• No results found

Treatment effect expressed as the novel Delay of Event measure is associated with high willingness to initiate preventive treatment - A randomized survey experiment comparing effect measures

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "Treatment effect expressed as the novel Delay of Event measure is associated with high willingness to initiate preventive treatment - A randomized survey experiment comparing effect measures"

Copied!
7
0
0

Loading.... (view fulltext now)

Full text

(1)

Treatment

effect

expressed

as

the

novel

Delay

of

Event

measure

is

associated

with

high

willingness

to

initiate

preventive

treatment

A

randomized

survey

experiment

comparing

effect

measures

Erik

Berglund

a,

*

,

Ragnar

Westerling

a

,

Johan

Sundström

b

,

Per

Lytsy

a a

DepartmentofPublicHealthandCaringSciences,UppsalaUniversity,Uppsala,Sweden

b

DepartmentofMedicalSciences,UppsalaUniversity,Uppsala,Sweden

ARTICLE INFO Articlehistory: Received21April2016

Receivedinrevisedform20July2016 Accepted21July2016 Keywords: Preventivemeasures Adherence Decision-making Treatmentoutcome Randomized Surveyexperiment ABSTRACT

Objectives:Thisstudyaimedtoinvestigatepatients’willingnesstoinitiateapreventivetreatmentand comparedtwoestablishedeffectmeasurestothenewlydevelopedDelayofEvents(DoE)measurethat expressestreatmenteffectasagaininevent-freetime.

Methods:Inthiscross-sectional,randomizedsurveyexperimentinthegeneralSwedishpopulation,1079 respondents (response rate 60.9%) were asked toconsider apreventive cardiovascular treatment. Respondentswererandomlyallocatedtooneofthreeeffectdescriptions:DoE,relativeriskreduction (RRR), or absolute risk reduction (ARR). Univariate and multivariate analyses were performed investigatingwillingnesstoinitiatetreatment,viewsontreatmentbenefit,motivationandimportance toadhereandwillingnesstopayfortreatment.

Results:Eighty-onepercentwerewillingtotakethemedicationwhentheeffectwasdescribedasDoE, 83.0%whenitwasdescribedasRRRand62.8%whenitwasdescribedasARR.DoEandRRRwasfurther associatedwithpositiveviewsontreatmentbenefit,motivation,importancetoadhereandWTP. Conclusions:PresentingtreatmenteffectasDoEorRRRwasassociatedwithahighwillingnesstoinitiate treatment.

Practiceimplications:Anapproachbasedonthenoveltime-basedmeasureDoEmaybeofvalueinclinical communicationandshareddecisionmaking.

ã2016TheAuthors.PublishedbyElsevierIrelandLtd.ThisisanopenaccessarticleundertheCC BY-NC-NDlicense(http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/).

1.Introduction

Patients’understandingofa proposedmedication’s effective-nessiscentraltoevidence-basedmedicineandmedical decision-making. Informed shared decision-making is possible when a patienthasanadequateunderstandingoftheirmedicalsituation/ increasedrisk includingasoundunderstandingoftheexpected benefits from treatment. It is well documented that the way treatment information is presented influences patients’ under-standing and decisions to initiate and adhere to a proposed treatment[1–3].Thisis ofspecial concerninthepreventionof chronicdiseases,wherethetreatmentgoalistopreventordelay majordiseaseeventsorprematuredeath.Long-termadherenceto such chronic treatments is known to be poor [4]. Insufficient adherencetocardiovascularpreventivetreatmentsisassociated

with increased morbidity [5–7], and thus nonadherence is consideredtobeacardiovascularriskfactor[8].

Thewayatreatment’seffectispresentedhasbeenshownto influencedecisionsby patientsas well asphysicians [1,9].It is therefore of clinical interest to determinethe optimal way of describingtreatmenteffectthatincreasespatients’willingnessto initiateandadheretotreatment.

Commonly used measures describing preventive treatment effectincluderelativeriskreduction(RRR),absoluteriskreduction (ARR) and number-needed-to-treat(NNT) [10]. Thesemeasures comparetheproportionofeventsinrelativeandabsolutetermsin twocomparedtrialarms.Whilethesemeasuresare methodologi-cally justified,theymay be difficultto usein shared decision-making as people typically have difficulties understanding proportional measures [11–13].ARR and its inverse, NNT, have the advantage of presenting the effect as absolute figures. However,researchhasshownthatpresentingbenefit asARRor NNT is suboptimal when communicating treatment benefits

[14,15].RRRtypicallycomesoutasmorefavorablethanARRand

NNTwhencomparingpatients’willingnesstoinitiatetreatment

* Corresponding author.Current Address:Department of PublicHealthand CaringSciences,UppsalaUniversity,Box564,SE-75122Uppsala,Sweden.

E-mailaddress:erik.berglund@pubcare.uu.se(E.Berglund).

