• No results found

Theory of Mind and cognition in individuals with hearing impairment : - A Pilot Study

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "Theory of Mind and cognition in individuals with hearing impairment : - A Pilot Study"

Copied!
26
0
0

Loading.... (view fulltext now)

Full text

(1)

       

Örebro University

School of Medicine

Medicine C, Degree Project, 15 ECTS

May 2014

Theory of Mind and cognition in individuals with

hearing impairment

- A Pilot Study

Author: Anna Dimic

Supervisors: Claes Möller, MD, PhD and Professor

Hans-Erik Frölander, Psychologist, PhD student

Örebro University

(2)

Table of Contents

Abstract ... 3 Abbreviations ... 4 Introduction ... 5 Objective ... 8 Hypothesis ... 8 Question formulation ... 9

Material and Methods ... 9

Ethical considerations ... 9

Participants ... 9

Hearing tests ... 10

Measures and scoring ... 10

Measure of phonological working memory ... 10

Measure of verbal ability ... 11

Measure of executive functions ... 11

Measure of advanced ToM ... 12

Procedure ... 13 Data analyses ... 13 Results ... 13 Discussion ... 17 Conclusion ... 22 Acknowledgements ... 23 References ... 23  

(3)

Abstract

 

Introduction: Understanding the mental state of others is an ability known as Theory

of Mind (ToM) that is required for successful social interactions. To put oneself in someone else’s mental shoes and imagine what they think and what they believe is an aptitude that becomes fully developed around 4 years of age. Previous studies testing ToM in children indicate that children’s view of how minds work is distinguished from the view an adult would have. However, delays in false belief understanding have been reported in deaf children, which has encouraged researchers to try to elucidate if an auditory deprivation possibly may be the reason to this delay.

Furthermore, correlations between certain executive functions have been suggested to be indispensable for prosperity on false belief tasks. Moreover, language is proposed to be a critical factor in the ability to deliberate about other people’s minds.

Objective: This present study aims to investigate the ToM-ability as well as the

phonological working memory, executive functioning and verbal ability in individuals with congenital or early-acquired hearing impairment (N=7), ages 20 to 40 years old, and to compare their results with a normal hearing control group (N=20).

Methods: Seven sets of dependable tasks were performed by the participants,

including the Strange Stories-test first introduced by Happé (1994), measuring the ToM-ability. Moreover, phonological and verbal skills were tested as well as executive functioning.

Results and conclusions: The results revealed a significant difference between the

two groups where the control group outperformed the hearing impaired group in almost every single item. This indicates that a hearing impairment may consequently lead to a poorer ToM and that adequate hearing could play a significant role in successfully interacting with other people.

Keywords: Theory of Mind, cognitive skills, hearing impairment.

(4)

Abbreviations

 

ToM Theory of Mind

HI Hearing impairment or hearing impaired

WM Working memory

WMC Working memory capacity

EF Executive function

dB Decibel

HL Hearing loss

CSC Cognitive spare capacity

(5)

Introduction

Understanding the mental state of others is an ability required for successful social interactions. This ability, called Theory of Mind (ToM), is essential in order to live in bonded social groups (1) and undergoes a rapid development during the preschool years in most of the children (2). A deficiency in ToM may consequently contribute to difficulties in everyday life (1). Virtually all children in the age of three fail to

explicitly comprehend how human’s personal manners are moderated by intentions, desires, frame of minds, memories and beliefs (2,3). However, deficiencies in ToM have been reported in groups of people with different types of disabilities including autism, Multiple Sclerosis (MS) and deafness (1,4-7).

A newly discovered class of neurons, called mirror neurons, were initially found in the ventral premotor cortex (area F5) of the macaque monkey (8,9) and these were activated when the monkey performed a given action as well as when it observed an action similar to the one it had performed itself (10). According to Kohler et al. (11) there are also neurons in the monkey premotor cortex that discharge when the monkey hears a certain sound related to a specific action. Hence these neurons, called audio-visual mirror neurons, also located in the area F5 of the macaque monkey, fire independently of the monkey observing or performing the action correlated to the characteristic sound. With the help of functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), researchers have discovered an equivalent of these mirror neurons in humans. These neurons of the human brain are centered in Broca’s area, located within the ventral premotor cortex. A somatotopically-organized activation of the premotor cortex was determined when subjects were observing object related and non-object related activities, i.e., different regions of the premotor cortex were activated dependently on whether the individual observed hand, foot or mouth actions (12). Gazzola et al. (13) studied empathy and the somatotopic auditory mirror system in humans. They discovered the activation of an area, more exactly the left hemispheric temporo-parieto-premotor circuit in humans while performing motor execution and when letting the individuals listen to the sound of that performance. This supports the assumption of humans possessing an auditory mirror neuron system. Other somatotopically activated brain areas were also discovered. These areas were activated dependently on what type of action that was observed or performed by the

(6)

individuals themselves. Moreover, they found that individuals who performed better on an empathy scale exhibited a stronger activation in these specific brain areas. This provides evidence for a potential association between empathy and the motor mirror system.

The mirror neuron system has been suggested to be important for understanding other individuals (11,13,14) and is believed to integrate visual, motor and auditory

stimulation, hence called the trimodal mirror neurons or system (15,16). The

hypothesis of these modalities working together has encouraged researchers to try to shed light on what influence the auditory system has on the mirror neuron system (16).

