definitely). The question concerning continued participation was reverse-scored before being averaged with the other items to produce a single variable.

Parental influence

For perceptions of parental influence, the Perceived Parental Autonomy Support Scale (P-PASS; Mageau et al.,2015) was used. The P-PASS is situated within SDT and asks about young people’s perceptions of parental autonomy support and psychological control. To manage the diversity of the modern family, the original wording of mother and father was changed to adult one and adult two. The participants were then able to identify which adults they perceived as their main guardians. The scores for the two guardians were then averaged, in line with previous research that has identified no major differences between maternal and paternal parental autonomy support (Mageau et al.,2015). All 24

(10)

items on the P-PASS are rated on a 7-point Likert scale from 1 (do not agree at all) to 7 (strongly agree), and participants completed all items for both of their main guardians. The parental autonomy support variable is comprised of 12 items from three subscales; choice within certain limits, rationale for demands and limits, and acknowledgment of feel-ings, with questions such as“within certain limits, my parents allowed me the freedom to choose my own activities”. The parental control variable is also comprised of 12 items from three subscales; threats to punish, performance pressures and guilt-inducing criticism, and questions included items such as,“my parents insisted that I always be better than others”. Several studies have supported the psychometric properties of the P-PASS in a wide range of countries and languages (Costa et al.,2016; Mageau et al.,2015; Mageau et al.,2016).

Procedure

Ethical approval was granted by the Swedish Ethics Review Authority in line with national, European and international guidelines. Appropriate schools, clubs and national teams in Sweden were informed about the study via email, and interested coaches/teachers dis-tributed the study information to performers. Informed consent was obtained from all participants and from parents as required. Pilot tests with three groups of dancers and gymnasts aged 12–16 years (N = 32; M age = 14.72) were conducted to provide an esti-mate for how long the questionnaire took to complete, and to ascertain that the youngest age groups were able to understand the questions. Questionnaires were administered at training venues with a researcher present (n = 240), or sent to the performers home address and returned via pre-paid post (n = 50). One-hour sessions were booked at train-ing venues to allow for variation in readtrain-ing and answertrain-ing speed, although participants typically took 20–45 min to complete the questionnaire pack. No incentive was given for participation, however, participants completing the questionnaire at their training venue were offered a small snack.

Statistical analysis

Data were analysed using IBM SPSS version 26.0 (2019). Descriptive statistics for all vari-ables can be seen inTable 1. Bivariate correlations were calculated using Spearman’s Rho

Table 1.Descriptives and bivariate correlations.

Mean SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 1. Early specialisation index 17.74 8.57 2. Age 15.88 2.34 −0.21** 3. Gender 0.13* −0.05 4. Autonomous motivation 5.63 0.97 0.03 0.15* 0.17** 5. Controlled motivation 2.16 1.13 −0.16* 0.22** −0.13* −0.09 6. Dropout intentions 3.17 1.50 −0.04 0.05 0.02 −0.34** 0.34** 7. Parental autonomy support 5.61 0.94 0.06 0.07 0.09 0.19** −0.27** −0.14* 8. Parental control 1.60 0.90 −0.01 0.10 −0.20** −0.07 0.42** 0.14* −0.47** Cronbach’s alpha 0.87 0.88 0.67 0.86 0.90

Note: The early specialisation index has a possible range of 0-42, all other variables are scored on a 1–7 Likert scale. Gender was scored as 1 = male and 2 = female. *p < .05, **p < .01.

(11)

due to the combination of continuous, dichotomous and ordinal data being explored. All variables, except dropout intentions (α = 0.67), produced acceptable Cronbach’s alphas at 0.7 or above (DeVellis,2016). As the dropout intentions variable is comprised of only three items and the inter-item correlations ranged from 0.33 to 0.46, the scale can still be con-sidered reliable (Briggs & Cheek,1986). Skewness and kurtosis exceeded an absolute value of 2 for parental control (skew = 2.82, kurtosis = 11.47), while all other variables were within the critical values. All tests were therefore conducted with both original and trans-formed versions of the variables, yet no notable differences were detected. Additionally, Hayes (2018) stated that regression-based analysis makes no assumptions concerning the shape of the distributions for y or its predictors; therefore, the original variables have been used throughout the statistical analyses.

Hayes’ PROCESS macro for SPSS (version 3.3, 2019) was used to explore the relationship between early specialisation and participants’ motivation, including parental autonomy support and control as moderating factors. As participant age and gender were both sig-nificantly related to reported degrees of specialisation, these were included as covariates within the analysis. Mahalanobis distance calculations were conducted with age, gender, specialisation index, parental autonomy support and parental control as independent variables; four cases violated the critical value and were removed from the moderation analysis.

Results

Descriptive statistics

Generally, the sample reported a moderate level of early specialisation (see Table 1). Specifically, participants reported reaching specialisation markers ≤ 12 years old, such as starting training in their main sport before age 7 (M = 6.88, SD = 3.03) and being selected into a specialist team or training programme around age 10 (M = 10.22, SD = 3.04). There are no guidelines on what training volume would indicate specialisation; however, in the present sample, training hours increased relatively sharply from just over one hour per week in the 0–6 age category, up to just over eight hours per week in the 10–12 age category. On average, participants reported low levels of controlled motivation and perceived parental control, moderate intentions to drop out, and high levels of autonomous motivation and perceived parental autonomy support.

