• No results found

The fundamental things apply... as time goes by : Students' long-term memories from an ecology field excursion

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "The fundamental things apply... as time goes by : Students' long-term memories from an ecology field excursion"

Copied!
7
0
0

Loading.... (view fulltext now)

Full text

(1)

THE FUNDAMENTAL THINGS APPLY… AS TIME GOES

BY: STUDENTS’ LONG-TERM MEMORIES FROM AN

ECOLOGY FIELD

Lars Björklund and Karin Stolpe1

1

Linköping University, Sweden

Abstract: The aim of this study was to investigate and analyze what biology students

remembered a long time after being out on an ecology excursion. The students’ memories were tested during a stimulated recall interview and analyzed using the dual memory system model of learning. Already after 6 months we found that the students had forgotten a lot of the scientific content. Very often they showed a familiarity (recognition) with the situations and objects showed to them but they were unable to identify (recall) and label them. However they did remember some spectacular moment, the sudden appearance of a fox and a moose. They did also remember things and situations when they were active themselves, digging, smelling, using their hands or their feet’s in the difficult balancing and walking on a pet bog. From literature we identified two different types of memories, depending of the question asked: recall and recognition. We connected memories used in recall to the explicit memory system (declarative knowledge), and memories used in recognition to the implicit memory system (tacit knowledge). The Explicit memory has a short retention but the implicit system is very stable and this will explain the difference in recall and recognition abilities. Since the implicit memory incorporates emotional, somatic, markers we were able to explain the specific flashbulb memories. The implicit system is active when we are doing things, using our senses and this may explain why those

memories still were strong even after a full year. The strong memories of patterns stored in the implicit system seemed to act as indices to the declarable labels and facts in the explicit system. Implications for research, education and type of assessment are discussed.

Keywords: teachers, memory, retention, dual-system, science education, biology

INTRODUCTION

Science education research very often focuses on teaching and examination is in most cases done close to the learning occasion and is rarely tested after a long time. There are not many studies on what students remember on long-term bases. Traditional memory models, like Tulving’s (1972) “Procedural, Semantic and Episodic memory model”, will not explain these result. Therefore researchers add models of motivation, emotions and affect to explain their results.

THEORETICAL FRAMWORK

Dual system-processing is a psychological model of two different cognitive systems for reasoning, making judgments and social cognition (Evans, 2008). There is a distinct

(2)

difference between rapid, automatic and non-conscious processes (implicit memory system) and a slow and deliberate explicit memory system. Additionally,

neuro-physiological research has identified two biologically separated memory systems in the human brain, namely the non-declarative and the declarative memory systems (Squire, 2004). Merging psychology and neurophysiological research allows for the idea of two separate systems for memory and learning to be strengthened, namely the dual memory system model (Björklund, 2007, 2008). Björklund has proposed the use of “implicit” and “explicit” memory systems as synonyms for Squire’s non-declarative and declarative systems. This proposal is also supported by neurological research on social

cognition(Satpute & Lieberman, 2006). The explicit memory system is characterized by dealing with what is traditionally referred to as facts, events, rules and labels in pedagogy and science education(Evans, 2008). The explicit knowledge is possible to verbalize and communicate. Associated to the explicit memory system is working memory, which is our conscious system(Sweller, van Merrienboer, & Paas, 1998). However, the explicit memory system is constrained by the limited capacity of working memory (Lieberman, Gaunt, Gilbert, & Trope, 2002; Marois & Ivanoff, 2005). Working memory may hold about five units at a time and the addition of any further units can lead to cognitive load(Ross, 1969; Sweller & Chandler, 1991).

The implicit memory system deals with non-conscious knowledge (Berry & Dienes, 1993). Implicit memories are stored as multimodal sensory patterns of phenomena that we

perceive, even non-consciously, in a specific situation – what we hear, feel, see and smell. Logan(1988) suggested that, “subjects store and retrieve representations of each individual encounter with a stimulus”. It is therefore feasible to suggest that each representation is stored in the implicit memory system as a unique holistic pattern (Björklund, 2008). The implicit memory system will constantly perform pattern matching processes. When we re-experience a situation, the match will help us feel and act in the same way as we did the last time (Lieberman, 2000). Hence, we will experience a feeling of familiarity with the situation. Pattern matching is an automatic and rapid process which can impact behavior directly without being constrained by the processing limitations of working memory. Since the use of implicit memories should be considered as knowledge that is “hidden” from the practitioner it could be characterized as tacit knowledge (Polanyi, 1966). Even though the process of pattern matching is non-conscious, the implicit memory system may trigger a corresponding declarable label in the explicit memory system (Figure 1).