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.pec.2016.07.028

0738-3991/ã2016TheAuthors.PublishedbyElsevierIrelandLtd.ThisisanopenaccessarticleundertheCCBY-NC-NDlicense( http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/).

ContentslistsavailableatScienceDirect

Patient

Education

and

Counseling

(2)

[1];however,RRRmaybedeceptiveasthemeasureisignorantof the baseline risk. Several authors have proposed that these established effect measures may be complemented by other estimatesprovidingtheeffectonthetimedimensionofinterest

[16–18]. Time-based effectmeasures of treatmenteffect are of interestinsharedclinicaldecision-makingassuchmeasuresseem tobepreferableandeasiertocomprehendforlaypeopleaswellas healthcareprofessionals[19,15,20].Previousstudiesinvestigating theroleofdifferenteffectmeasuresinmedicaldecision-making haveusedtime-basedmeasuresbased onextrapolationofdata

[21],whichraisesquestionsaboutthecorrectnessand compara-bility.Withthegoalof combiningtheclinicalbenefitsof time-basedmeasureswiththerigorofstatisticalcalculatedmeasures,a relativelynewwayofdemonstratingtreatmentonatimescalehas beenproposed [22]. DoE may beestimated modelling survival percentileswithLaplaceregression,astatisticalmethod appropri-ateforcensoredquantilesandsurvivaldistribution[23].TheDoE may be understood as the horizontal difference between a treatmentarm andcontrol armin aKaplan-Meier curve.Delay ofEventsisanabsolutemeasurethatisconditionalontheevent, whichmeansthatifa patienthastheeventwithouttreatment duringthefollow-upperiod,theDoEdepictsthetimethatevent may be delayed by due to treatment. Thus, DoE captures the magnitudeoftheeffectexpressedasagainindisease-freetimefor treatedcomparedtountreatedpatients.

The objective of this study is to compare the established proportionaleffectmeasures,RRRandARR,withthenovel time-basedmeasureDoE,todeterminehowthedifferentformatsaffect

a person’s willingness to initiate a preventive medication treatment.Asecondaryobjectiveistoinvestigatehowthedifferent waysofdemonstratingtreatmenteffectaffectindividuals’views on treatment benefit, motivation to initiate and adhere to treatment,andwillingnesstopayfortreatment[24,25].

2.Methods

2.1.Designandsample

This is a cross-sectional postal questionnaire study in a population-based sample (age 45–75). We randomly selected 1800peoplefromtheSwedishnationalpopulationregistry. The sample was then further randomized into three equally sized groups (A, B and C), which received different treatment effect informationaboutahypotheticalcardiovasculartreatment,further described below. The questionnaire was returned by 1079 individuals whogaveinformed consenttoparticipate.Fourteen questionnaireswerereturnedunopenedtothesenderbythemail service,astherecipientcouldnotbereached/werenolongerliving attheaddress.Thirteenrecipientsdeclinedtoparticipate,and694 personsdidnot answeratall,making theresponserate of the distributedquestionnaires60.4%(1079/1786).Theresponseratein thethreegroupsvariedfrom56.8%to65.2%.Datawerecollected fromNovember2013toFebruary2014.Fig.1presentsaflowchart ofthestudy.Thosehowdeclinedtoparticipatedidthateitherby sendingusaletteroremailorthrutelephone.

Fig.1.Theflowdiagramshowsgroupallocationandnumberofrespondentsineachgroup.GroupAreceivedtreatmenteffectdescribedasDoE,groupBreceivedinformation describedasRRRandgroupCreceivedinformationdescribedasARR.

(3)

2.2.Measuresandsurveyexperiment

The postal questionnaire contained a total of 55 questions. Demographicdatawerecollectedthroughquestionsthatassessed therespondents’gender,ageandeducationallevel(categorizedas compulsory school, secondary school or university). History of heartattackand/orangina wasassessedwithquestionsusedin previous studies [26,27]. Data were also collected regarding whetherthe respondents wereon current medical treatments, and if so, the number of prescription drugs. The written information aboutthe effects and theoutcome questions were testedinapilotstudyandmodifiedafterwardsbeforetheywere includedinthisquestionnaire.