Bivariate correlations

The correlation matrix for the specialisation index and all the composite variables can be seen inTable 1. Results indicated that older participants within the sample (range = 12–20 years) reported a lower degree of early specialisation (r =−.21, p < .01), higher auton-omous (r = .15, p < .01), and higher controlled motivation (r = .22, p < .01). Participants who were female were likely to report higher levels of early specialisation (r = .13, p < .05) and autonomous motivation (r = .17, p < .01), but lower levels of controlled motiv-ation (r =−.13, p < .05), and perceptions of parental control (r = −.20, p < .01) than their male peers.

(12)

When analysing potential relationships between degree of early specialisation and the motivational variables, a significant, yet small, result was identified for controlled motiv-ation (r =−.16, p < .05). Autonomous motivation and dropout intentions were not signifi-cantly related to reported degrees of early specialisation (both p > .05). There were no significant relationships between degree of early specialisation and parental influence being perceived as either autonomy supportive or controlling (both p > .05). Perceived parental autonomy support was, however, significantly related to autonomous motiv-ation (r = .19, p < .01), controlled motivmotiv-ation (r =−.27, p < .01), and dropout intentions (r =−.14, p < .05). Perceived parental control was significantly related to controlled motiv-ation (r = .42, p < .01), and dropout intentions (r = .14, p < .05), but not to autonomous motivation.

Moderation analysis

Next, the potential moderation of relationships between degree of early specialisation and autonomous motivation or dropout intentions were explored. While it may seem counter-intuitive to explore non-significant bivariate relationships with moderation ana-lyses, it is possible that relationships between variables emerge when moderators are introduced (Hayes,2018).

From the regression output provided by PROCESS (seeTable 2for full details) degree of early specialisation was not identified as a significant independent predictor of auton-omous motivation (b = .11, t(214) = 1.59, p > .05), controlled motivation (b = .06, t(217) = .79, p > .05), or dropout intentions (b = .09, t(217) = .83, p > .05). However, autonomous motivation was significantly predicted by age (b = .07, t(214) = 2.30, p < .05) and perceived

Table 2.Moderation and regression results.

b se t p Δr2

Dependent variable: Autonomous motivation (n = 222)

Early specialisation index 0.11 0.07 1.59 0.11

Parental autonomy support 0.44 0.20 2.22 0.03

Parental control 0.04 0.24 0.15 0.88

ES index X Parental autonomy support −0.02 0.01 −1.79 0.08 0.01 ES index X Parental control −0.00 0.01 −0.24 0.81 0.00

Age 0.07 0.03 2.30 0.02

Gender 0.26 0.17 1.50 0.14

Dependent variable: Controlled motivation (n = 225)

Early specialisation index 0.06 0.08 0.79 0.43

Parental autonomy support 0.19 0.23 0.84 0.40

Parental control 0.30 0.28 1.09 0.28

ES index X Parental autonomy support −0.02 0.01 −1.52 0.13 0.01

ES index X Parental control 0.02 0.02 1.12 0.27 0.01

Age 0.05 0.03 1.49 0.14

Gender 0.05 0.20 0.26 0.79

Dependent variable: Dropout intentions (n = 225)

Early specialisation index 0.09 0.11 0.83 0.41

Parental autonomy support 0.05 0.31 0.15 0.88

Parental control 0.12 0.38 0.31 0.75

ES index X Parental autonomy support −0.02 0.02 −1.10 0.27 0.01

ES index X Parental control 0.00 0.02 0.04 0.97 0.00

Age 0.00 0.05 0.09 0.93

Gender 0.29 0.27 1.08 0.28

(13)

parental autonomy support (b = .44 t(214) = 2.22, p < .05). Controlled motivation and dropout intentions were not significantly predicted by any of the independent variables. Autonomous motivation (F (7, 222) = 2.81, p < .01, R2= .08), and controlled motivation (F (7, 225) = 7.48, p < .001, R2= .19) were significantly predicted by the independent vari-ables in the model, however, dropout intention was not significantly predicted (F (7, 217) = 1.48, p > .05, R2= .05).

Results also suggested that, when controlling for age and gender, no significant inter-action effects (all p > .05) were found for any of the dependent variables. That is, differen-tial perceptions of parental influence had not masked any relationships between early specialisation and motivational indices in the previous, simpler, analyses.

Discussion

The aim of this study was to explore potential relationships between early specialisation and motivation in high-level aesthetic performers and consider perceptions of parental influence as potential moderators of this relationship. Overall, our results do not support the widely accepted notion that early specialisation is associated with sub-optimal motivation, as degree of early specialisation was not significantly related to autonomous motivation or intentions to drop out. In fact, those who reported a higher degree of early specialisation reported slightly lower controlled motivation, which directly contradicts much of the previous literature. However, the results presented here are in line with a wider movement in early specialisation research, where researchers have recently begun to reconsider some of the beliefs and recommendations surrounding early specialisation (Baker et al., 2021; DiSanti & Erickson, 2019; Downing et al., 2020; Kliethermes et al., 2021; Larson et al., 2019; Mosher et al., 2020). Such concerns are driven by an influx of recent research reporting null results when investigating the psychological associations of early specialisation (DiSanti & Erickson,2019; Larson et al.,

2019; Russell et al.,2017). Below we critically discuss our researchfindings and situate them within previous research, SDT, and the Swedish context.