Schick et al. (4) studied ToM capabilities in 176 Deaf children, ages three to eight years, that use either oral English or American Sign Language (ASL), with Deaf or hearing parents. The researchers intention was to examine if language potentially could have a role in ToM development. Schick et al. propose that a critical factor in a Deaf child’s ability to deliberate about the mind is language, and their study indicates a substantial delay in ToM in Deaf children of hearing parents. Consequently, this discovery suggests that insufficiencies in the mirror neuron system developmentin Deafness could possibly underlie deficits in ToM development in Deaf children. However, the specific role of language in the development of ToM remains ambiguous (4).

A more thorough description of ToM is the ability to impute mental states such as feelings, thoughts and beliefs to oneself and to other individuals (7). Nonetheless, these beliefs are not simply copies of the world, only representations of it, and to be able to register them as representations, one needs to comprehend the beliefs as exemplifications of the world, and that they, per se, can be wrong. This has led to the acceptance of false belief understanding being a fundamental marker of ToM (17). In normally developing children, the attainment of realistic understanding of mind takes place at around four years of age (7). However, as mentioned before, a delay in false belief understanding has been reported in Deaf children (4,5) and autistic children (7,18).

(7)

Executive functions (EFs) denote a group of mental processes required in situations when you have to pay attention and concentrate. They are also essential for being able to prevent us from making indiscreet impulsive acts, resisting temptations and

meeting unexpected challenges. It is believed that these cognitive functions are governed from the prefrontal cortex (19). Benson et al. (20) submit that by facilitating experiential learning, executive functions, more precise response conflict executive function (RC-EF), i.e. withhold impulses, promote the development in ToM. In their study they demonstrated that there is a correlation between pre-schoolers’ RC-EF skill level and to what degree their false belief performance could improve subsequent to RC-EF training.

It is generic that an inadequate ToM is a trait of Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD) (7,21) and the development of language in autism has been revealed to be closely associated to ToM as well as other cognitive functions (7). However, as mentioned earlier, the ability to understand other minds is fully developed around 4 years of age, and it has been postulated that a key role in ToM development for all children at this stage may be played by specific comprehension of the structure of complement constructions (7). In their investigation about what influence language has on the establishment of ToM, Hale and Tager-Flusberg (22) could confirm that by training on certain linguistic constructions, viz. sentential complements, leads to increased scores on a range of ToM tasks.

Another aspect of the executive functioning that has been demonstrated to contribute to the development of ToM is the working-memory capacity (WMC) (23), which refers to the ability to maintain and manipulate information over a short period of time in order to achieve tasks or reach a goal (24). Slade and Ruffman (25) report that there are some uncertainties over whether learning about false belief could be facilitated by ameliorations in working memory (WM) in children. However, they contend that a certain degree of WM may be indispensable for prosperity on false belief tasks. In contrast to what Hale and Tager-Flusberg (22) confirmed, Slade and Ruffman submit that it is general language, i.e. both syntax and semantics, which associates and contributes to later ToM.

(8)

Baddeley and Hitch (26) suggested in 1974 that the WM could be distributed in three subsystems, one concerning the acoustic and verbal information, namely the

phonological loop and another visio-spatial component supplying the visual

correspondent. The third constituent, whom these two other constituents are reliant on, is the central executive, which is a central unit controlling the WM. A fourth subsystem was later suggested, given the name the episodic buffer. The episodic buffer stores information temporary and integrates information from the three other subsystems and the long-term memory in order to engender wholeness (27).

There are three executive functions (EF), shifting, inhibition and updating that have been proposed as significant (28-30). Shifting, also known as “attention switching”, implicates the shift between mental states or tasks e.g. disengage an immaterial operation and instead actively concentrate on the succeeding relevant one. Inhibition concerns an individual’s ability to intentionally constrain involuntary, dominant or predominant responses when necessary. Updating is a function necessary to monitor and code information that is of relevance for the task and subsequently revises the information being held in working memory. This revision refers to replacing information no longer relevant, with more significant, newer information (28). Objective  

 

The aims were to examine ToM in individuals, ages 20 to 40 years, with congenital or early-acquired hearing impairment and to explore possible correlations between ToM andexecutive functions and verbal ability. We tested these abilities in seven hearing impaired individuals to see if we could find any correlations between these

capabilities and their ToM-ability. This was accomplished with the help of totally seven tests described in the method section.

Hypothesis

The hypothesis for this study is that a congenital or early-acquired hearing impairment could lead to a deficiency in ToM in adults, i.e. that the development in ToM requires adequate auditory processing. Furthermore, another supposition is that the

(9)

Question formulation  

Is Theory of Mind and cognition in individuals with hearing impairment, ages 20 to 40 years, comparable to Theory of Mind and cognition in normal hearing persons?

Material and Methods

Ethical considerations  

This study was approved by the Regional Ethical Board (08/11). The participant’s names and date of birth was coded in numerical series. Only me, my supervisors Claes Möller and Hans Erik Frölander had access to the code key. Data is stored in a control server – Audiological Research Centre, University Hospital Örebro. The participants were not subjected to any risks and all the tests were implemented at the hospital. If the attendants wish to take part of the results, they are permitted to do that. Participants

 

Seven young adults, three men and four females, with congenital or early-acquired hearing impairment between the ages of 20 and 40 years participated in the study. They all had a moderate to severe bilateral symmetrical sensory neural hearing loss (HL), ranging from 30-70 dB, which implied them wearing a hearing device on both ears (see table 1). All participants had Swedish as their first language. The

participants did not have any additional disability such as syndromes, severe visual impairment etc.