2.2.1.Writteninformationandsurveyexperiment

Thisstudywasperformedasasurveyexperiment[28,29].All respondentswereaskedtoimaginethattheywereatincreased risk of cardiovascular disease, and that their physician had suggesteda preventive cardiovasculardrugtreatment.The text wasasfollows:“Imaginethatinthenextfiveyearsyouwillhavean increasedriskofhavingaheartattack.Yourphysicianoffersyoua drug,withinfrequentandmildsideeffects,whichistobetaken orallyoncedaily.Theusefulnessofthedrughasbeenevaluatedin scientificstudies,andtheeffectcanbedescribedasfollows:”The following text described the effect differently based on group allocation, where the first group (A) received treatment effect described as Delay of Events, the second group (B) received information described as RRRand the third group(C)received informationdescribedasARR(seeTable1).Thedifferentwaysof displaying the same treatment benefit were all derived and calculatedfromtheScandinavianSimvastatinSurvivalStudy(4S),a randomized controlled trial presenting the first evidence that statintreatmentimprovessurvivalinpatientswithcoronaryheart disease[30].InthissurveytheDoEfigurewasassessedattheend ofthe4Sstudy,atthesametimeasthepointestimatesforthe othermeasures. Thescenario and outcome questions was pre-testedinaprimarycarepilotsurvey(n=180)andthenadjusted andfinalizedintoitspresentform.Thephrasingsandmethodsare similar to other studies investing different ways of explaining medication benefit [31]. NNT was not included since previous research has found that the NNT measures are ineffective for communicatingtreatmentbenefits[14,15].

2.2.2.Outcomevariables

Information about willingness to initiate treatment was assessedusingthequestion:“If youwereinthesamesituation asthepersoninthiscasewouldyouinitiatetreatment?”Answers weredichotomous:“yes”or“no.”Otherquestionsthatwereused toevaluatetheviewsonthetreatmentwere:“Towhatextentdoes thedescriptionhelpyoumakeamedicaldecision?”;“Howmuch benefitdoyouthinkthatthedrugwouldhave?”;“Wouldyoufeel safetotakethedrug?”;“Wouldyou,basedonthedescription,be

motivatedtotakethemedicationona dailybasis?”;and “How important do youconsiderit toadheretothetreatment?” The answerstothesequestionswereassessedonseven-pointLikert scalesrangingfrom1(notatall)to7(verymuch).Willingnessto payisanassessment ofthe(maximum)amountofmoney that mustbepaidbyanindividualtoequalizeautilitychange[25].To evaluate willingness to pay (WTP), the contingent valuation method (CVM) was used. The CVM is a questionnaire-based methodfirstproposedasamethodforelicitingmarketvaluationof nonmarketgoodsandpublicgoods[24,25].Themethodiswidely usedtoelicit themonetaryvalueaperson iswillingtopayfor health-care services [32], and likewise as a complementary measureoftheexpectedutility.InthisstudyWTPwasassessed byoneopen-endedquestion:“Whatisthemaximumamountyou would pay per month over a five-year period to receive the treatment?”WTPwasassessedinSwedishcurrency(SEK),andwas convertedinthisstudytoeuros(s)atanexchangerateof0.10. 2.3.Statisticalanalysis

Differencesinproportionsbetweengroupsweretestedusing Chi-squareanalyses,anddifferencesinordinaldatawereassessed using the Kruskal-Wallis H test. Pairwise comparisons were performed with the Mann-Whitney U test using Bonferroni correction to maintainthe risk of type 1 errorat 0.05 [33].In Bonferroni correction for pairwise comparisons among three groups, the significance level is set toa p-value0.017 (0.05/ 30.017).Binarylogisticregressionmodelswereusedtoanalyze associations betweendifferent effectprescriptions, gender,age, education level, history of heart disease,number of prescribed medicationsandwillingnesstoinitiatetreatment.Samplesizewas determinedpriortothestudytohavean80%powertodeterminea 10% difference in proportion to the main outcome with a significancelevelof0.05%.TheStatisticalPackagefor theSocial Sciences (SPSS) version 20 (IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Armonk,NY:IBMCorpChicago,IL,USA)wasusedfordescriptive statisticsandstatisticaltests.

2.4.Ethicalconsideration

ThestudywasapprovedbytheregionalEthicalCommitteeof ClinicalInvestigationinUppsala.Participationwasvoluntaryand performedunderinformedconsent.

3.Results

The study population was on average 61.9 years old and consistedofslightlymorewomenthanmen.Secondaryschoolwas themostcommoncompletededucationlevel.Thedistributionof demographicvariables,overallandaccordingtogroupallocation, isshowninTable2.

Table1

Threedifferentwaysofcommunicatingtreatmentinformation.

Sharedsetting Imaginethatinthenextfiveyearsyouwillhaveanincreasedriskofhavingaheartattack.Yourphysicianoffersyouadrugwithrareand mildsideeffects,tobetakeninpillformoncedaily.Theusefulnessofthedrughasbeenevaluatedinscientificstudies,andtheeffectcanbe describedasfollows:

Group Effectmeasure Magnitude Textinsurvey

A. Delayofevents(DoE) 18months Ifyouhaveaheartattackinthenextfiveyearsitwillbedelayedbyupto1.5yearsifyoutakethetreatment. B. Relativerisk

reduction(RRR)

27% Ifyoutakethetreatmentforfiveyears,youwillreducetheriskofaheartattackby27%.

C. Absoluterisk reduction(ARR)

2% Withouttreatment,yourriskofaheartattackinthenextfiveyearsis8%,andifyoutakethetreatment,theriskofaheart attackinthenextfiveyearswillbe6%.