Early specialisation and motivation

In the present sample of high-level aesthetic performers, reported autonomous motivation was distinctly higher than controlled motivation. Suchfindings mirror previous research in gymnastics and dance which found enjoyment to be a key component in success (Hume et al.,1993) and adherence (Aujla et al.,2014). Interestingly, our results also revealed that those reporting a greater degree of early specialisation reported somewhat lower levels of controlled motivation. Considering how many academics and professional organisations have aligned themselves to the notion that early specialisation has negative motivational outcomes (e.g., Côté & Vierimaa, 2014; Wall & Côté,2007; Wiersma,2000) this is a surprising finding. From a self-determination theory perspective, this suggests that those who specialised to a high degree would not be particularly at risk of lower motivation quality in their training. Therefore, ourfindings directly question the suitability of applying prin-ciples of the DMSP, at least within the context of Swedish aesthetic activities. While our results can possibly be explained from a methodological standpoint, whereby much of the early specialisation research utilises different measures (DiSanti & Erickson, 2019),

(14)

we can also consider that early specialisation may be driven by autonomous motivation. For instance, it is arguably possible for a child to initiate the specialisation process by asking to do more training within a single activity. Equally, if a child enjoys their training, they may not perceive external motives from parents or coaches to be pressurising. Future research may benefit from qualitative inquiry to explore different drivers behind early specialisation and how they are experienced.

The results also show that athletes who reported higher levels of early specialisation were no different from their less early specialised peers in terms of their intentions to drop out of their sport. This again is contentious, as a particularly well-referenced paper reports early specialisation to be a factor in dropout (Wall & Côté, 2007). However, ourfindings do mirror those of a recent study which found no relationship between early specialisation and dropout in high-level swimmers (Larson et al., 2019). As such, identifying no relationship between degrees of early specialisation and dropout in our results continues to build scepticism towards the application of the DMSP in aesthetic activities. This is arguably due to the reductionist nature of the DMSP being ill equipped to handle the complexity of reasons for dropout in sport. Indeed, research suggests that dropout intentions arise in response to a combination of multiple factors (Aujla et al.,2014; Eliasson & Johansson,2020). Therefore, early special-isation alone may not be the main contributing factor in dropout.

When interpreting the results presented here, we must consider that only those continu-ing in their traincontinu-ing at the time of our data collection were captured in our cross-sectional study: put simply, those who dropped out prior to, or indeed after, our data collection are naturally not represented. As such, it is possible that they did drop out due to sub-optimal motivation. Our results therefore reflect only that a higher degree of early specialisation, for those who continued training after age 12 and who took part in this particular study, was associated with somewhat lower controlled motivation but no higher/lower reported autonomous motivation or intentions to drop out. Recruiting athletes who have left their sport and investigating whether and in what ways early specialisation played a role in their dropout would, therefore, be a valuable addition to the literature.

Early specialisation and perceptions of parental influence

The results indicate that those who perceived higher levels of parental autonomy support also reported higher autonomous motivation, and those who perceived higher levels of parental control reported higher controlled motivation. The correlation coefficients observed in the present study are somewhat aligned to previous literature, such as the previous reported relationship between parental autonomy support and motivation in leisure time physical activity (Hagger et al.,2009). Although there are no studies using the same combination of measures as those presented in this paper, we do believe that our results show perceptions of parental influence, particularly regarding autonomy support, to be a moderately important factor in young athletes’ motivation towards train-ing. However, perceived parental influence did not impact, or change, the investigated relationships between early specialisation and motivation. As such, we can be reasonably confident that, within the present sample at least, there is no relationship between early specialisation and motivation regardless of how the performers perceived their parents’ levels of autonomy support and control.

(15)

It is notable that perceptions of parental influence were particularly positive in this study. Specifically, performers reported high autonomy support and low control from parents. Other studies using the P-PASS questionnaire have found a more even spread of scores for perceived parental control (e.g., Costa et al., 2016; Mageau et al., 2016). This potentially reflects the notion that Scandinavian sport culture is grounded in auton-omy supportive principles (Carlsson et al., 2011; Haraldsen et al., 2020; Ronglan,2015). Beyond sport, it has been suggested that parents in Scandinavia adopt a more autonomy supportive position when compared to parenting internationally. Specifically, one study found that adolescents from Denmark perceived higher autonomy support from authority figures (including parents), than adolescents in South Korea and America (Ferguson et al.,

2011). This raises questions concerning to what extent a parent would pressure their child to participate in competitive sport, or indeed initiate early specialisation, within a Scandi-navian context.

While previous research has suggested that parents might externally drive their child’s early specialisation (Patel & Jayanthi,2018), we argue that parents have the potential to initiate this process in an autonomy supportive way. For example, parents can actively seek out additional training opportunities, which are then discussed with the child before reaching a mutual decision regarding training. Alternatively, a child could be part of an extremely sporty family where everyone is or has been, for example, a gymnast. While this home environment has the potential to be perceived as pressurising, participating in gymnastics may also be a way for the child to gain a sense of identity from within the sport, and feel a greater sense of belonging with their family. As such, future research may wish to explore what it means for parents, or indeed coaches, to initiate or drive early specialisation, and whether this can be done in a way which places the ath-letes’ psychological wellbeing in the forefront.