The reference population comprised 20 individuals, nine men and eleven females. There was no difference in the distribution of gender between the group of hearing impaired individuals and the reference groups.

There was no significant difference in mean age between the two groups. However, the two groups differed slightly in terms of educational level defined by years of schooling, viz. the control group had altogether a higher educational level.

Educational level did not relate to ToM performance, neither in the group of hearing impaired individuals, nor in the control group.

(10)

Table 1. Audiological data

Sex Age PTA4 Degree of HL

1 Male 28 Right ear 60 dB, left ear 60 dB

Moderate to severe bilateral symmetrical sensorineural HL

2 Female 22 Right ear 36 dB, left ear 34 dB

Moderate bilateral symmetrical sensorineural HL

3 Female 38 Right ear 53 dB, left ear 50 dB

Moderate to severe bilateral symmetrical sensorineural HL

4 Female 22 Right ear 60 dB, left ear 63 dB

Moderate to severe bilateral symmetrical sensorineural HL

5 Male 26 Right ear 43 dB, left ear 45 dB

Moderate bilateral symmetrical sensorineural HL

6 Female 36 Right ear 61 dB, left ear 61 dB

Moderate to severe bilateral sensorineural HL

7 Male 31 Right ear 74 dB, left ear 68 dB

Severe bilateral symmetrical sensorineural HL

Hearing tests    

Pure tone audiometry was performed at the Audiological Research Centre, University Hospital, Örebro by a licensed audiologist, prior to the cognitive and ToM tests. The pure tone audiometry included air and bone conduction tests, which were done according to the ascending or modified Hughson-Westlake method. Frequencies between 0,25 and 8 kHz were tested (31).

Measures and scoring

Measure of phonological working memory

 

Phonological working memory was evaluated with the Serial Recall of words and non-words-test (32). At first, sequences of real words were presented orally. Initially, the words were presented in series of three in three sets, and subsequently in series of four and five words. The same approach was practised with the non-words. The participants were asked to repeat each non-word and each real word sequence immediately after the words had been presented. The answers were recorded on an

(11)

external tape recorder to facilitate later scoring of correctness. Three different criteria were taken into consideration when evaluating the accuracy of the participants’ repeated words, the percentage of correctly repeated words and non-words out of the total amount of words and non-words, respectively, and segmental accuracy; the percentage of correct reproduced consonants out of the total number of consonants presented in the test and supra-segmental accuracy; the proportion of words

pronounced with the correct vowel and total number of syllables, e.g. if the individual reproduced the vowel and syllable lengths of the word (despite the contingent fact that consonants could be incorrectly reproduced) correctly, they earned one point for that specific word.

Measure of verbal ability

 

To evaluate the participant’s verbal ability, the vocabulary subtest in the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (3rd ed.) (WAIS-III) was used. The participants were asked to explain the meaning of words. For each word, they could score two, one or zero points depending on the accuracy of the explanation. The total score, ranging from 0-66 points, was calculated by combining the score of each word.

Measure of executive functions

 

Assessment of the participant’s executive functions was done by means of two tests named “Odd and Man” and “Highest”. “Odd and Man” engaged the individuals’ ability to advisedly inhibit certain stimuli, known as inhibition (30). Sequences of two-digit numbers were expressed by a male and female-recorded voice on a

computer. Each sequence consisted of 13 numbers. The participants were told to keep two odd numbers in mind, which the male mediated, and disregard the odd numbers the female mediated. Three different sequences of 13 numbers were played back and immediately after each of these three, the participants were told to repeat the odd numbers the male voice enounced. The participants got a score for each correct number they reported; two points per sequence, totally six points for all three sequences.

“Highest” engaged the participants’ aptitude to monitor and code incoming

information as well as testing their ability to properly adjust items held in working memory by substituting information, no longer relevant, with new and more relevant

(12)

such (30). The participants executed two subtests of the test “Highest” designated

“Updating low load” and “Updating high load”. Again, the subjects were listening to

recorded voices expressing three sequences of two-digit numbers, each sequence consisting of 13 numbers. In “Updating low load”, the participants were told to remember the two highest numbers in the particular sequence. Immediately after each of the sequences, subjects were told to retail the two highest numbers in the particular sequence. “Updating high load” was performed in the same way. However, subjects were no instructed to, in addition to the two highest numbers, remember the first number in the particular sequence. “Updating low load” was graded in like manner as the “Odd and Man”-test. However, the first number in the “Updating high load”-test was not included in the awarding of points, since it is considered being “a dummy corporation” and therefore solely the correct reproductions of the highest numbers were graded.

Measure of advanced ToM

 

ToM was assessed by a set of social stories from Happé (1994) advanced test of ToM – the strange stories (3). The subjects were presented eight ToM-stories that were read one by one. If necessary, subjects were offered one more verbal presentation. A “why question” was asked after each presented story to examine the individual’s attribution of mental states, including desires, beliefs and intentions (18). Subsequently the question was scored as correct or incorrect. A correct implication could either concern a correct physical implication or a correct mental implication or vice versa, an

incorrect physical implication or an incorrect mental implication, if regarding an incorrect implication. The correct inference was scored if both a correct and incorrect inference was given. Moreover, if both a physical and a mental conclusion were given, the mental conclusion was scored. Physical state inferences correlated with descriptions such as “to get rid of” or “to get more”. Mental state attributions referred to thoughts, feelings, desires, traits and dispositions comprising terms such as “think”, “know”, “like”, “afraid” and “lie”. The participants were scored with 2 points if they indicated the exact mental state; 1 point if they retailed a more general answer or an incomplete answer, and 0 if the answer did not correlate at all with the mental or physical inference. The score for the ToM-stories ranged from 0-16.