(4)

3.1.Cardiovasculardiseasesandmedicaltreatments

Inthestudypopulation,6.5%reportedahistoryofestablished cardiovascular disease. The reported medical history of heart attackwas 3.9%and forangina 4.0% (seeTable 2).Significantly more men (10.0%) than women (3.4%) reported a history of cardiovasculardisease(p<0.001).

About 60% of the group used one or more prescribed medications.Therewerenosignificantsexdifferencesregarding useofprescriptionmedication.

3.2.Willingnesstoinitiatetreatment

Atotalof80.5%werewillingtotakethemedicationwhenthe effectwasdescribedasDoE,83.0%whenitwasdescribedasRRR and62.8%whenit wasdescribedasARR. Pairwisecomparisons revealedthatthereweresignificantdifferencesbetweenDoEand ARR(p<0.001),and betweenRRR andARR(p<0.001), butnot betweenDoEandRRR(P=0.396)(seeTable3).Thewillingnessto initiate treatment was further investigated in multiple logistic regressionsusingthedifferenteffectdescriptionsasindependent variables(seeTable4).Therewasasignificantlyhigherwillingness toinitiatetreatmentinthefullyadjustedmodelswhentheeffect wasdescribedasDoE(OR2.64,95%CI1.81–3.85)andRRR(OR3.12, 95%CI2.11–4.61)ascomparedtoARR.

3.3.ViewsontreatmentandWTP

There were significant differences regarding respondents’ viewsonthebenefitfromtreatment,motivationtotaketreatment, theimportanceofadheringtotreatmentandWTP.

In pairwise comparisons, DoE scored higher than ARR in assessmentsofbenefitfromtreatment(p<0.001),motivationto taketreatment(p<0.001)andimportancetoadhere(p=0.005)

(seeTable3).RRRscoredhigherthanARRinassessmentsofbenefit

from treatment (p<0.001) and motivation to take treatment (p<0.001).TherewerenosignificantdifferencesbetweenDoEand RRRinpairwisecomparisons.

ThemedianWTPforthethreedifferenttreatmentdescriptions was s20 for DoE, s15 for RRR and s10 for ARR. In pairwise comparisons,WTPforDoEwassignificantlyhigherthanthatof ARR(p=0.001),WTPfor RRRwasalsosignificantlyhigherthan that of ARR (p=0.008). There were no significant differences betweenDoEandRRRintermsofWTP(p=0.353).

4.Discussion

Theobjectiveof thisstudywas tocomparetheproportional effectmeasures,RRRandARR,withthetime-basedmeasure,DoE, toassesstheirassociationwithpeople’swillingnesstoinitiatea preventive medicationtreatment. A further aim was to assess whether the different treatment effect descriptions affected favorableviewsandWTPinrelationtotheproposedmedication. Ahigherproportionofindividualswerewillingtoinitiatethe treatmentwhentheeffectwaspresentedasDoEorRRRthanas ARR.Previousstudieshaveshownthatpatientsprefermedication whenthebenefitispresentedinrelativeratherthaninabsolute terms[34].TheresultsinthisstudyimplythatDoEmightserveas an adequate alternative or complement to other established measures.TheresultsalsoimplythatDoEandRRRaresuperiorto ARR when it comes to giving patients positive views of the treatment effects as well as motivating them to take it.

Table2

Distributionofcharacteristicsamongparticipants.

Category Subcategory DoE RRR ARR Overall

Gender Women,%(n) 55.7(186) 49.2(174) 55.1(211) 53.3(571) Men,%(n) 44.3(148) 50.8(180) 44.9(172) 46.7(500) Age Mean(s.d.) 61.8(8.6) 62.2(8.7) 61.6(8.7) 61.9(8.5) Educationlevel Compulsoryschool,%(n) 27.5(92) 28.7(100) 27.1(103) 27.8(295)

Secondaryschool,%(n) 35.8(120) 38.8(135) 37.6(143) 37.4(398) University,%(n) 36.7(123) 32.5(113) 35.3(134) 34.8(370) Historyofcardiovasculardiseases Noheartattack,%(n) 97.3(321) 95.4(330) 95.8(363) 96.1(1014)

Heartattack,%(n) 2.7(9) 4.6(16) 4.3(16) 3.9(41) Noangina,%(n) 96.4(318) 94.3(328) 97.4(369) 96.0(1015) Angina,%(n) 3.6(12) 5.8(20) 2.6(10) 4.0(42) Quantityofprescriptionmedications Nouse,%(n) 39.0(124) 38.0(125) 42.8(154) 40.0(403)

1,%(n) 20.4(65) 16.7(55) 16.1(58) 17.7(178) 4–2,%(n) 30.2(96) 31.0(102) 31.9(115) 31.1(313) >5,%(n) 10.4(33) 14.3(47) 9.2(33) 11.2(113)

Table3

Treatmentdecisionandviewsoftreatmentaccordingtodifferenttreatmentdescriptions.