The finding that participants within our study reported generally high autonomy support further reinforces the idea that perhaps early specialisation can be at least partly child-led. Specifically, if parents are not pressurising their child to specialise early, or indeed sample other sports or activities, this may lead to children following the typical pathways towards elite performance within their particular training context. As previously mentioned, aesthetic activities have long standing traditions of early specialis-ation, and perhaps children are happy to continue along this trajectory if it provides them with the opportunity to continue training in something they enjoy.

Early specialisation in relation to age

The relationship between age and reported degree of early specialisation provides some tentative support for the notion that early specialisation has become more common in recent years (Wiersma,2000). What has driven this potential increase in early specialisation is still relatively unknown, but sports clubs may be providing more opportunities for young athletes to undertake specialised training such as training camps (Coakley, 2010). This raises questions about the extent to which future, or even current, generations of young children will specialise early. It is particularly interesting that younger participants reported a higher degree of early specialisation within a sample of aesthetic performers, where early specialisation is, arguably, already commonplace (Hume et al., 1993). In other words, younger performers are specialising early to a higher degree within training cultures

(16)

where early specialisation is already considered the norm. As such, we are uncertain what a greater extent of early specialisation within this context would look like, or consist of. Although this study has found no evidence of negative associations between motivation and early specialisation, we cannot assume this relationship will be stable over time as training norms continue to change and develop, for better or worse. Furthermore, more research is needed for a comprehensive understanding of the relationships between early specialisation and other variables, such as injury, burnout and identity.

Early specialisation among Swedish aesthetic performers

Identifying a trend towards increased early specialisation in aesthetic activities is particu-larly interesting from the Swedish perspective. The Swedish Sports Confederation (Riksi-drottsförbundet) has consistently held a strong position against early specialisation. Their guidelines stress the importance of letting children participate in different sports and that the child’s all-round sporting development is prioritised (Riksidrottsförbundet,2019). Moreover, youth sport in Sweden must be organised from a child rights’ perspective which means that sport should be conducted in the best interest of the child. Despite this, it is possible to begin training in aesthetic activities from a young age, and we question whether later specialisation is possible within the context of Swedish aesthetic sports. For instance, do clubs provide beginner-level training also for teenagers? In the present study, the relatively large standard deviation shows that there is variation in the degree of early specialisation exhibited by high-level Swedish aesthetic performers, but the relatively high mean score also suggests that many of them can still be considered early specialisers. There continues to be a debate about how early specialisation is conceptualised and measured (Mosher et al., 2020). This debate is perhaps especially complicated within the Swedish context where children sample activities as part of compulsory education. In this study, only organised sport outside of compulsory education were included as “sampled” activities. As such, even those scoring the highest possible scores for degrees of early specialisation will have had some experience of other sports and physical activities. Furthermore, instances of intra-sport diversification were identified. For example, it was fairly commonplace forfigure skaters to take part in dance training as a means to strengthen physical or artistic skills. Future research is needed to explore the definition of sampling within sports in general, and specifically in aesthetic sport where intra-sport diversification appears be prevalent.

Strengths, limitations and future research

Firstly, measuring early specialisation has been identified as problematic (DiSanti & Erick-son,2019; Downing et al.,2020; Mosher et al.,2020). Here we adopted an index approach, which is a new way to capture early specialisation. Although this definition-driven measurement tool allows exploration of more nuanced research questions, it is not without limitations (e.g., issues identifying what should/should not be included as part of training history). It is important to note that due to the use of a new measurement approach, the results of this paper are not easily compared to previous research.

Another limitation of this study is its retrospective cross-sectional design. This is not a unique limitation; in fact, the majority of previous research on early specialisation has

(17)

utilised such designs (DiSanti & Erickson,2019). While training history and specialisation were captured retrospectively, motivation and parental influence were captured cross-sectionally. Therefore, we cannot ascertain whether the degree of autonomous and con-trolled motivation is consistent throughout the early specialisation process, and previous research has shown that autonomous motivation for sport may develop over time (e.g., Deci & Ryan, 2000; Vink et al., 2015). It is also important to highlight that parental influence, too, may change over time. It is understandably complex to capture early specialisation longitudinally due to the vast age range over which it can occur (i.e., any-thing≤12 years is considered “early”; LaPrade et al.,2016). However, future researchers should utilise longitudinal research designs to better understand the process, and impli-cations, of early specialisation.

A notable strength of this paper is the sampled population. Much of the previous research exploring early specialisation has sampled non-aesthetic athletes, such as soccer and hockey players (DiSanti & Erickson, 2019). As such, this paper provides a unique contribution to thefield by exploring early specialisation within a sample of aes-thetic performers. This research is particularly topical as early specialisation has previously been suggested as necessary and even beneficial in aesthetic activities (e.g., Coutinho et al.,2016; Côté et al.,2007; Jayanthi et al.,2013; LaPrade et al.,2016). This study, there-fore, provides a foundation for larger and more sophisticated studies to follow. Impor-tantly, the assumption that aesthetic performers must begin intensive training early has been described as a socially constructed norm or tradition rather than a scientific fact (Barker-Ruchti,2009). Within the context of women’s artistic gymnastics, the tradition of early specialised training is believed to have originated from the Olympic success of Russian and Romanian gymnasts in the 1970s, which has since been widely adopted inter-nationally (Barker-Ruchti,2009). There is no known empirical evidence stating that start-ing gymnastics from a young age, with intensive trainstart-ing, and retirstart-ing early, is the only legitimate pathway towards expertise within the sport. As such, future researchers may want to explore not only the potential motivational, and other, outcomes of early special-isation, but also unpick how this tradition of early intensive training has been strongly rooted in modern society.