(13)

Procedure  

Testing took place in a quiet room. The three cognitive tests and the ToM-test took altogether 45 minutes to execute. The whole procedure was recorded to facilitate scoring after the test was completed.

Data analyses  

SPSS (Statistical Package for Social Science) was used for statistical analyses. T-tests were performed to show group differences. Spearman’s rho: nonparametric test of correlation was used to show correlations. A significance level of p< 0,05 was employed.

Results

 

The results that are about to be presented revealed significant differences between the control group and hearing impaired group. The descriptive data exhibited an

interesting pattern, which is most explicit in figure 1. The control group performed better in almost every single item.

An established significant difference between the control group (N = 20) and the group of individuals with HI (N = 7) was the one regarding the serial recall of non-words task, where the control group outperformed the HI group. A substantial disparity in the ability to reproduce non-words emerged (see fig 1 and table 2). Furthermore, a difference in the ability to reproduce correct consonants of the non-words between the control group and HI-group was found (see fig 1 and table 2) as well as the ability to reproduce correct vowels and correct number of syllables of the non-words (see fig 1 and table 2).

The second analysis examined possible forthcoming disparities in vocabulary. A difference was discovered between the hearing impaired group and the control group (see fig 1 and table 2). The control group outperformed the HI group.

Concerning the executive function tests, no significant difference appeared between the control group and HI-group in inhibition (see fig 1 and table 2) and updating low load (see fig 1 and table 2). However, a difference in updating high load was revealed

(14)

between the two groups, where the control group performed better than the HI group (see fig 1 and table 2).

However, no correlations between any of the ToM-measures and the executive functions were found within the group of HI individuals. To emphasize this, no correlation was found between ToM-ability and EF, including inhibition, updating low load and updating high load, within the HI-group. Neither could a correlation between correct ToM mental judgement and EF be found.

Moreover, an evaluation considering differences between the individuals with hearing impairment (HI) and the normal hearing control group was done, regarding their ability to impute mental states as well as correct such, and the frequency of giving physical inferences within the two groups.

A comparison between the control group and the group of individuals with HI displayed a significant difference between the groups in ToM total score (see fig 1 and table 2) and mental judgements (see fig 1 and table 2). The control group performed better than the HI group. However, no significant differences in correct mental judgements between the groups were found (see fig 1 and table 2).

Physical judgements were significantly more frequent within the group of individuals with HI than in the control group (see fig 1 and table 2).

(15)

Figure 1. Descriptive statistic for age, educational level, phonological working

memory task, executive functions, ToM-ability and verbal ability. The figure shows the mean percentage values in all tests.

(16)

Table 2. This table shows the same means as displayed in fig 1 one + standard

deviation and standard error of mean.

N Mean Std. Deviation Std. Error of Mean Vocabulary (%) Hearing impaired group 7 49,00 15,748 5,952 Control group 20 68,35 10,624 2,376 ToM stories total points (%) Hearing impaired group 7 86,71 10,515 3,974 Control group 20 96,95 6,770 1,514 ToM stories mental judgement (%) Hearing impaired group 7 82,29 14,092 5,326 Control group 20 99,10 2,936 ,657 ToM story physical judgement (%) Hearing impaired group 7 18,00 14,259 5,390 Control group 20 ,60 2,683 ,600 ToM story correct mental judgement (%) Hearing impaired group 7 96,43 9,449 3,571 Control group 20 98,80 3,694 ,826 ToM story correct physical judgement (%) Hearing impaired group 7 41,57 36,248 13,701 Control group 20 0,00 0,000 0,000 Educational

level Hearing impaired group 7 2,71 ,488 ,184

Control group 20 3,30 ,571 ,128 Age Hearing impaired group 7 29,29 6,651 2,514 Control group 20 29,30 4,692 1,049 Inhibition (%) Hearing impaired group 7 87,86 12,851 4,857 Control group 20 94,10 12,481 2,791 Updating low

load (%) Hearing impaired group 7 78,43 15,747 5,952

Control group 20 89,05 13,578 3,036

Updating high

load (%) Hearing impaired group 7 71,43 18,447 6,972

Control group 20 87,50 14,077 3,148 Non-words (%) Hearing impaired group 7 41,29 9,878 3,733 Control group 20 65,00 13,619 3,045 Non-words

consonants (%) Hearing impaired group 7 62,43 7,413 2,802

Control group 20 76,15 9,719 2,173 Non-words right vocal with consonants (%) Hearing impaired group 7 78,29 12,619 4,770 Control group 20 93,55 5,482 1,226

Real words (%) Hearing

impaired group 7 76,29 12,566 4,750

Control group 20 94,85 6,319 1,413

Real words

consonants (%) Hearing impaired group 7 82,57 10,596 4,005

Control group 20 94,20 6,833 1,528

Real words right vocal with consonants (%) Hearing impaired group 7 83,86 12,130 4,585 Control group 20 96,65 5,008 1,120

(17)

Table 3. T-values and p-values for the cognitive tests and the ToM-test.