Outcome DoE RRR ARR Overall Teststatistics p-value Willingnesstoinitiatetreatment,%(n) 80.5(268) 83.0(288) 62.8(238) 75.0(794) 47.24a <0.001

Useofinformation,md(q1,q3) 5(4,6) 5(4,6) 5(4,6) 5(4,6) 0.61b 0.736

Benefitfrommedication,md(q1,q3) 5(4,6) 5(3,6) 4(2,5) 4(3,6) 34.57b <0.001

Feelsafewithmedication,md(q1,q3) 4(3,6) 4(3,5) 4(2,6) 4(3,6) 2.29b

0.318 Bemotivatedtotakemedication,md(q1,q3) 5(3,6) 5(4,6) 4(2,6) 5(3,6) 27.56b <0.001

Importancetoadhere,md,(q1,q3) 6(5,7) 6(5,7) 6(4,7) 6(5,7) 8.53b 0.014 Willingnesstopayc (Euro),md(q1,q3),mean 20(10,30),25.7 15(10,30),22.9 10(5,25),18.9 10(8.3,30),22.5 13.17b 0.001 a PearsonChi-Square. b Kruskal-WallisHtest.

(5)

Respondents’willingnesstopayforthetreatmentwasalsohigher forDoEandRRRthanforARR,implyingthatagainindisease-free timeisvaluabletoindividuals.

Thereisacriticaldifferencebetweenthecomparedmeasures. Proportional measures, suchas RRR and ARRdepict treatment benefitsasincreasedchancesofavoidingdiseaseevents.Whilethis istruewithinalimitedstudytimeperiod,itislesslikelytobetrue fromanindividual’slifetimeperspective.Chronicdiseases,suchas cardiovasculardisease,developoverthelifecourseandpreventive interventions are from that perspective more likely to delay disease events rather than fully avoid them. RRR is a well-establishedeffectmeasure.Itissuitablesinceitgivesanestimate oftheriskreductionfromtheindividualpointofview.RRRmay, however,beproblematicinshareddecision-makingas,ifrightfully used, it pre-assumes that the decision-maker has a sound understandingof thebaseline risk, which is rarely likely to be thecase.Furthermore,itisdifficulttointuitivelysaywhenanRRR is low or high, and lay people seem to have difficulties in discriminating betweenlevels of effectiveness in RRR [12]. For thesereasons,health professionals maychoosetouseabsolute measuresoritsmathematicalreciprocal,NNT,whendescribinga treatment’seffect.Thesemeasuresare,however,alsoproblematic astheyaretime-andpopulation-specificanddisplaythegroup perspective,whichisnotoptimalin individualshared decision-making.

Relative and absolute risk reductions are point estimates, which means that they are summary statistics of group differences at a specific point in time: usually at the end of thefollow-up.DoEis notapointestimate,buta curveofhow theeffectisdelayed(orhastened) duringthefollow-upperiod

[22].TheDelayofEventmeasuredemonstratestreatmenteffect asagainindisease-freetime,whichcapturesthemagnitudeof treatmenteffectfromtheindividualperspective.SincetheDoE

is conditional having an event, if untreated, a patient has to assumetohave theeventin thetimeperiodcorrespondingto the clinical study follow-upto understand the benefit as DoE. This means that patients, in contrast to the RRR, intrinsically facethequestionabouttheirownabsolutebaselinerisk. Time-based effects have been shown to be more easily understood than other figures [15].Statements about timeperiods can be understood differently by people and patients in treatment situations.Timecanbeseenasascarceresource[35],andwhen time is related to health it may be an even more scarce resource. Whentime is a part of consumer models it is often viewed as lesstransferable than other “types” of wealth[35]; uncertaintyin relationtotimemayalsobemoreaversive[35]. This makesplanning in termsof timeespeciallyimportantfor individuals; conversely uncertainty can disrupt planning, and makesplanningmoredifficult.TheDoEcurvemayalsoapplyto the patient lifetime perspective as it likely increases with treatment time, in contrast tothe RRR. If the curve showsan increased effect throughout the treatment period, this may encourage persons to continue treatment beyond the time frames from controlled clinical trials. Consequently, an effect measurethatdescribes a timeperiod,such asDoE, maybe of great importance in helping people with their planning, and medical decision-making. DoE as a communication strategy may be of certain value in increasing treatment uptake in patients with poor health literacy when simple and direct diction is desirable[36].

TheDelayofEventmeasurecallsforashiftinthinkingofevents ofchronicdiseaseasdelayableratherthanfullyavoidable.Thisis arguablyajustifiedviewgiventheunderlyingpathologyandthat theriskofeventsincreaseswithageformostchronicdisease.This shiftinthinking mayaffectpatient'sviewsonpreventive treat-ments'abilityaswellastheirviewofthediseaseitself.Itispossible

Table4

Resultsoflogisticregressionmodelsoffactorspredictingpatients’willingnesstoinitiatetreatment.