As aesthetic activities are typically female dominated, it was somewhat expected to recruit a greater number of female participants, despite significant attempts to identity males who met the recruitment criteria. Due to the ratio of female to male participants, it is unsuitable to speculate around gender differences in relation to early specialisation based on our results. Further research may wish to further explore the role of gender in early specialisation, particularly around ideas of prevalence and experiences of gender-specific specialised training.

Our results, which do not identify a negative relationship between degrees of early specialisation and motivation, are in opposition to the widely accepted Developmental Model of Sport Participation (DMSP; Côté et al., 2007). Our lack of support for the DMSP highlights some potential problems with the generalisability of the model to Swedish aesthetic activities. Researchers have also begun to highlight other areas of con-tention with the model in terms of the definition of “sampling” (Downing et al.,2020; Hen-riksen et al., 2010; Storm et al., 2012). Future research should continue to explore the suitability of generalised recommendations in relation to early specialisation.

(18)

Early specialisation is continuing to characterise high-level youth sport and dance, and therefore researchers must continue to unpick the potential consequences. One such direction may be via qualitative methods, whereby performer experiences of early special-isation can be voiced. Further research should also continue to consider moderators, or indeed mediators, in the relationship between early specialisation and various psycho-logical factors. This could be done with a variety of methods. For example, exploring the role of the training environment within early specialised training programs would make a valuable addition to the literature. If early specialisation does continue to happen within aesthetic activities, we need to build a solid foundation of empirical evi-dence to help us decide whether to work with (i.e., exploring ways to minimise risks and enhance potential benefits) or against (i.e., suggest workable alternatives) this tradition.

Conclusions

This is the first known study to explore associations between early specialisation and motivation, and potential moderating factors, in a large sample of aesthetic performers. No negative associations between early specialisation and indices of motivation were found. Such results are an important addition to early specialisation literature, as they provide further rationale to explore the nuances of early specialisation. Furthermore, the saturation of literature exploring the dichotomous narrative of early specialisation as either “good” or “bad”, is providing little new evidence to the research area. Future researchers are encouraged to move away from first generation research questions, and develop studies which explore moderating, and indeed mediating, variables to help understand the complex mechanisms behind the psychological correlates of early specialisation. In this study, exploring perceptions of parental autonomy support and control as moderators did not reveal, or change, any relationships between early special-isation and motivation. However, many other factors may impact the athlete experiences, or the potential outcomes, of early specialisation. Research questions addressing why early specialisation may have different outcomes for different athletes is a warranted endeavour for future research.

Disclosure statement

No potential conflict of interest was reported by the author(s).

Funding

This work was supported by The Swedish Research Council for Sport Science: [grant number P2017-0071].

ORCID

Charlotte Downing http://orcid.org/0000-0002-7937-5918

Karin Redelius http://orcid.org/0000-0001-9965-0123

(19)

References

Adie, J. W., Duda, J. L., & Ntoumanis, N. (2008). Autonomy support, basic need satisfaction and the optimal functioning of adult male and female sport participants: A test of basic needs theory. Motivation and Emotion, 32(3), 189–199.https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-008-9095-z

Aujla, I. J., Nordin-Bates, S., & Redding, E. (2014). A qualitative investigation of commitment to dance: Findings from the UK centres for advanced training. Research in Dance Education, 15(2), 138–160.

https://doi.org/10.1080/14647893.2013.825764

Baker, J., Mosher, A., & Fraser-Thomas, J. (2021). Is it too early to condemn early sport specialisation? British Journal of Sports Medicine, 55(3), 179–180.https://doi.org/10.1136/bjsports-2020-102053

Baker, J., & Young, B. (2014). 20 years later: Deliberate practice and the development of expertise in sport. International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 7(1), 135–157. https://doi.org/10. 1080/1750984X.2014.896024

Barker-Ruchti, N. (2009). Ballerinas and pixies: A genealogy of the changing gymnastics body. The International Journal of the History of Sport, 26(1), 43–61. https://doi.org/10.1080/ 09523360802500089

Briggs, S. R., & Cheek, J. M. (1986). The role of factor analysis in the development and evaluation of personality scales. Journal of Personality, 54(1), 106–148. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494. 1986.tb00391.x

Carlsson, B., Norberg, J. R., & Persson, H. T. R. (2011). The governance of sport from a Scandinavian perspective. International Journal of Sport Policy and Politics, 3(3), 305–309. https://doi.org/10. 1080/19406940.2011.619827

Coakley, J. (2006). The good father: Parental expectations and youth sports. Leisure Studies, 25(2), 153–163.https://doi.org/10.1080/02614360500467735

Coakley, J. (2010). The“logic” of specialization: Using children for adult purposes. ASCA Newsletter, 2013(4), 1–6. doi:https://doi.org/10.1080/07303084.2010.10598520

Costa, S., Cuzzocrea, F., Gugliandolo, M. C., & Larcan, R. (2016). Associations between parental psychological control and autonomy support, and psychological outcomes in adolescents: The mediating role of need satisfaction and need frustration. Child Indicators Research, 9(4), 1059– 1076.https://doi.org/10.1007/s12187-015-9353-z

Côté, J., Baker, J., & Abernethy, B. (2007). Practice and play in the development of sport expertise. In G. Tenenbaum, & R. C. Eklund (Eds.), Handbook of sport psychology (Vol. 3, pp. 184–202). John Wiley & Sons Inc.

