• No results found

Methodological considerations when adding new measures to the Lane Change Test

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2022

Share "Methodological considerations when adding new measures to the Lane Change Test"

Copied!
5
0
0

Loading.... (view fulltext now)

Full text

(1)

METHODOLOGICAL CONSIDERATIONS WHEN ADDING NEW MEASURES TO THE LANE CHANGE TEST

Grane, C

Department of Business Administration, Technology and Social Sciences, Division of Human Work Sciences

Luleå University of Technology, SE-971 87 Luleå, Sweden E-mail: camilla.grane@ltu.se

The Lane Change Test measures one aspect of driving performance, but other measures can be added. This paper discusses the effects of adding a measure for missed signs without adjusting the specified task pace. Two experiments with two secondary task conditions and a design with either a continuous or a controlled number of tasks were compared. The data suggest that effective tasks increase the risk of missing signs if the tasks are presented continuously and require drivers to take their eyes off the road.

Keywords: Lane Change Test, Driving performance measures, Missed signs

1 Introduction

Safety is important when developing new products for vehicles. In both real world settings and driving simulators, driving performance, one aspect of safety, can be measured as break reaction time, visual time off road, hazard detection, and more. One driving simulator tool developed to measure the influence of secondary tasks on driving performance is the Lane Change Test (Mattes, 2003). The Lane Change Test measures driving performance as the driving deviation from a normative driving route

(DaimlerChrysler AG, 2004). This is a broad measure that covers several aspects of deteriorated driving performance such as late reaction to a sign, missed signs, or low lane keeping quality (Mattes, 2003). Several studies (Engström et al., 2005; Liang &

Lee, 2010) have identified different influences on driving performance depending on the type of secondary task. The type of driving performance deterioration can help explain why a secondary task is distracting. As the Lane Change Test in its original form does not discriminate between different driving behaviours, additional metrics seem

necessary (Rognin et al., 2007). This testing problem has been addressed by several studies by adding measures for correct lane changes such as missed signs and erroneous lane changes (Bruyas et al., 2008; Engström & Markkula, 2007; Young et al., 2011).

However, since the Lane change Test was developed to measure the deviation from a normative driving route, there is a risk that added measures could produce misleading results. The effects of adding new measures to the Lane Change Test is important to study and discuss since misleading results can have the unwanted effect of developers choosing more demanding and less safe solutions.

2 Objective

This paper compares and discusses the effects of adding a new measure – missed signs – to the Lane Change Test with and without adjusting the Lane Change Test

specifications for secondary task pace.

(2)

3 Method

Two experiments with similar design but continuous or controlled secondary task pace were compared.

3.1 Participants

The experiment with continuous presentation of secondary tasks had 18 participants and the experiment with a controlled number of secondary tasks had 20 participants. None of the participants had previous experience with the equipment used in the study.

3.2 Experimental tasks and equipment

In both studies, the Lane Change Test was used as a primary task to simulate driving and measure driving performance. A Logitech Momo Racing steering wheel was used and the driving task was displayed on a computer screen in front of the participants (Figure 1). The secondary task was a simple menu selection task that was identical in both studies. The participants were asked to find and select a target item in a menu with four menu items. The target was announced through headphones and displayed on a small screen beneath the driving scene (Figure 1). The menu items were always the same, but their arrangement and the target item changed between tasks. The menu was displayed on a computer screen next to the driving scene and a haptic rotary device (Alps Haptic Commander) was used for menu interaction (Figure 1). The rotary device provided haptic information that matched the visual menu interface. Two conditions – with and without haptic information – were compared. With haptic information, the transition from one menu item to another was sensed and the transitions felt like soft ridges. When no haptic information was provided, the rotary device moved smoothly, without resistance.

Figure 1. The experimental setup. The driving task is on the screen directly in front of the participant and the menu selection task is on the screen next to this screen. The target appears on the small screen below the driving scene screen and is announced via headphones.