Cognitive tests and ToM-test t df p-value

Vocabulary -3,655 25 0,001 ToM stories total points -2,975 25 0,006 ToM stories mental judgement -5,2 25 <0,001 ToM story physical judgement 5,378 25 <0,001 ToM story correct mental judgement 0,958 25 ns ToM story correct physical judgement 5,331 25 <0,001

Inhibition -1,131 25 ns

Updating low load -1,712 25 ns Updating high load -2,401 25 0,024 Non-words -4,212 25 <0,001 Non-words consonants -3,39 25 0,002 Non-words right vocal with consonants -4,448 25 <0,001 Real words -5,117 25 <0,001 Real words consonants -3,351 25 0,003 Real words right vocal with consonants -3,95 25 0,001

Discussion

The intentions of this pilot study were to investigate ToM in individuals with

congenital hearing impairment and to explore possible correlations between ToM and executive functions and verbal ability. However, it was not possible to explore any correlations between ToM and the degree of hearing impairment since the HI group was overly homogenous, in other words, the seven hearing impaired individuals had all a similar degree of hearing loss. We had a hypothesis that a discrepancy in ToM-ability between the control group and HI group would be forthcoming and that executive functioningand verbal ability are closely interconnected with ToM-ability. Some tendencies were revealed that support this supposition, which I will deliberate about later in this section.

We recruited seven individuals with a hearing impairment. The subjects were

retrieved from medical records at the Audiological Clinic with respect to the inclusion criteria, which consequently made them representative for this study. Indeed it was a small group of individuals; nevertheless they were a rather homogenous group regarding their hearing impairment. Furthermore, all of the hearing impaired individuals but one had pursued or are pursuing postgraduate studies and none of them were on sick leave pension.

(18)

Our intention was to make the reference population and the group of audiologically deprived individuals as equal as possible. We found no significant difference between the two groups in age or in gender. However, the control group had a slightly higher education level than the hearing impaired group.

All four tests took place in a quiet room and were performed in the same manner as with the control group. Everything was recorded to facilitate scoring afterwards. It is possible that the scoring process may leave room for speculations about whether this procedure is reliable or not, considering it was a subjective awarding of points. However, all of the tests had supplementary distinct descriptions of how the scoring should be proceeded. Moreover, I consulted a college to review the results and when ambiguity emerged. Concerning the ToM-test, the validated test from Happé, which has been in operation since 1994, was employed. This test has been widely

administered and applied on both children and adults with autism (3,33).

Statistical analyses were made; nonetheless we are well aware of the fact that there are infirmities with employing T-tests in such small groups (HI group N = 7) of individuals, e.g. that the probability of getting a lower significance level increases. In spite of the fact that overestimating the results is conceivable, interesting results were revealed and we could see some remarkable trends. Namely, the control group

performed better in virtually every single item.

When estimating results in small groups as these it is important to pay particular attention to the variations in the different groups. If the variations are insignificant, then the likelihood of them being substantial, rather than coincidences, increases. If a great diversity in results is prevailed, this could consequently lead to a bigger standard deviation, which furthermore demands much greater thoroughness to dealing with these results and drawing conclusions about them. Table three emphasises the mean percentage value in all tests. It also reveals the standard deviations, which are slightly high in the HI group, indicating that they should be assessed cautiously.

An establishment made was that the control group outperformed the HI-group considering their ability to attribute mental states and providing correct mental implications in ToM-stories. The results also disclose that physical judgements were

(19)

considerably more frequent within the group of individuals with HI than in the control group (see fig 1 and table 2). This corresponds with the hypothesis of the study, namely that in order to have an adequate ToM, one also has to have adequate auditory processing. However, despite this suggestion that a hearing impairment may

contribute to a deficient ToM, it is worth keeping in mind that the group of hearing impaired individuals only consisted of totally seven persons. Nevertheless, it is an interesting correlation that could be further investigated, considering previous studies proposing that language may play a role in ToM development (4,5,34). Different linguistic aspects have been suggested to be facilitative in the development of ToM (22,25); nonetheless there is a difference of opinion about which language trait plays the primary role (4). The results in this present study also indicate a possible impact of an individual’s verbal ability since the HI group’s results on WAIS-III vocabulary test significantly differed from the control group’s performance. This furthermore

corroborates the fact that language has an influence on ToM ability.

As mentioned above, figure 1 displays a higher tendency to provide physical

inferences in the HI group compared with the control group. Happé (3) suggests that this could imply a general lack of cognitive maturity, meaning that the hearing impaired individuals had a general incapacity to perceive the main point of the story. Nevertheless, incorrect mental state imputations seem to be a more reasonable explanation to the hearing impaired group’s inferior performance on the ToM-task.

The results regarding the serial recall of non-words test displayed significantly better results within the control group in different aspects; namely, the group of HI

individuals had more difficulties to reproduce non-words as well as to recall correct consonants and vowels comprising the non-words. This was an assumptive outcome, since it is uncontested that an audiological impairment can contract one’s ability to comprehend words and subsequently repeating them. However, an interesting thought is what influence a hearing loss may have on the cognition and whether this decrease in audiological skills could restrict the working memory capacity. There are studies that report that a reduced hearing capacity can decelerate the process of

comprehending speech in a manner where the greater effort hearing impaired individuals put into perceiving what they hear, comes at the expense of cognitive properties. These properties are believed to otherwise consolidate what has recently

(20)

been heard for subsequent recalling (35,36). This may consequently be an explanation of why our hearing impaired group performed poorer in the non-words task than the control group.