The explicit and implicit memory systems have different retention properties. Explicit memories degrade fast (Fleischman, Wilson, Gabrieli, Bienias, & Bennett, 2004). This may serve as one possible explanation to why recall items, which request a declarable answer, have a shorter retention (Tunney, 2003). Implicit memories have shown to be long-lasting, and even lifelong (Dennis, Howard Jr, & Howard, 2006; Jenkins & Hoyer, 2000). This would imply that the feeling of familiarity, or recognition, is more

long-lasting. Our starting point for this study is that recall tasks require explicit memories, while recognition tasks call for implicit memories..

(3)

RATIONALE

We have formulated the following research questions to frame the study:

 Investigate university students’ long-term retention from an ecology field excursion.

 How do different ways of formulating the questions influence the type of memories evoked?

 How may those results be explained using the dual memory system model?

METHOD

The original data collection took place during an ecological field excursion in southern Sweden. A biology teacher at university level brought his class out into the woods as a mandatory part of an ecology course. The excursion with a focus on the teacher was analyzed in a recently reported study by the present authors (Stolpe & Björklund, 2012a). The present study is focusing on the students and their memories 6 and 12 months later. The excursion was video recorded and from the video, we identified 16 important

episodes. Each episode was then represented by a picture or a short film clip to be used as cues in a stimulated recall interview (Lyle, 2003). 6 months later four female students and one male student volunteered to participate in the interview. The interview lasted between 45 and 60 minutes and was video recorded and fully transcribed. In every first encounter with a student we asked them “Could you please tell me what happened during this day?” They gave us a more or less comprehensive account of what took place. In the next step we gave them cues consisting of pictures or video clips and asked: “Could you please tell me something about this picture?”. We define this as a recall question. Later on, the questions were more focused (“Do you remember if anything special happened here?” or “Do you remember what the teacher talked about?”). At this stage the questions were still of recall character. Finally, if the student did not remember the situation or species at hand but showed a familiarity with the situation, a recognition question was posed. For example, “If I tell you that this is Ptilidium, is that familiar to you?” Twelve months following the excursion, all five students participated in a follow up interview. Three pictures related to the two categories of memories were selected as stimuli for this second interview, which was done by telephone. The interviews lasted for 10 to 15 minutes and were audio recorded and fully transcribed.

RESULTS

The six months interviews were analyzed by looking for examples of when and where the students did recall the names of species or when they were able to tell what was happening. We also searched for examples where the students recognized species or were familiar with a situation. Factors that may have influenced learning was noted, were the students active doing things, what was their mood, hunger, affect etc. Degradation of memories of a specific narrative told by the teacher during the day was also analyzed and have been

(4)

published elsewhere (Stolpe & Björklund, 2012b). Everybody remembered two sensational moments, an Elk and a Fox that surprised them all.

J:I remember the big Elk at the lake and also the fox who came so close when we had a brake on the meadow.

E:It was really exciting to meet wild animals.

This could be described as “Flashbulb memories”, strong but not so relevant to the scientific content of the excursion.

The open question only gave incomplete and fragmented memories from the day but the cued, stimulated recall were effective.

We found rather few recall of names and identification of species, and when so; they were connected to strong sensory or affective experiences. Some student identified the old man’s beard, Usnea filipendula:

L:It’s bearded, long and with a beard. It was elastic so when you pulled, it snapped and sounded like this !

T: It feels a bit spoungy, something between a horses wet tail and a mushroom

Detailed memories also surfaced from their own practical tasks, when they were digging, drilling or collecting samples. Even though we found few examples of recall there were a lot of strong recognitions, feelings of familiarity. The recognized the plant, and even the name, when told.

T: I’m not totally sure.

I: [. . .] There is a hillock, or large rock covered with vegetation. T: Mm, well on the hillock there were some orchids growing. [. . .] I: Do you remember its name?

T: No, actually I don’t.

I: Do you recognize. . . Creeping lady’s tresses [Goodyera repens]. T: Creeping lady’s tresses, yes I recognize it.

CONCLUSIONS

Recall questions and identification of species were hard to answer already after six months; this short-time retention could be understood if these kind of memories are stored in the explicit memory system. (Figure 1)

Recognition questions use memories in the implicit memory system having a much longer retention. (Figure 2) Although the loss of names and declarable knowledge was evident the recognition of Signal Species is valuable even though the name of the plant is forgotten. Attentional and procedural skills that builds on implicit memories need to be learned in situ which shows the importance of field work and excursions in biology.

(5)
(6)

REFERENCES

Berry, D. C., & Dienes, Z. (1993). Implicit learning : theoretical and empirical issues. Hove: L. Erlbaum.