Côté, J., Lidor, R., & Hackfort, D. (2009). ISSP position stand: To sample or to specialize? Seven pos-tulates about youth sport activities that lead to continued participation and elite performance. International Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 7(1), 7–17. https://doi.org/10.1080/ 1612197X.2009.9671889

Côté, J., & Vierimaa , M. (2014). The developmental model of sport participation: 15 years after its first conceptualization. Science & Sports, 29S, S63–S69.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.scispo.2014.08. 133

Coutinho, P., Mesquita, I., & Fonseca, A. M. (2016). Talent development in sport: A critical review of pathways to expert performance. International Journal of Sports Science & Coaching, 11(2), 279– 293.https://doi.org/10.1177/1747954116637499

Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in human behavior. Springer Science & Business Media.

Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). The‘what’ and ‘why’ of goal pursuits: Human needs and the self-determination of behavior. Psychological Inquiry, 11(4), 227–268. https://doi.org/10.1207/ S15327965PLI1104_01

DeVellis, R. F. (2016). Scale development: Theory and applications (Vol. 26). Sage Publications. DiSanti, J. S., & Erickson, K. (2019). Youth sport specialization: A multidisciplinary scoping systematic

review. Journal of Sports Sciences, 37(18), 2094–2105.https://doi.org/10.1080/02640414.2019.1621476

Downing, C., Redelius, K., & Nordin-Bates, S. M. (2020). An index approach to early specialization measurement: An exploratory study. Frontiers in Movement Science and Sport Psychology, 11 (999), 1–9.https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00999

(20)

Elbe, A. M., & Wikman, J. M. (2017). Psychological factors in developing high performance athletes. In J. Baker, S. Cobley, J. Schorer, & N. Wattie (Eds.), Routledge handbook of talent identification and development in sport (1st ed., pp. 169–180). Taylor & Francis.

Eliasson, I. (2015).“In different sports worlds”: Socialisation among children, coaches, and parents in girls’ and boys’ football teams. European Journal for Sport and Society, 12(2), 187–214.https://doi. org/10.1080/16138171.2015.11687962

Eliasson, I., & Johansson, A. (2020). The disengagement process among young athletes when with-drawing from sport: A new research approach. International Review for the Sociology of Sport, 56 (4), 537–557.https://doi.org/1012690219899614

Ericsson, K. A., Krampe, R. T., & Tesch-Römer, C. (1993). The role of deliberate practice in the acqui-sition of expert performance. Psychological Review, 100(3), 363–406. https://doi.org/10.1037/ 0033-295X.100.3.363

Fahlström, P. G., Gerrevall, P., Glemne, M., & Linnér, S. (2015). Vägarna till landslaget: Om svenska eli-tidrottares idrottsval och specialisering [The roads to the national team: About Swedish elite athletes’ choices and specialisation]. (Vol. 1). FoU-rapport.

Fenton, S. A., Duda, J. L., & Barrett, T. (2016). Optimising physical activity engagement during youth sport: A self-determination theory approach. Journal of Sports Sciences, 34(19), 1874–1884.

https://doi.org/10.1080/02640414.2016.1142104

Ferguson, Y. L., Kasser, T., & Jahng, S. (2011). Differences in life satisfaction and school satisfaction among adolescents from three nations: The role of perceived autonomy support. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 21(3), 649–661.https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-7795.2010.00698.x

Ford, P., & William, A. M. (2017). Sport activity in childhood. Early specialization and diversification. In J. Baker, S. Cobley, J. Schorer, & N. Wattie (Eds.), Routledge Handbook of Talent Identification and Development in sport (1st ed., pp. 179–201). Routledge.

Gagné, M. (2003). Autonomy support and need satisfaction in the motivation and well-being of gymnasts. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 15(4), 372–390.https://doi.org/10.1080/714044203

Gould, D. (2010). Early sport specialization: A psychological perspective. Journal of Physical Education, Recreation & Dance (JOPERD), 81(8), 33–37.https://doi.org/10.1080/07303084.2010.10598525

Gould, D., Lauer, L., Rolo, C., Jannes, C., & Pennisi, N. (2008). The role of parents in tennis success: Focus group interviews with junior coaches. Sport Psychologist, 22(1), 18–37.https://doi.org/10. 1123/tsp.22.1.18

Guillet, E., Sarrazin, P., Fontayne, P., & Brustad, R. (2006). Understanding Female Sport Attrition in a Stereotypical Male Sport Within the Framework of Eccles’s Expectancy–Value Model. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 30(4), 358–368.https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1471-6402.2006.00311.x

Hagger, M., Chatzisarantis, N. L., Hein, V., Soos, I., Karsai, I., Lintunen, T., & Leemans, S. (2009). Teacher, peer and parent autonomy support in physical education and leisure-time physical activity: A trans-contextual model of motivation in four nations. Psychology and Health, 24(6), 689–711.

https://doi.org/10.1080/08870440801956192

Haraldsen, H. M., Nordin-Bates, S. M., Abrahamsen, F. E., & Halvari, H. (2020). Thriving, striving, or just surviving? TD learning conditions, motivational processes and well-being among Norwegian elite performers in music, ballet, and sport. Roeper Review, 42(2), 109–125.https://doi.org/10.1080/ 02783193.2020.1728796

Hayes, A. F. (2018). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis second edition: A regression-based approach. Guilford Press.