Rönnberg et al. (37) discuss the RAMBPHO function, which is an abbreviation for Rapid, Automatic Multi-modal binding of Phonology. The RAMBPHO function is believed to rapidly and implicitly match input with consolidated phonological representations in long-term memory. This is the case when ideal circumstances are forthcoming. However, in the instance of a hearing loss, these stored representations may not be successfully activated and phonological mismatches may occur (37). Phonological coding has been proposed to be a core operation in processing oral communication (38). Henceforth, a further assumption is that the audiologically deprived individuals’ processing of spoken language in this study is deficient due to inadequate phonological coding or matching of consolidated phonological

representations with what they have recently heard. Perhaps this inadequacy could confine one’s opportunity to acquire a big vocabulary, since consolidating of words appears to be dependent on being able to comprehend and process new words. A suggestion is that this is the reason why the HI group did not perform at the same level as the control group on the real words test (see fig 1). A presumption is that the individuals would find it easier to recall the real words, which indeed both of the groups had (see fig 1). The reason why this is possible may be that the individuals in both of the groups had representations of these words in their memory and perhaps therefore could recall these words easier than the non-words. However, a question is, would the hearing impaired group have performed even better on the real words-task if they had perfect hearing? Perhaps the RAMBPHO function unsuccessfully matched the mediated words and a phonological mismatch occurred due to their hearing loss.

Another finding was that the control group and the group of HI individuals performed similarly on the inhibition and updating low load test. Nevertheless, the result

demonstrates a better performance in the updating high load task within the control group. The poorer performance in the HI group may explain why this group also performed at a lower level on the phonological working memory task. Updating has namely been suggested to play a pronounced role in speech apprehension in normal hearing individuals, especially under ambitious circumstances (39). Mishra et al. (39)

(21)

mention the cognitive spare capacity (CSC), more precisely the cognitive capacity remaining once successful speech comprehension has occurred. The intention of the

“Updating high load”-test was to investigate the cognitive property updating in the

HI individuals. Hence, their less successful results could be a consequence of an increase in cognitive load with a subsequently lower CSC, since a lot more of the CSC is used to perceive what numbers are being conveyed. A suggestion is that engaging the CSC to a larger extent than necessary may set requirements on the ability to update information. This could consequently make it more problematic to replace lower no longer relevant numbers with new higher numbers and

simultaneously remember the initial mediated number.

It has been submitted that executive functions may contribute to the development of ToM (20,25). However, no correlations between any of the ToM-measures and the executive functions were detected within the group of hearing impaired individuals in this study.

An interesting topic worth mentioning is the mirror neuron system. As declared in the introduction, this system is considered to integrate three different types of sensory stimulation, which has aroused curiosity to explore what impact the auditory system possibly could have on this trimodal mirror neuron system. Hence, an interesting question is what consequence a deprivation in e.g. audition, which is the circumstance in hearing impaired individuals, may lead to then. Gazzola et al. (13) indicate an assumptive link between our ability to feel empathy with another person and the motor mirror system. By letting people listen to the sound of an action they were familiar with and which they previously had performed, certain brain areas were activated. A possible sequel of poorer audiological skills or even deafness could therefore be having difficulties with relating to people and enter into their feelings. Hence, it is plausible that adequate hearing is necessary in order to develop ToM. Is there possibly a biological explanation to why and how humans feel empathy? Mike et al. (1) intended to investigate what impact brain pathology in multiple sclerosis could have on taking another person’s perspective by testing their ability to recognise mental states and emotions. They discovered correlations between these tests and specific brain areas involved in different aspects of mentalization, which led to their proposal that brain lesions in MS may consequently influence these individuals’

(22)

ability to comprehend and process emotions and mental states mediated by other individuals. Moreover, deficits in ToM have been reported in patients with

Alzheimer’s disease (40), which is the most common neurodegenerative disease in the elderly (41). So, let me yet again bring up the mirror neuron system for discussion, which is believed to be trimodal, meaning that it integrates visual, motor and auditory stimulation (15,16). If one person’s auditory modality is absent, then only the visual and motor modalities will be obtainable. Is this enough to fully activate the mirror neuron system and could this partial activation lead to deficient stimulation of auditory mirror neurons? Conceivably, this insufficient stimulation of these auditory mirror neurons may result in degeneration of them, which could lead to a deficient ToM in the same manner as in Alzheimer’s disease and MS.

Conclusion

This study culminated in some interesting results that show tendencies that corroborate with my hypothesis and proposes that a hearing impairment may contribute to a deficient ToM. This proposal is supported by the descriptive data indicating significantly better results within the control group than in the hearing impaired group with respect to their ability to attribute mental states and, furthermore, the control groups slightly better performance in providing correct mental

implications in ToM-stories. In summation, the control group performed better in all of the practiced tasks. The poorer performance on the test measuring the phonological working memory within the hearing impaired group gave rise to the suggestion that an inadequate hearing could influence the working memory capacity and the matching of recently heard words with consolidated representations. The hearing impaired group performed poorer at the “Updating high load”-test indicating a supposedly increased cognitive load due to the individuals’ hearing loss. Furthermore, it is possible that a poorer verbal ability could contribute to a deficient ToM-ability, since the hearing-impaired group performed inferior to the control group in both the ToM-test and the vocabulary ToM-test. However, unfortunately no correlations between any of the ToM-measures and the executive functions were found within the group of HI individuals.