Björklund, L. (2007). The intuitive practitioner: Cognitive aspects on the development of

Expertise. Paper presented at the 13 International Conference on Thinking,

Norrköping.

Björklund, L. (2008). Från novis till expert: förtrogenhetskunskap i kognitiv och didaktisk

belysning (From novice to expert: practical visdom, implications for education from a cognitive perspective). Doctoral, Linköping university, Norrköping.

(7)

Dennis, N. A., Howard Jr, J. H., & Howard, D. V. (2006). Implicit sequence learning without motor sequencing in young and old adults. Experimental Brain Research,

175(1), 153-164.

Evans, J. S. B. T. (2008). Dual-Processing Accounts of Reasoning, Judgment, and Social Cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 255-278.

Fleischman, D. A., Wilson, R. S., Gabrieli, J. D. E., Bienias, J. L., & Bennett, D. A. (2004). A longitudinal study of implicit and explicit memory in old persons.

Psychology and Aging, 19(4), 617-625.

Jenkins, L., & Hoyer, W. J. (2000). Instance-based automaticity and aging: acquistion, reacquistion, and long term retention. Psychology and Aging, 15(3), 551-565. Lieberman, M. D. (2000). Intuition: a social cognitive neuroscience approach.

Psychological Bulletin, 126(1), 109-137.

Lieberman, M. D., Gaunt, R., Gilbert, D. T., & Trope, Y. (2002). Reflection and reflexion: A social cognitive neuroscience approach to attributional inference. Advances in

Experimental Social Psychology, 34, 199-249.

Logan, G. D. (1988). Toward an instance theory of automatization. Psychological Review,

95, 492-527.

Lyle, J. (2003). Stimulated recall: a report on its use in naturalistic research. British

Educational Research Journal, 29(6), 861-878.

Marois, R., & Ivanoff, J. (2005). Capacity limits of information processing in the brain.

TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences, 9(6), 296-305.

Polanyi, M. (1966). The Tacit Dimension. Gloucester, Mass.: Doubleday & Company, Inc. Ross, B. M. (1969). Sequential visual memory and the limited magic of the number seven.

Journal of Experimental Psychology, 80(2), 339-347.

Satpute, A. B., & Lieberman, M. D. (2006). Integrating automatic and controlled processes into neurocognitive models of social cognition. Brain Research, 1079, 86-97. Squire, L. R. (2004). Memory systems of the brain: A brief history and current perspective.

Neurobiology of Learning and Memory(82), 171-177.

Stolpe, K., & Björklund, L. (2012a). Seeing the wood for the trees. Applying the dual memory system model to investigate expert teachers' observational skills in natural ecological learning environments. International Journal of Science Education,

34(1), 101-125.

Stolpe, K., & Björklund, L. (2012b). Students' long-term memories from an ecology field excursion. Retelling a narrative as an interplay between implicit and explicit memories. Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research.

Sweller, J., & Chandler, P. (1991). Evidence for cognitive load theory. Cognition and

Instruction, 8(4), 351-362.

Sweller, J., van Merrienboer, J. J. G., & Paas, F. (1998). Cognitive Architecture and Instructional Design. Educational Psychology Review, 10(3).

Tulving, E. (1972). Episodic and semantic memory. In E. Tulving, W. Donaldson & G. H. Bower (Eds.), Organization of memory (pp. xiii, 423 p.). New York,: Academic Press.

Tunney, R. J. (2003). Implicit and Explicit Knowledge Decay at Different Rates: A Dissociation Between Priming and Recognition in Artificial Grammar Learning.

References

Related documents

With this motivation, we extend the analysis and show how to connect the TaskInsight classification to changes in data reuse, changes in cache misses and changes in performance

The questions I wanted to answer were what brain areas are activated by the retrieval of symbols associated with either a sound or a face and if any residual

The pantograph out voltage and current signals during the high power mode at 36 km/h vehicle speed are shown in the figure 6.9 and figure 6.10 respectively at 40 m away from

Preserved or resored Demolished 1970 - 2019 Beirut Central

Since we Lottas believe that the memory of these brave mothers ‒ mothers who for the freedom of the Fatherland many times sacrificed their sons, their lives’

The lighter lines, showing options predicted as fast selling, are more separated from the darker lines of the ordinary options for 35 days of data than for a smaller time frame, but

Resultaten visar att både silhuetter, objekt och abstrakta mönster kan kopplas till lagidrott. Inriktningarna kan dock anses koppla till ämnet olika mycket beroende på respondentens

An old Italian proverb, often used by Italo Calvino, states that “A city without old buildings is like an old man without memories.” Although it is true that the