CrudeOR95%CI Model1OR95%CI Model2OR95%CI Treatmentinformation Effectdescription:

ARR 1 1 1 DoE 2.44**(1.74–3.44) 2.67**(1.86–3.83) 2.64**(1.81–3.85) RRR 2.89**(2.04–4.10) 3.18**(2.20–4.60) 3.12**(2.11–4.61) Demographic Gender: Female 1 1 1 Male 0.95(0.72–1.25) 0.88(0.65–1.19) 0.95(0.69–1.31) Age 1.07** (1.05–1.09) 1.07** (1.05–1.09) 1.05** (1.03–1.07) Educationlevel: University 1 1 1

Secondaryschoolorequal 3.27**(2.17–4.91)

2.54**(1.65–3.92)

2.25**(1.44–3.52)

Compulsoryschool 1.26(0.92–1.72) 1.40(0.99–1.95) 1.21(0.85–1.72) Health-relatedfactors Historyofheartattack:

Noheartattack 1 1

Oneormoreheartattack 3.13*(1.10–8.86)

1.11(0.35–3.54) Historyofangina:

Noangina 1 1

Oneormoreanginaattack 7.03**(1.69–29.27) 5.53(0.71–43.33)

Prescribedmedications: Nouse 1 1 One 15*(1.4–2.26) 1.44(0.94–2.21) Twotofour 3.02**(2.09–4.37) 2.32**(1.56–3.44) Fiveormore 5.71**(2.89–11.28) 3.12**(1.49–6.54) Nagelkerker2 0.18 0.21

Oddsratio(OR),significancelevelandconfidenceinterval(CI)forthebinarylogisticregressions. Model1=Treatmentinformation+gender+age+educationlevel+occupation.

Model2=Model1+historyofheartattack+historyofangina+prescribedmedications.

*

p0.05.

**

(6)

thataviewofdiseaseasmerelydelayablewithtreatmentaffects patient'shealthbeliefs,healthbehaviorsandsubjectivehealthin bothpositiveandnegativeways.Itissuggestedthattheeffectsof viewingdiseaseasdelayableratherthanavoidableisatopicfor futureresearch.

One findinginthis studywas thathavinga historyof heart attackoranginawasassociatedwithhigherpatientwillingnessto initiatetreatmentincrudeanalysesbutnotintheadjustedmodels. These results may have occurred due to a small number of respondentswithahistoryofheartattackorangina.

4.1.Strengthsandlimitations

Thestrengthsofthisstudyincludethecommunitysetting,the randomized survey experimental approach and the manner in whichthetreatmenteffects werepresentedtotherespondents. Thisstudyalsohassomelimitationsworthnoting.Theresponse rate is reasonable in relation to what is anticipated from questionnaire-basedstudies,buttherewasnoinformationabout nonresponders. The hypothetical setting used in the question-naires is, further, different from clinical situations where informationmaybeindividualizedand clarified.Althoughmany respondentsreportedusingmedication,thestudypopulationwas asamplefromthegeneralpopulation,whichisnotthesameasa targetedpatientpopulation.Thedatausedtocalculatetheeffect came from a study of secondary preventive treatment, and therefore a study population at higher risk of CVD than the majorityof therespondentsin thisstudy. Preventivetreatment effectsexpressedasthenovelapproachDelayofEventsorrelative riskreductionwereseparatelyassociatedwithahighwillingness toinitiatetreatment.

4.2.Conclusion

TheresultsinthisstudyimplythatDoEandRRRarecomparable ina populationsetting,and that theyarepreferabletoARR, in motivatingindividuals toinitiate a treatment.Preventive treat-menteffectsexpressedasthenovelapproachDelayofEventsor relative risk reduction were separately associated with a high willingnesstoinitiatetreatment.Presentingpreventivetreatment effectas Delay of Eventsor relative risk reduction was further associatedwithpositiveviewsontreatmentbenefit,motivation, importancetoadhereandwillingnesstopayfortreatment.There isaneedtofurtherinvestigateifthenovelDoEmayhavearolein clinical decision-making for specific patient groups and treat-ments,especiallyifitmayimprovelong-termadherence. 4.3.Practiceimplications

Expressing treatmenteffect usingthe novel Delayof Events measure holds value for clinical communication and medical decisionmaking,andthisstudyimpliesthatpresentingtreatment effect as Delay of Events might increase the likelihood that a patientwillacceptandadheretoaproposedpreventivetreatment. Conflictofinterest

All theauthors declare that there isno potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgements

The studywas financiallysupportedbygrantsfromUppsala University and The Swedish Society of Medicine. The authors acknowledgethecontributionofCharlottaArnessonBerglundat

Ekebyhealthcentreinhelpingwiththepre-testofscenarioand evaluationquestions.