Henriksen, K., Stambulova, N., & Roessler, K. (2010). Holistic approach to athletic talent development environments: A successful sailing milieu. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 11(3), 212–222.https:// doi.org/10.1016/j.psychsport.2009.10.005

Hume, P. A., Hopkins, W. G., Robinson, D. M., Robinson, S. M., & Hollings, S. C. (1993). Predictors of attainment in rhythmic sportive gymnastics. Journal of Sports Medicine & Physical Fitness, 33(4), 367–377.

IBM Corp. Released. (2019). IBM SPSS statistics for windows, version 26.0. IBM Corporation.

Jayanthi, N., Pinkham, C., Dugas, L., Patrick, B., & LaBella, C. (2013). Sports specialization in young athletes: Evidence-based recommendations. Sports Health: A Multidisciplinary Approach, 5(3), 251–257.https://doi.org/10.1177/1941738112464626

(21)

Kliethermes, S. A., Marshall, S. W., LaBella, C. R., Watson, A. M., Brenner, J. S., Nagle, K. B., Jayanthi, N., Brooks, M. A., Tenforde, A. S., Herman, D. C., DiFiori, J. P., & Beutlers, A. I. (2021). Defining a research agenda for youth sport specialisation in the USA: The AMSSM youth early sport special-ization summit. British Journal of Sports Medicine, 55(3), 135–143. https://doi.org/10.1136/ bjsports-2020-102699

Knight, C. J. (2017). Family influences on talent development in sport. In J. Baker, S. Cobley, J. Schorer, & N. Wattie (Eds.), Routledge Handbook of Talent Identification and Development in sport (1st ed., pp. 181–191). Routledge.

LaPrade, R. F., Agel, J., Baker, J., Brenner, J. S., Cordasco, F. A., Côté, J., Engebretsen, L., Feeley, B. T., Gould, D., Hainline, B., Hewett, T. E., Jayanthi, N., Kocher, M. S., Myer, G. D., Nissen, C. W., Philippon, M. J., & Provencher, M. T. (2016). AOSSM early sport specialization consensus statement. The Orthopaedic Journal of Sport Medicine, 4(4), 1–9.https://doi.org/10.1177/2325967116644241

Larson, H. K., Young, B. W., McHugh, T.-L. F., & Rodgers, W. M. (2019). Markers of early specialization and their relationships with burnout and dropout in swimming. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 41(1), 46–54.https://doi.org/10.1123/jsep.2018-0305

Law, M. P., Côté, J., & Ericsson, K. A. (2007). Characteristics of expert development in rhythmic gym-nastics: A retrospective study. International Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 5(1), 82–103.

https://doi.org/10.1080/1612197X.2008.9671814

Le Bars, H, Gernigon, C., & Ninot, G. (2009). Personal and contextual determinants of elite young ath-letes’ persistence or dropping out over time. Scandinavian Journal of Medicine & Science in Sports, 19(2), 274–285.https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0838.2008.00786.x

Lonsdale, C., Hodge, K., & Rose, E. A. (2008). The Behavioral Regulation in Sport Questionnaire (BRSQ): Instrument development and initial validity evidence. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 30(3), 323–355.https://doi.org/10.1123/jsep.30.3.323

Mageau, G. A., Bureau, J. S., Ranger, F., Allen, M.-P., & Soenens, B. (2016). The role of parental achieve-ment goals in predicting autonomy-supportive and controlling parenting. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 25(5), 1702–1711.https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-015-0341-1

Mageau, G. A., Ranger, F., Joussemet, M., Koestner, R., Moreau, E., & Forest, J. (2015). Validation of the perceived parental autonomy support scale (P-PASS). Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science, 47 (3), 251–262.https://doi.org/10.1037/a0039325

Malina, R. M. (2010). Early sport specialization: Roots, effectiveness, risks. Current Sports Medicine Reports (American College of Sports Medicine), 9(6), 364–371. https://doi.org/10.1249/JSR. 0b013e3181fe3166

Martindale, R., Collins, D., & Daubney, J. (2005). Talent Development: A Guide for Practice and Research Within Sport. Quest, 57(4), 353–375.https://doi.org/10.1080/00336297.2005.10491862

Moesch, K., Hauge, M.-L. T., Wikman, J. M., & Elbe, A.-M. (2013). Making it to the top in team sports: Start later, intensify, and be determined!. Talent Development & Excellence, 5(2), 85–100. Mosher, A., Fraser-Thomas, J., & Baker, J. (2020). What defines early specialization: A systematic

review of literature. Frontiers in Sports and Active Living, 2(164), 1–23.https://doi.org/10.3389/ fspor.2020.596229

Patel, T., & Jayanthi, N. (2018). Health-related quality of life of specialized versus multi-sport young athletes: A qualitative evaluation. Journal of Clinical Sport Psychology, 12(3), 448–466.https://doi. org/10.1123/jcsp.2017-0031

Riksidrottsförbundet. (2019). Idrotten vill– idrottsrörelsens idéprogram [The sports will – The sport movement’s idea program].

