• No results found

Road user behavioural adaptation to infrastructure safety treatments : evidence, implications and mitigation

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "Road user behavioural adaptation to infrastructure safety treatments : evidence, implications and mitigation"

Copied!
5
0
0

Loading.... (view fulltext now)

Full text

(1)

ROAD USER BEHAVIOURAL ADAPTATION TO INFRASTRUCTURE

SAFETY TREATMENTS – EVIDENCE, IMPLICATIONS AND

MITIGATION

Bhagwant Persaud

Professor, Department of Civil Engineering Ryerson University

350 Victoria Street, Toronto, Canada M5B2K3 Phone: +01 416-979-5345 E-mail: bpersaud@ryerson.ca Co-author: Craig Lyon, Persaud and Lyon, Inc.

1. INTRODUCTION

Over the years there has been an accumulation of evidence that suggests that road users in general, and drivers in particular, respond and adapt to safety treatments. This adaptive behavior may occur over time or over space. Although in same cases, red light cameras for example, the adaptation may result in a positive safety effect, in most cases the consequences can be negative due to increased risk taking behavior such as speeding, aggressiveness, or inattention.

The idea of behavioural adaptation to road safety measures is not new (See, e.g., OECD, 1990.). Wilde (1988) suggested that it is so prevalent that the level of risk in the transport system essentially remains constant despite safety interventions -- the so-called risk homeostasis theory. However, researchers such as Underwood et al. (1993), who cite several examples of adaptive behavior following safety interventions, have overwhelmingly disputed the risk homeostasis theory.

The purpose of this paper is not to contribute to the philosophical debate on the presence and extent of adaptive behavior, but to make the case, based on empirical crash-based evidence, that the influence of driver adaptation on treatment effectiveness needs to be considered in applying crash modification factors for these treatments in making cost-effective infrastructure investment decisions. The empirical evidence presented focuses to some extent on results from several studies involving the authors and is not intended to be exhaustive. Treatments for which evidence is presented and discussed are curve delineation, raised pavement markers, red light cameras and pavement friction improvement.

2. SOME EMPIRICAL EVIDENCE OF BEHAVIOURAL ADAPTATION

Presented below is a discussion of road user behavioural adaptation as it pertains to several infrastructure safety treatments. The discussion generally addresses the implications for crash modification factors, as well as possible mitigation measures in the case of negative consequences, and reinforcement opportunities in the case of positive adaptation.

2.1 Raised Pavement Markers

As a classic example of road user behavioural adaptation, Persaud et al. (2004) found that nighttime crashes on sharp horizontal curves on two-lane rural roads with low traffic volumes increased after permanent raised pavement markers (PRPMs) were installed. The study results are reproduced as Table 1.

(2)

Table 1: Crash Modification Factors for PRPMs (from Persaud et al., 2004) AADT CMF for Flatter curves CMF for Sharper curves

<5000 1.16 1.43

5001-15000 No change 1.26

15001-20000 0.76 1.03

It was argued by Persaud et al. that enhanced visibility could encourage drivers to increase speed, especially where traffic volumes were low, and that this increased risk taking can be especially dangerous on sharper curves. Conversely, they found that PRPMs were beneficial on flatter curves with higher AADTs where speed increases are more tolerable, and in any case are minimized due to the calming effects of higher traffic volumes.

The results suggest, naturally, that this treatment should be targeted where it is likely to be effective, and vice versa, a strategy that is supported by other results from the study that revealed significant reductions in crashes, especially those in wet weather night time, in New York state where the treatment was targeted at sites with a high frequency of wet-night crashes. On the other hand, overall effects were negligible in New Jersey where the treatment was applied non-selectively. If it is desired to implement the treatment on sharper curves with lower AADTs then it seems prudent to simultaneously implement treatments aimed at discouraging increased speeds (such as more prominent advisory signing) or mitigating the effects (such as high friction surfaces).

2.2 Curve Delineation

Results in Srinivasan et al. (2012) provide further evidence of behavioural adaptation to road delineation, specifically for curve treatments for two-lane rural roads that included improved retro-reflectivity of existing signs, post-mounted delineators, chevrons, raised pavement markers, and edge-lines. These results are reproduced in Table 2.