3.3 Lane Change Test

In the Lane Change Test, the participants drove on a straight three-lane road on which no other cars were present. Road signs showing lane change information were placed along the road. The driving task was to keep the car within a driving lane and change

(3)

lanes as quickly and efficiently as possible when directed to by the signs. The driving speed was set at 60 km/h throughout the experiments. To reduce individual variations in driving performance, each participant drove one baseline without performing a

secondary task at the beginning and at the end of the experimental session. The driving performance with secondary tasks was compared to a quotient between the two driving baselines.

3.4 Task pace

In the Lane Change Test, a continual presentation of secondary tasks while driving is recommended to obtain even distraction throughout the driving session. This set up was used in one of the experiments. Once a participant had made a menu selection a new target was announced through the headphones. In the other experiment, the secondary task pace was fixed to 12 menu selections spread out evenly during the driving round.

The targets were announced at the same driving route locations for all participants in the experiment with controlled task pace.

3.5 Missed signs

The standard measure in the Lane Change Test is the driving deviation from a normative driving round. In this test, however, a measure for missed signs was used.

The Lane Change Test analysis program provides driving curves for each participant and driving round. These curves were manually scanned for missed lane changes.

Missed signs were counted as the number of signs that were passed without a lane change. Figure 2 shows an example of a missed sign.

Figure 2. Example of a missed sign. The white curve shows the participant driving and the black curve shows the normative driving route. The arrow and the two X’s show the current sign information directing the driver to change to the middle lane.

3.6 Task completion time

For the discussion in this paper, the task completion time was added as a measure of secondary task performance.

3.7 Analysis

The experiment with continuous secondary task pace had a counterbalanced within- subjects design thus the results were analysed using the Wilcoxon signed-rank test. The other experiment with controlled secondary task pace had a between-subjects design, so

(4)

the Mann-Whitney test was used in the task completion time analyses. In both

experiments, the missed sign analysis was performed using the Wilcoxon signed-rank test since individual baseline data were compared. The α-level was set to .05.

4 Results

The secondary task condition with haptic information had significantly shorter task completion time than the condition with no haptic support. This was found in both the experiment with continuous secondary task pace (T = 34, p = .024, r = -.50) and in the experiment with controlled secondary task pace (U = 11.00, p = .003, r = -.66). More signs were missed in the experiment with continuous secondary task pace than in the experiment with controlled secondary task pace. In the experiment with continuous secondary task pace, significantly more signs were missed compared to the individual baselines when haptic information was provided (T = 1.50, p = .003, r = -.63), but not when haptic information was not provided (T = 1, p = .063, r = -.45). In the experiment with controlled secondary task pace, no significant increase of missed signs compared to the individual baselines was found for either the condition with haptic information (T

= 3, p = .625, r = 0) or without haptic information (T = 4, p = .438, r = -.08).

5 Discussion

Although the two experiments described in this paper were close to identical, they produced different results. Negative results for adding haptic information was only found with a continuous secondary task pace. More importantly, the negative results for adding haptic information might be misleading.

The addition of haptic information had a clear positive effect on task completion time.

In a previous study, haptic information also had a positive effect on error rate and users found the haptic information helpful (Grane & Bengtsson, 2012). Why would helpful haptic information make participants miss signs? If the haptic information increases cognitive load in the task, the results could be an effect of cognitive tunnelling. The haptic information used some processing resources; otherwise, it would not have been perceived. However, further processing and analyses that demand higher cognitive resources was probably not needed. Rather the missed signs were an effect of the experimental design. With a continuous task rate, the participants solved as many tasks as they could. That is, the task completion time implies more tasks were solved when haptic information was provided. The participants had to take their eyes off the road and look at the displayed menu to find the target. With continuous task pace, an increased task rate, which is positive, also increased the time eyes were taken off the road, which is negative. A natural consequence of taking the eyes off road is an increased risk of not

“seeing” obstacles on or beside the road, such as the signs in the Lane Change Test.

With a controlled number of tasks that are held constant between participants and conditions, the eyes off road rate describes the visual load in the tasks rather than reflects an efficient task rate.