References

[1]J.E. Hux, C.D.Naylor, Communicating the benefits of chronic preventive therapy:doestheformatofefficacydatadeterminepatients'acceptanceof treatment?Med.Decis.Making15(2)(1995)152–157.

[2]A.Edwards,G.Elwyn,N.Stott,Communicatingriskreductions.Researchers shouldpresentresultswithbothrelativeandabsoluterisks,BMJ318(7183) (1999)603–604(authorreply).

[3]C. De LasCuevas, W.Penate, L. deRivera, Towhat extent is treatment adherenceofpsychiatricpatientsinfluencedbytheirparticipationinshared decisionmaking?PatientPreferenceAdherence8(2014)1547–1553. [4]WHO,AdherencetoLong-termTherapies:EvidenceforAction,WorldHealth

Organization,Geneva,2003.

[5]D.F. Blackburn, R.T. Dobson, J.L. Blackburn, T.W. Wilson, Cardiovascular morbidityassociatedwithnonadherencetostatintherapy,Pharmacotherapy 25(8)(2005)1035–1043.

[6]C.W.Cheng,K.S.Woo,J.C.Chan,B.Tomlinson,J.H.You,Associationbetween adherencetostatintherapyandlipidcontrolinHongKongChinesepatientsat highriskofcoronaryheartdisease,Br.J.Clin.Pharmacol.58(5)(2004)528– 535.

[7]L. Wei,J. Wang, P.Thompson,S.Wong, A.D.Struthers,T.M.MacDonald, Adherencetostatintreatmentandreadmissionofpatientsaftermyocardial infarction:asixyearfollowupstudy,Heart88(3)(2002)229–233. [8]M.A.Munger, B.W.VanTassell,J. LaFleur, Medicationnonadherence:an

unrecognizedcardiovascularriskfactor,MedGenMed:MedscapeGen.Med.9 (3)(2007)58.

[9]J.Nexoe,D.Gyrd-Hansen,J.Kragstrup,I.S.Kristiansen,J.B.Nielsen,DanishGPs' perceptionofdiseaseriskandbenefitofprevention,Fam.Pract.19(1)(2002) 3–6.

[10]J.Covey,Ameta-analysisoftheeffectsofpresentingtreatmentbenefitsin differentformats,Med.Decis.Making27(5)(2007)638–654.

[11]G.Gigerenzer,W.Gaissmaier,E.Kurz-Milcke,L.Schwartz,M.S.Woloshin, Helpingdoctorsandpatientsmakesenseofhealthstatistics,Psychol.Sci. PublicInterest8(2)(2007)53–96.

[12]L.Sorensen,D.Gyrd-Hansen,I.S.Kristiansen,J.Nexoe,J.B.Nielsen,Laypersons' understandingofrelativeriskreductions:randomisedcross-sectionalstudy, BMCMed.Inform.Decis.Mak.8(2008)31.

[13]D.Mason,S.Boase,T.Marteau,A.L.Kinmonth,T.Dahm,N.Minorikawa,S. Sutton,One-weekrecallofhealthriskinformationandindividualdifferences inattentiontobarcharts,Health,RiskSoc.16(2)(2014)136–153. [14]D.Misselbrook,D.Armstrong,Patients'responsestoriskinformationabout

thebenefitsoftreatinghypertension,Br.J.Gen.Pract.51(465)(2001)276–279. [15]P.M. Christensen, K. Brosen, K. Brixen, M. Andersen, I.S. Kristiansen, A

randomizedtrialoflaypersons'perceptionofthebenefitofosteoporosis therapy:numberneededtotreatversuspostponementofhipfracture,Clin. Ther.25(10)(2003)2575–2585.

[16]M.A.Hernán,Thehazardsofhazardratios,Epidemiology(Cambridge,Mass.) 21(1)(2010)13–15.

[17]H.Uno,B.Claggett,L.Tian,E.Inoue,P.Gallo,T.Miyata,D.Schrag,M.Takeuchi,Y. Uyama,L.Zhao,H.Skali,S.Solomon,S.Jacobus,M.Hughes,M.Packer,L.J.Wei, Movingbeyondthehazardratioinquantifyingthebetween-groupdifference insurvivalanalysis,J.Clin.Oncol:Off.J.Am.Soc.Clin.Oncol.32(22)(2014) 2380–2385.

[18]H.Uno,J.Wittes,H.Fu,S.D.Solomon,B.Claggett,L.Tian,T.Cai,M.A.Pfeffer,S.R. Evans,L.-J.Wei,Alternativestohazardratiosforcomparingefficacyorsafetyof therapiesinnoninferioritystudies,Ann.Intern.Med.163(2)(2015)127–134. [19]B.J.McNeil,S.G.Pauker,H.C.Sox,A.TverskyJr.,Ontheelicitationofpreferences

foralternativetherapies,N.Engl.J.Med.306(21)(1982)1259–1262. [20]P.A.Halvorsen,T.F.Wisloff,H.Stovring,O.Aasland,I.S.Kristiansen,Therapeutic

decisionsbynumberneededtotreatandsurvivalgains:across-sectional surveyoflipid-loweringdrugrecommendations,Br.J.Gen.Pract.61(589) (2011)e477–83.