Robinson, T. T., & Carron, A. V. (1982). Personal and situational factors associated with dropping out versus maintaining participation in competitive sport. Journal of Sport Psychology, 4(4), 364–378.

https://doi.org/10.1123/jsp.4.4.364

Ronglan, L. T. (2015). Elite sport in Scandinavian welfare states: Legitimacy under pressure? International Journal of Sport Policy and Politics, 7(3), 345–363. https://doi.org/10.1080/ 19406940.2014.987309

Russell, W., Dodd, R., & Lee, M. (2017). Youth athletes’ sport motivation and physical activity enjoy-ment across specialization status. Journal of Contemporary Athletics, 11(2), 83–95.

(22)

Russell, W., & Symonds, M. (2015). A retrospective examination of youth athletes’ sport motivation and motivational climate across specialization status. Athletic Insight, 7(1), 33–46.

Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being. American Psychologist, 55(1), 68–78.https://doi.org/10.1037/ 0003-066X.55.1.68

Ryska, T., Hohenee, D., Cooley, D., & Jones, C. (2002). Participation motives in predicting sport dropout among Australian youth gymnasts. North American Journal of Psychology, 4(2), 199–210. Salguero, A., Gonzalez-Boto, R., Tuero, C., & Marquez, S. (2003). Identification of dropout reasons in

young competitive swimmers. Journal of Sports Medicine and Physical Fitness, 43(4), 530–534. Sjöblom, P., & Fahlén, J. (2010). The survival of thefittest: intensification, totalization and

homogen-ization in Swedish competitive sport. Sport in Society, 13(4), 704–717.https://doi.org/10.1080/ 17430431003616514

Stenling, A., Ivarsson, A., Lindwall, M., & Gucciardi, D. F. (2018). Exploring longitudinal measurement invariance and the continuum hypothesis in the Swedish version of the Behavioral Regulation in Sport Questionnaire (BRSQ): An exploratory structural equation modeling approach. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 36, 187–196.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychsport.2018.03.002

Storm, L., Heniksen, K., & Christensen, M. (2020). Specialization pathways among elite Danish ath-letes: A look at the developmental model of sport participation from a cultural perspective. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 43(3), 199–222.https://doi.org/10.7352/IJSP.2012.43.199

Vallerand, R. J., & Bissonnette, R. (1992). Intrinsic, extrinsic, and amotivational styles as predictors of behavior: A prospective study. Journal of Personality, 60(3), 599–620.https://doi.org/10.1111/j. 1467-6494.1992.tb00922.x

Vink, K., Raudsepp, L., & Kais, K. (2015). Intrinsic motivation and individual deliberate practice are reciprocally related: Evidence from a longitudinal study of adolescent team sport athletes. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 16, 1–6.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychsport.2014.08.012

Waldron, S., DeFreese, J. D., Register-Mihalik, J., Pietrosimone, B., & Barczak, N. (2020). The costs and benefits of early sport specialization: A critical review of literature. Quest, 72(1), 1–18.https://doi. org/10.1080/00336297.2019.1580205

Wall, M., & Côté, J. (2007). Developmental activities that lead to dropout and investment in sport. Physical Education and Sport Pedagogy, 12(1), 77–87. https://doi.org/10.1080/ 17408980601060358

Wiersma, L. D. (2000). Risks and benefits of youth sport specialization: Perspectives and recommen-dations. Pediatric Exercise Science, 12(1), 13–22.https://doi.org/10.1123/pes.12.1.13

Zanna, M. P., & Fazio, R. (1982). The attitude behavior relation: Moving toward a third generation of research. In M. P. Zanna, E. T. Higgings, & C. P. Herman (Eds.), Consistency in social behavior: The Ontario symposium (Vol. 2, pp. 283–301). Erlbaum.

References

Related documents

The third research question about how is study motivation linked to various aspects of education according to teachers and students, is highlighted both in the theoretical

“på fältet” i skolan ​. Utgångspunkten för att skriva uppsatsen är att studera hur lärarna använder begreppet motivation i klassrummet. Begreppet motivation tycks vara

While there is an increasing interest in knowledge-intensive firms, there are relatively few studies that relate the working conditions of IT consultants to factors in

According to organizational justice theory, employees are going to be more motivated to perform at high levels when they perceive that the procedures used to make decisions

Framförallt för att enklare kunna koppla empirin till Hirdmans (1988) genusteori samt vår tredje frågeställning som är: Påverkas motivationen utav faktorn om det är en manlig

• To attract new customers or strengthen current customer relations.. • To attract

För att skilja ut sambandet mellan autonomi och förväntad medellivslängd från den variation som kan förklaras av graden av demokrati bör detta fungera väl, men det kan även

Det är dock svårt att i efterhand rekonstruera och belägga att doktrinförfattarna medvetet och i specifika fall har använt sig av Linds teorier om