(23)

We certainly saw some significant differences between the two groups. However, there is uncertainty as to whether these revealed results are substantial or not since the hearing impaired group comprised very few people. To determine whether these discrepancies between the two groups are considerable, we need to have a bigger sample. This would facilitate the judgement of the results, since a larger study could either confirm or deny that substantial differences are forthcoming between the two groups concerning ToM.

This is, to the best of my knowledge, the first study on mentalization in normal adults with congenital or early-acquired hearing loss. I encourage the implementation of further investigating the possible relationship between ToM and deprived hearing. Concluding such an association could help the elaboration of investigation methods and interventions within rehabilitation operation.

Acknowledgements

 

I will like to thank my supervisor, Claes Möller, for giving me the opportunity to work with this interesting project and for all his support and guidance throughout my writing process. I also want to thank my other supervisor, Hans-Erik Frölander, for his big support and the commitment he has shown to this project. Also, thanks to all the friendly staff at the Audiological Research Centre, University hospital Örebro for helping me with the technicalities, registration of data and to retrieve individuals. A specific thanks to Kristina Ingvall who helped me to retrieve the seven hearing impaired individuals. Finally, I would like to thank all of the individuals who participated in this study.

References

(1) Mike A, Strammer E, Aradi M, Orsi G, Perlaki G, Hajnal A, et al. Disconnection mechanism and regional cortical atrophy contribute to impaired processing of facial expressions and theory of mind in multiple sclerosis: a structural MRI study. PLoS One 2013 Dec 13;8(12):e82422.

(2) Wellman HM, Cross D, Watson J. Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: the truth about false belief. Child Dev 2001 May-Jun;72(3):655-684.

(24)

(3) Happé FGE. An advanced test of theory of mind: Understanding of story

characters' thoughts and feelings by able autistic, mentally handicapped, and normal children and adults. J Autism Dev Disord 1994;24(2):129 <last_page> 154.

(4) Schick B, de Villiers P, de Villiers J, Hoffmeister R. Language and theory of mind: a study of deaf children. Child Dev 2007 Mar-Apr;78(2):376-396.

(5) Peterson CC, Wellman HM. From fancy to reason: scaling deaf and hearing children's understanding of theory of mind and pretence. Br J Dev Psychol 2009 Jun;27(Pt 2):297-310.

(6) White S, Hill E, Happe F, Frith U. Revisiting the strange stories: revealing mentalizing impairments in autism. Child Dev 2009 Jul-Aug;80(4):1097-1117. (7) Tager-Flusberg H. Language and Understanding Minds: Connections in Autism. In: Baron-Cohen S, Tager-Flusberg H, Cohen DJ, editors. Understanding other minds: Perspectives from autism and developmental cognitive neuroscience. 2nd ed. New York, NY, US.: Oxford University Press; 1994.

(8) Caggiano V, Fogassi L, Rizzolatti G, Casile A, Giese MA, Thier P. Mirror

neurons encode the subjective value of an observed action. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2012 Jul 17;109(29):11848-11853.

(9) di Pellegrino G, Fadiga L, Fogassi L, Gallese V, Rizzolatti G. Understanding motor events: a neurophysiological study. Exp Brain Res 1992;91(1):176-180.

(10) Gallese V, Fadiga L, Fogassi L, Rizzolatti G. Action recognition in the premotor cortex. Brain 1996 Apr;119 ( Pt 2)(Pt 2):593-609.

(11) Kohler E, Keysers C, Umilta MA, Fogassi L, Gallese V, Rizzolatti G. Hearing sounds, understanding actions: action representation in mirror neurons. Science 2002 Aug 2;297(5582):846-848.

(12) Buccino G, Binkofski F, Fink GR, Fadiga L, Fogassi L, Gallese V, et al. Action observation activates premotor and parietal areas in a somatotopic manner: an fMRI study. Eur J Neurosci 2001 Jan;13(2):400-404.

(13) Gazzola V, Aziz-Zadeh L, Keysers C. Empathy and the somatotopic auditory mirror system in humans. Curr Biol 2006 Sep 19;16(18):1824-1829.

(14) Keysers C, Kohler E, Umilta MA, Nanetti L, Fogassi L, Gallese V. Audiovisual mirror neurons and action recognition. Exp Brain Res 2003 Dec;153(4):628-636. (15) Le Bel RM, Pineda JA, Sharma A. Motor-auditory-visual integration: The role of the human mirror neuron system in communication and communication disorders. J Commun Disord 2009 Jul-Aug;42(4):299-304.

(16) D'Ausilio A. The role of the mirror system in mapping complex sounds into actions. J Neurosci 2007 May 30;27(22):5847-5848.

(25)

(17) Hala S. The development of social cognition. Hove: Psychology Press; 1997. (18) White S, Hill E, Happe F, Frith U. Revisiting the strange stories: revealing mentalizing impairments in autism. Child Dev 2009 Jul-Aug;80(4):1097-1117. (19) Miller EK, Cohen JD. An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annu Rev Neurosci 2001;24:167-202.