[21]H. Stovring, D. Gyrd-Hansen, I.S. Kristiansen, J. Nexoe, J.B. Nielsen, Communicatingeffectivenessofinterventionforchronicdiseases:what singleformatcanreplacecomprehensiveinformation?BMCMed.Inform. Decis.Mak.8(2008)25.

[22]P.Lytsy,L.Berglund,J.Sundstrom,Aproposalforanadditionalclinicaltrial outcomemeasureassessingpreventiveeffectasdelayofevents,Eur.J. Epidemiol.27(12)(2012)903–909.

[23]N.Orsini,A.Wolk,M.Bottai,Evaluatingpercentilesofsurvival,Epidemiology 23(5)(2012)770–771.

[24]H.R. Bowen, Theinterpretation of votingin the allocation of economic resources,Q.J.Econ.58(1)(1943)27–48.

[25]I.M.Kopits,C.Chen,J.S.Roberts,W.Uhlmann,R.C.Green,Willingnesstopayfor genetictestingforAlzheimer'sdisease:ameasureofpersonalutility,Genet. Test.Mol.Biomarkers15(12)(2011)871–875.

[26]P.Lytsy,R.Westerling,Patientexpectationsonlipid-loweringdrugs,Patient Educ.Couns.67(1-2)(2007)143–150.

[27]E.Berglund,P.Lytsy,R.Westerling,Adherencetoandbeliefsinlipid-lowering medicaltreatments:astructuralequationmodelingapproachincludingthe necessity-concernframework,PatientEduc.Couns.(2012).

(7)

[28]P.M.Sniderman,D.B.Grob,Innovationsinexperimentaldesigninattitude surveys,Annu.Rev.Sociol.22(1996)377–399.

[29]B.J.Gaines,J.H. Kuklinski,P.J.Quirk, Thelogicofthe surveyexperiment reexamined,PoliticalAnal.15(1)(2007)1–20.

[30]S.S.S.S.Group,Randomisedtrialofcholesterolloweringin4444patientswith coronaryheartdisease:theScandinavianSimvastatinSurvivalStudy(4S), Lancet344(8934)(1994)1383–1389.

[31] F.Goodyear-Smith,B.Arroll,L.Chan,R.Jackson,S.Wells,T.Kenealy,Patients preferpicturestonumberstoexpresscardiovascularbenefitfromtreatment, Ann.Fam.Med.6(3)(2008)213–217.

[32]T.Klose,Thecontingentvaluationmethodinhealthcare,HealthPolicy47(2) (1999)97–123.

[33]K.Strassburger,F.Bretz,Compatiblesimultaneouslowerconfidencebounds fortheHolmprocedureandotherBonferroni-basedclosedtests,Stat.Med.27 (24)(2008)4914–4927.

[34]D.J.Malenka,J.A.Baron,S.Johansen,J.W.Wahrenberger,J.M.Ross,Theframing effectofrelativeandabsoluterisk,J.Gen.Intern.Med.8(10)(1993)543–548. [35]F.Leclerc,B.H.Schmitt,L.Dube,Waitingiimeanddecisionmakingistimelike

money?J.Consum.Res.22(1)(1995).

[36]A.Carlisle,K.L.Jacobson,L.DiFrancesco,R.M.Parker,Practicalstrategiesto improvecommunicationwithpatients,Pharm.Ther.36(9)(2011)576–589.

References

Related documents

measures: Analysis I Cross- sectional study Men and women be- tween 16 and 84 years of age (n = 100,433, response rate 52.9%) - Primary ad- herence and refraining

Second, analyses from the Systolic Hypertension in Europe (Syst-Eur) trial generally found no J-curve for diastolic blood pressure and cardiovascular mortality, with the

Lay people’s willingness to initiate preventive therapy was sensitive to the length of time periods when treat- ment effect was presented as the time-based measure Delay of Event?.

Faktorerna rangordnades som följer; möjlighet att boka via Internet 5,02, antal avgångstider per dag 4,82, servering på flygplanet inkluderat i priset 4,75, möjlighet att boka

In Paper IV our impression is that the demarcation line often seen after crosslinking treatment (Seiler &amp; Hafezi 2006) is a part of what we measured as an increase in

Aims: The aims of this thesis were to determine whether mechanical compression of the cornea during corneal crosslinking for keratoconus using a sutured rigid

The purpose of this study was to describe the early chain of care of ALLI patients with particular emphasis on early detection and the use of Emergency Medical Services (EMS) as

suppression and decrease HIV transmission. Hendershot el al [15] conclude that alcohol use in general seem to be detrimental for treatment adherence, with a greater effect seen