Table 2: Crash Modification Factors for Curve Delineation (from Srinivasan et al., 2012) Crash Type AADT Range CMF

*

(standard error)

Lane departure crashes < 3,800 1.206 (0.136) > 3800 0.731 (0.067)

Crashes during dark < 3,800 1.192 (0.136)

> 3800 0.678 (0.085)

Lane departure crashes during dark < 3,800 1.200 (0.138) > 3800 0.712 (0.093)

*Results that are statistically significant at the 5% level indicated in boldface

Similar to the findings of Persaud at al. (2004), it was found that target crashes increased where traffic volumes were low (<3800) and conducive to increased speeds, and that the treatment was beneficial at higher traffic volumes where increasing speed is a challenge. Note that the AADT threshold is much lower than that found in Persaud et al. (2004); however, the treatment combination is also quite

(3)

be effective, and vice versa, or that speed mitigation measures be simultaneously implemented where the treatment on its own can increase target crashes.

2.3 Red Light Cameras

In a large multi-jurisdiction study, Persaud et al. (2005) found, as shown in Table 3, a decrease in the right-angle crashes that are targeted by this measure and which are likely to have severe consequences.

Table 3: Crash Modification Factors for Red Light Cameras (from Persaud et al., 2005) Right-angle Rear-end

Total Injury Total Injury

Estimate of CMF (standard error) 0.754 (0.029) 0.843 (0.059) 1.149 (0.030) 1.240 (0.116)

It seems logical that this safety benefit is caused by a positive behavior change – less red-light running. However, the increase in rear-end crashes (which tend to be minor and to be regarded as collateral damage) is likely a result of an abundance of caution that manifests in increased stops at the onset of amber and an increase in the dilemma zone. As important, was the limited evidence suggestive of a general deterrence effect of this treatment; there was a modest decrease in right angle crashes while there was only a negligible increase in rear-end crashes. This supports the expectation that driver adaptation will not be confined to the treated intersections and suggests that these spillover benefits ought to be considered in planning and evaluating this treatment. To counteract the unintended consequences of increases in rear-end crashes, adjustments to the intergreen period could be considered, as well as improving the friction on intersection approaches.

2.4 Pavement Friction Improvement

Lyon et al. (2016), using a multi-state database, found that dry road crashes can increase following certain types of friction improvement treatments, including microsurfacing, open graded friction course, chip seal and thin hot mix overlay. Table 4 summarizes the pertinent results, which indicate fairly consistent increases in dry road crashes that counteract the generally positive effects for the targeted wet road crashes. It seems evident that there is speed adaptation during dry road conditions, leading to unintended consequences. These negative consequences could potentially be minimized with the application of mitigation measures such as increased enforcement and/or more prominent advisory and speed limit signing.

Table 4: Crash Modification Factors for Pavement Friction Improvements (Lyon et al., 2016)

Treatment Road Type CMF

Wet Road Dry Road

Thin hot mix overlay

Multilane 0.865

Freeway 0.797

Two-lane rural 1.256 1.181

Open graded friction course

Freeway 0.685

Multilane 1.108

Two-lane rural 1.120

Chip Seal Multilane 0.373 1.206

Two-lane rural 0.950 0.937

Microsurfacing Multilane 0.785

(4)

2.5 Other Treatments

There are several examples of driver behavioural adaptation in results from other studies. These include:

• A study by Zegeer et al. (2002) that found that pedestrian crash risk was greater at marked crosswalks compared to unmarked croswalks at vehicle traffic volumes greater than 10,000 vehicles per day. The result has been largely attributed to the false sense of security created by the marking of crosswalks.

• A now dated study by Persaud (1988) that found that while crashes decreased where intersections were converted from two-way to all-way stop control, they increased at nearby unconverted intrsections in neighbourhoods where there was a proliferation of conversions. It was argued that drivers approachng a stop sign at two-way stop controlled intersection became conditioned to expecting drivers on adjacent approaches to stop.