(20) Benson JE, Sabbagh MA, Carlson SM, Zelazo PD. Individual differences in executive functioning predict preschoolers' improvement from theory-of-mind training. Dev Psychol 2013 Sep;49(9):1615-1627.

(21) Velloso Rde L, Duarte CP, Schwartzman JS. Evaluation of the theory of mind in autism spectrum disorders with the Strange Stories test. Arq Neuropsiquiatr 2013 Nov;71(11):871-876.

(22) Hale CM, Tager-Flusberg H. The influence of language on theory of mind: a training study. Dev Sci 2003 Jun;6(3):346-359.

(23) Gordon AC, Olson DR. The relation between acquisition of a theory of mind and the capacity to hold in mind. J Exp Child Psychol 1998 Jan;68(1):70-83.

(24) Fletcher PC, Henson RN. Frontal lobes and human memory: insights from functional neuroimaging. Brain 2001 May;124(Pt 5):849-881.

(25) Slade L, Ruffman T. How language does (and does not) relate to theory of mind: A longitudinal study of syntax, semantics, working memory and false belief. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 2005;23(1):117-141.

(26) Baddeley A, Hitch G. Working Memory. In: Bower G, editor. Psychology of Learning and Motivation: V.8 New York, U.S.A.: Academic Press, INC.; 1974. p. 47-87.

(27) Baddeley A. The episodic buffer: a new component of working memory? Trends Cogn Sci 2000 Nov 1;4(11):417-423.

(28) Miyake A, Friedman NP, Emerson MJ, Witzki AH, Howerter A, Wager TD. The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex "Frontal Lobe" tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cogn Psychol 2000 Aug;41(1):49-100.

(29) Baddeley A. Exploring the Central Executive. The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology A, Human experimental psychology 1996;49(1):5-21. (30) St Clair-Thompson HL, Gathercole SE. Executive functions and achievements in school: Shifting, updating, inhibition, and working memory. Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) 2006 Apr;59(4):745-759.

(31) Almqvist B, Arlinger S, Bergholtz L. Metodbok i praktisk hörselmätning. 2nd ed. Bromma: C-A Tegnér; 2004.

(26)

(32) Gathercole SE, Pickering SJ. Working memory deficits in children with low achievements in the national curriculum at 7 years of age. Br J Educ Psychol 2000 Jun;70 ( Pt 2)(Pt 2):177-194.

(33) White S, Hill E, Happe F, Frith U. Revisiting the strange stories: revealing mentalizing impairments in autism. Child Dev 2009 Jul-Aug;80(4):1097-1117. (34) Ruffman T, Slade L, Crowe E. The relation between children's and mothers' mental state language and theory-of-mind understanding. Child Dev 2002 May-Jun;73(3):734-751.

(35) McCoy SL, Tun PA, Cox LC, Colangelo M, Stewart RA, Wingfield A. Hearing loss and perceptual effort: downstream effects on older adults' memory for speech. Q J Exp Psychol A 2005 Jan;58(1):22-33.

(36) Piquado T, Benichov JI, Brownell H, Wingfield A. The hidden effect of hearing acuity on speech recall, and compensatory effects of self-paced listening. Int J Audiol 2012 Aug;51(8):576-583.

(37) Rönnberg J, Rudner M, Foo C. The cognitive neuroscience of signed language: Applications to a working memory system for sign and speech. In: Bäckman L, Nyberg L, editors. Memory, aging and the brain: A Festschrift in honour of Lars-Göran Nilsson: Psychology Press; 2010. p. 265-86.

(38) Casserly ED, Pisoni DB. Nonword repetition as a predictor of long-term speech and language skills in children with cochlear implants. Otol Neurotol 2013

Apr;34(3):460-470.

(39) Mishra S, Lunner T, Stenfelt S, Ronnberg J, Rudner M. Visual information can hinder working memory processing of speech. J Speech Lang Hear Res 2013 Aug;56(4):1120-1132.

(40) Le Bouc R, Lenfant P, Delbeuck X, Ravasi L, Lebert F, Semah F, et al. My belief or yours? Differential theory of mind deficits in frontotemporal dementia and

Alzheimer's disease. Brain 2012 Oct;135(Pt 10):3026-3038.

(41) Purves D, Augustine G, Fitzpatrick D, Hall W, Lamantia A, White L.

Neuroscience. 5th ed. Sunderland, Massachusetts, U.S.A.: Sinauer Associates, Inc.; 2011.

References

Related documents

The effects of the students ’ working memory capacity, language comprehension, reading comprehension, school grade and gender and the intervention were analyzed as a

Her main research interests concern adults with hearing loss and the International Classification of Functio- ning, Disability, and Health (ICF).. She has previously worked as

The findings from previous research point at the adverse relationship between adults with hearing loss and important aspects of everyday life such as social relations,

In the present thesis, the ability to repeat prosodic and segmental features of real words and nonwords was investigated in Swedish-speaking four- to six-year-old chil- dren with

Volume, total sperm numbers and progressive motility in epididymal caudae (CE) of free ranging wild European red deer stags (Cervus elaphus L) (n =36) hunted in three

Studies from the Swedish Institute for Disability Research

Intraoperativa strategier för att hantera ventilationen hos den vuxne obese patienten som genomgår laparoskopisk kirurgi i generell anestesi.. Intraoperative strategies for

I sådana här landskap är människan inte bara sällsynt exponerad för väder och vind, som stekande sol eller bitande vindar, utan också för andras blickar och avlyssning.. De