The Lane Change Test is effective and reliable when two conditions are compared, such as in this paper. However, the method lacks ecological validity: the results do not reflect what would happen in real life driving. A condition that yields missed signs with the Lane Change Test may or may not cause drivers to miss signs in real driving. This result depends on the focus and priorities of the driver. However, the Lane Change Test can

(5)

distinguish a more demanding task form a less demanding task. Therefore, the method is suitable in early product development stages or in research when new areas are studied. However, when new measures are added to the Lane Change Test the method should be corrected accordingly.

6 Conclusions

When new measures are added to the Lane Change Test, the results can be misleading and reflect the experimental design rather than task demand. When adding a measure for missed signs without changing the continuous secondary task pace to a controlled number of secondary tasks, the missed signs could reflect the task rate. Continuous secondary tasks will increase the risk of missing signs if the tasks require drivers to take their eyes off the road.

7 References

Bruyas, M.-P., Brusque, C., Tattegrain, H., Auriault, A., Aillerie, I. & Duraz, M. 2008.

Consistency and sensitivity of lane change test according to driving simulator characteristics. IET Intelligent Transport Systems, 2, 306-314.

DaimlerChrystler AG, Research and Technology. 2004. Lane Change Test – User Guide 1.2. Retrieved May 24, 2011, from: http://people.usd.edu/~schieber/pdf/LCT- UserGuide.pdf

Engström, J., Johansson, E. & Östlund, J. 2005. Effects of visual and cognitive load in real and simulated motorway driving. Transportation Research Part F, 8, 97-120.

Engström, J. & Markkula, G. 2007. Effects of visual and cognitive distraction on lane change test performance. Proceedings of the Fourth International Driving Symposium on Human Factors in Driver Assessment, Training and Vehicle Design, Stevenson, Washington, 199-205.

Grane, C. & Bengtsson, P. 2012. Haptic addition to a visual menu selection interface controlled by an in-vehicle rotary device. Advances in Human-Computer Interaction, 2012, 12 pages. doi:10.1155/2012/787469

Liang, Y. & Lee, J. D. 2010. Combining cognitive and visual distraction: Less than the sum of its parts. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 42, 881-890.

Mattes, S. 2003. The Lane-Change-Task as a tool for driver distraction evaluation. In H.

Strasser, K. Kluth, H. Rausch & H. Bubb (Eds.), Quality of Work and Products in Enterprises of the Future (pp. 57-60). Stuttgart, Germany: Ergonomia Verlag.

Rognin, L., Alidra, S., Val, C. & Lescaut, A. 2007. Occurrence of secondary tasks and quality of lane changes. In D. Harris (Ed.), Engineering Psychology and Cognitive Ergonomics, HCII 2007 (pp.397-406). Berlin Heidelberg: Springer-Verlag.

Young, K. L., Lenné, M. G. & Williamsson, A. R. 2011. Sensitivity of the lane change test as a measure of in-vehicle system demand. Applied Ergonomics, 42, 611-618.

References

Related documents

46 Konkreta exempel skulle kunna vara främjandeinsatser för affärsänglar/affärsängelnätverk, skapa arenor där aktörer från utbuds- och efterfrågesidan kan mötas eller

The routine for uploading content did not require any specific adjustment as it did not differ much from the previous process where ‘The Sign group’ was uploading content to the fixed

We argue that Secondary Task (ST) techniques have certain advantages over existing Subjective Rating Scales (SRS) instruments, and report a study, in which a particular ST

13 This conclusion was further supported by the results gained from the mZac1 overexpression study performed in the chicken, where a reduction in the number of cells entering

Simulation I is made for the purpose of illustrating the robustness of the particle filter algorithm against course and velocity measurement noise with zero bias.. Still, the

Empirical methods are keen about measuring mental effort and the performance; they rely on collecting subjective data using rating scales, performance data using

One might also want to introduce a new chunk in the BEAM file where the translated bit- string comprehensions could be kept, in this chunk the kernels should be ordered so that

The same Hilbert spaces make it possible to model term and document content as if it was some kind of intellectual charge inherent in components of lan- guage, leading to