• In a crash-based study of pavement marking retroreflectivity, Bahar et al. (2006) hypothesized, based on their results, that any effect of the level of brightness of pavement markings may be minimized by driver adaptation to road conditions. They concluded that ”the best estimate of the joint effect of retroreflectivity and driver adaptation is approximately zero for non-intersection road segments during non-daylight hours.” (It should be noted that these results have been subject to considerable debate and discussion; however, they are supportive of the reality of driver behaviour adaptation and the need for considering it is planning safety improvements.)

• Results from Assum et al. (1999) showed that drivers compensate for road lighting in terms of increased speed and reduced concentration. This is an example of studies that are not crash- based so could not be used in quantitatively assessing the influence of driver adaptation in planning infrastructure safety measures.

REFERENCES

Assum T., Torkel Bjørnskau T, Fosser S. and F. Sagberg. Risk compensation—the case of road lighting. Accident Analysis and Prevention 31(5), 545-553, 1999.

Bahar G., Masliah M., Erwin T., Tan E., and E. Hauer. Pavement Marking Materials and Markers: Testing the Relationship Between Retroreflectivity and Safety. National Cooperative Highway Research Program Research Results Digest 305, 2006

Lyon C., Persaud B. and D. Merritt. (2016). Quantifying the safety effects of pavement friction improvements – results from a large-scale study. In press, International Journal of Pavement Engineering.

OECD-Road Transport Research. Behavioural Adaptations to Changes in the Road Transport System. Paris. 1990.

Persaud B., “`Migration' of accident risk after remedial blackspot treatment”. Traffic Engineering + Control, 28:1, 1987.

Persaud B., Bahar G., Mollett C. and C. Lyon. (2004). Safety evaluation of permanent raised snowplowable pavement markers. Transportation Research Record 1897, pp. 148-155.

(5)

Persaud B., Council F., Lyon C., Eccles K. and M. Griffith. (2005). Multi-jurisdictional safety evaluation of red light cameras. Transportation Research Record: Journal of the Transportation Research Board 1922, pp. 29-37.

Srinivasan R., Baek J., Carter D., Persaud B., Lyon C., Eccles K., Gross F., and N. Lefler. Safety Evaluation of Improved Curve Delineation. (2009). Federal Highway Administration Publication No. FHWA-HRT-09-045. Available at: https://www.fhwa.dot.gov/publications/research/safety/09045/

09045.pdf.

Underwood G., Jiang C. and C.I. Howarth. Modelling of safety measure effects and risk compensation. Accident Analysis and Prevention 25(3), 277-288, 1993.

Wilde, G. J. S. Risk homeostasis theory and traffic accidents: Propositions, deductions and discussion of dissension in recent reactions. Ergonomics 3 1(4):44 I-468: 1988.

Zegeer C., Stewart J., Huang H., and P. Lagerwey (2002) Safety Effects of Marked vs. Unmarked Crosswalks as Uncontrolled Locations. FHWA Report FHWA-RD-01-075).

Figure

Table 2: Crash Modification Factors for Curve Delineation (from Srinivasan et al., 2012)
Table 4: Crash Modification Factors for Pavement Friction Improvements (Lyon et al., 2016)

References

Related documents

The road safety analysis shows, for the short after period that was analyzed, a clear reduction in the number of fatalities and severe injuries which is in good agreement with

Det är gott nog att be- skriva och utforska ett fenomen, men i syfte att skapa en forskning som är till nytta för både vetenskapen och praktiken, som ändå den interaktiva

psykosociala problem. De har bland annat studerat samverkan mellan skola och socialtjänst. Deras slutsats är att olika myndigheter och organisationer ofta har svårt att samordna

The expected cost for non-seasonal demand, assuming that no safety stock is used, would be 25 659 SEK for both the systems, with an expected fill rate of 98% for the reorder

In order to get access to a consistency protocol, a proxy object is required. The proxy object is logically located in the coordination layer of the DSS and connected to an

Totalt sett visar mätningarna att användningen bland barn stagnerat mellan 1990 och 1991, medan den ökat bland vuxna som cyklar till arbetet och bland cyklister på allmänna

Redovisade friktionsdata har inte korrigerats för dubbutstick. Motiveringen för detta är att utsticken kan betraktas som en egenskap hos det dubbdäck som kon- sumenten

This essay aims to examine how the EU Courts can establish mechanisms that improve the right to an effective judicial review in the targeted sanctions cases when there is a